770
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Emotionalized Social Media Environments: How Alternative News Media and Populist Actors Drive Angry Reactions

, , & ORCID Icon

ABSTRACT

This study employs a comparative analytical framework to enhance our understanding of the conducive opportunity structures that foster emotionally charged political discourse. We examined 175,539 Facebook posts characterized by variations in content (in terms of themes and populist rhetoric), authorship (including populist politicians, traditional news, and alternative media), and geographic context (Belgium, France, Germany, Switzerland, UK, US). We then analyzed 360,000 emotional responses from Facebook users to determine which posts create the most conducive conditions for eliciting angry emotions. Our key findings show that posts from alternative and hyperpartisan media, as well as those from populist politicians and parties, tend to elicit elevated levels of angry reactions. These posts often use anti-elitist and exclusionary language. This finding has significant implications, as the anger generated by such accounts can propagate incivility and polarization and facilitate the spread of ideologically driven misinformation. A particular case is Donald Trump, who, as a populist governing figure, manages to elicit positive emotions, including “love,” despite delivering seemingly antagonistic messages. To strengthen the robustness of our findings, we conducted a replication analysis with 67,620 Facebook posts from three of the six countries and examined two different time periods. This analysis confirmed the persistence of our findings over time. Our opportunity-structure framework offers valuable insights for designing targeted strategies to improve the quality of public discourse and promote informed and constructive political engagement in diverse societies.

Introduction

Social networking sites, such as Facebook, have become important platforms for political discussions, as they allow users to show support for a political party and to make their opinions known. News media and political actors compete for the attention of social media users and try to reach them with emotional content (Eberl et al., Citation2020; Heiss et al., Citation2019). Research has established that emotions such as anger and anxiety can increase attention to political campaign communication (Gerstlé & Nai, Citation2019; Neuman et al., Citation2018). Populist politicians, in particular, seem to have mastered this strategy of emotionalization particularly successfully, thus increasing their reach and attracting new followers (Ernst, Esser, et al., Citation2019).

Emotional social media posts attract attention and provoke emotional user reactions, mainly if controversial issues are addressed (Eberl et al., Citation2020). For example, posts that include hostility toward out-groups receive significantly more “angry” reactions than others (Rathje et al., Citation2021). In the longer term, such emotional reactions can influence political attitudes, for example, when emotionalized blame attributions are shared by populist actors (Hameleers et al., Citation2016). Similarly, alternative and hyper-partisan news outlets publish emotionalized content to attract high user engagement, which can increase the virality and visibility of their content (Hiaeshutter-Rice & Weeks, Citation2021; Larsson, Citation2019). Emotional user reactions also serve as a sentiment barometer for the account holders (i.e., politicians and media outlets) who seek to build strong follower networks. Furthermore, they can influence other users, as they suggest a particular climate of opinion, and can lead to emotional spill-over effects (Bene et al., Citation2022; Heiss et al., Citation2019).

In this study, we combine an analysis of emotions with an opportunity structure approach that distinguishes contextual conditions on the level of countries, actor types, media types, and message types. We are interested in how various countries differ regarding emotional user reactions to political posts on Facebook and what kind of content provokes these reactions. How do types of political actors (i.e., populist vs. traditional politicians) and news media actors (i.e., hyper-partisan vs. traditional media) differ regarding those emotional user reactions? Finally, what kinds of posts drive such emotional reactions, particularly anger? Studying these conditions will provide a more differentiated understanding of the communicative strategies employed by political actors and news media to attract large audiences.

Further, contextual conditions might explain how strongly citizens from different countries are exposed to different emotions, such as anger while using social media. To answer these questions, we study emotional reactions to accounts of hyper-partisan news media and populist politicians and parties in six countries (Belgium, France, Germany, Switzerland, the United Kingdom, and the United States) and compare them to traditional news media and traditional political actors and parties. We argue that analyzing audience engagement is decisive in understanding whether and how different actors contribute to the emotionalization of political discussion, with important implications for the quality of public discussions in various countries (Mason, Citation2018).

Emotions in Online Political Discussions

Research is increasingly focusing on the role of emotions in political discussions on social media (Hameleers et al., Citation2016; Kim & Kim, Citation2019). Political communication on social media is characterized by emotions, primarily negative ones (Hasell & Weeks, Citation2016; Majó-Vázquez et al., Citation2020). This is relevant insofar as political attitudes consist of both cognitive and emotional components, and emotions can have a strong influence on the perception and evaluation of topics and events (Kühne et al., Citation2011) and influence the processing of information (Kühne & Schemer, Citation2015). Against this backdrop, it is crucial to understand which actors purposefully elicit emotional responses from online users and what content they use.

Moreover, recent research has shown that users are increasingly exposed to negative emotions in online debates (D’Errico & Paciello, Citation2018; Humprecht et al., Citation2020; Kim & Kim, Citation2019), which can both influence their perception of the topic and reinforce the affective polarization of societies (Anderson et al., Citation2014; Yarchi et al., Citation2020). Moreover, rude, disrespectful, or unreasonable comments can make people leave a discussion, undermining participation and inclusion in public discourse (J. W. Kim et al., Citation2021; Majó-Vázquez et al., Citation2020). For example, a multilingual study in 2020 found that comments evoking hostile or negative emotions accounted for 21% of overall conversations on the COVID-19 pandemic on Twitter (Majó-Vázquez et al., Citation2020).

In addition to commenting, users can show their emotional reactions on Facebook by clicking the “like” button or using one of the emoji icon buttons (“love,” “haha,” “wow,” “sad,” and “angry”). These mutually exclusive icons allow for a more nuanced reaction than just “liking” content. This form of engagement can serve as an emotional cue to other users, allowing news media and political actors to derive an opinion climate among their followers (Blassnig & Wirz, Citation2019). Researchers have cautioned that the targeted provocation of hostile emotions, as expressed in Facebook’s “angry” icon, leads to an emotionalized social media environment where spirals of aggression and in-group/out-group thinking unfold, ultimately driving the polarization of societies (D’Errico & Paciello, Citation2018; Humprecht et al., Citation2020).

We adopt a context-sensitive approach and theorize that emotional responses are not universal but are dependent on favorable opportunity structures. In comparative media use and effects research, opportunity structures are examined at four levels (Boomgaarden & Song, Citation2019): country characteristics, actor characteristics, media characteristics, and message characteristics. We thus use this logic to introduce our research questions and hypotheses.

Country Characteristics

Cultural and political contexts matter. Previous research has established that country differences exist regarding the amount of hostile emotions in user comments (Humprecht et al., Citation2020), which can be partly attributed to different discourse cultures (Hellmueller et al., Citation2021). For example, one study compared the United States to Germany and found that hostile comments and negative emotions were more prevalent in U.S. comments. In contrast, German comments used more of a neutral tone when responding to Facebook posts (Humprecht et al., Citation2020). In other words, although negative emotions in online discussions are prevalent in most countries, the phenomenon does not seem to be driven purely by technology and depends on contextual factors, such as different norms in political discussions (Freelon, Citation2013; Humprecht et al., Citation2022; Kalogeropoulos et al., Citation2017). Against this background, this study explores the following research question (RQ1): How do emotional user responses on social media, particularly Facebook, differ across countries?

Previous research has shown that spill-over effects of emotional online content on user responses occur and showed that sentiment as a content characteristic is a good predictor of audience emotional engagement (Blassnig & Wirz, Citation2019; Eberl et al., Citation2020). For example, negative news about salient issues such as war or climate change can elicit negative affective responses from audiences (Zollo et al., Citation2015). Correspondingly, news with positive framing can evoke positive emotions, such as hope or excitement (Lecheler, Bos, & Vliegenthart, Citation2015). In terms of online communication, negative online messages are more likely to be shared on both Facebook and Twitter, leading to higher visibility of this kind of content (Trilling et al., Citation2017). On Facebook, negative posts by political actors can lead to an increase in “shares” and comments, while “likes” show no clear relationship with the sentiment of a post (Bene, Citation2017; Heiss et al., Citation2019).

Actor Characteristics

Research into the communication strategies of populist actors has shown that populists deliberately provoke negative emotions, such as anger or fear, to reach their supporters (Skonieczny, Citation2018; Wirz, Citation2018). Such a deliberate provocation of emotions serves different purposes, such as creating a sense of urgency among their followers. By provoking hostile emotions such as anger, fear, and resentment, populist actors can make their followers feel that they are in a shared struggle and that they need to act quickly and decisively to defend themselves (Mudde, Citation2009). Another purpose is to exacerbate social and political divides. By provoking negative emotions, populists can deepen existing divisions and attract supporters who feel alienated from the “establishment” and the political system more broadly (Hameleers et al., Citation2016).

Moreover, populist actors frequently use emotional appeals to connect with their followers rather than relying on logical arguments or facts (Moffitt, Citation2018). By provoking hostile emotions, they can tap into their follower’s deepest fears and anxieties, making them more likely to follow their lead without questioning their claims or motives. Finally, by provoking anger or fear, populists can simplify complex issues and present themselves as the solution to these problems. This often involves identifying a common enemy or scapegoat (e.g., immigrants, elites, or other groups) as the source of the problems, which can further strengthen the bond between the populist leader and their supporters (Rhodes-Purdy et al., Citation2021). Therefore, researchers have argued that such populist communication strategies can lead to increased polarization and scapegoating, making it more difficult to find common ground and work together to address complex issues, such as to mitigate a pandemic (Ringe & Rennó, Citation2022).

Negativity and emotionality have thus been conceptualized as stylistic elements of populist communications, particularly on social media (Ernst, Blassnig, et al., Citation2019). Anger, in particular, leads to increased support for populist actors (Rico et al., Citation2017). A study of Italian politicians has shown that negative emotions are associated with populistic ideology and that emotional appeals by populists evoke more interactions and reactions, both negative and positive. “Love” reactions, for example, were elicited primarily by blaming the “dangerous others” (Martella & Bracciale, Citation2022).

Given the empirical evidence discussed above, we expect populist politicians (H1a) and populist parties (H1b) to elicit more angry reactions to their posts than traditional politicians and parties.

Media Characteristics

Like populist actors, alternative and hyper-partisan news media have been found to purposefully provoke negative emotions among their readers, particularly in social media (Tuomola & Wahl-Jorgensen, Citation2022). By blaming elites, such as the government or traditional news media, alternative and hyper-partisan news media trigger negative emotions among their readers and gain visibility on social media (Figenschou & Ihlebæk, Citation2019; Ihlebæk & Holter, Citation2021). In a study on negative emotions toward journalism in Korea, Shin et al. (Citation2021) show that alternative media deliberately stir up hostile emotions against journalists to strengthen their community and create a common enemy image. Negative emotions are thus deliberately used to attack traditional media and increase reach and legitimize themselves. Similar strategies can be observed in Norway (Figenschou & Ihlebæk, Citation2019), Germany (Boberg et al., Citation2020), and India (Bhat & Chadha, Citation2020). Studies from Germany (Müller & Freudenthaler, Citation2022) and Norway (Ihlebæk & Holter, Citation2021) further show that the most successful alternative media outlets have a far-right orientation.

Based on the research discussed above, we suggest that alternative and hyper-partisan news media provoke higher levels of angry reactions with their posts than traditional news media (H2).

Message Characteristics

In addition to the source, the content-related characteristics of media or political messages, such as the topics they address, may affect emotional engagement. For example, research has shown that salient topics such as immigration are associated with higher levels of emotional responses (Lecheler et al., Citation2015). Previous research on news stories has shown that topics such as racism, war, and political conflict lead to more hostile and toxic comments than news topics such as science and technology or arts and culture (Salminen et al., Citation2020). In a study of Brazilian news websites and Facebook pages, Rossini (Citation2022) shows that policy-related news stories and stories on international affairs or civil society received significantly more angry comments than other topics, such as general politics or celebrities. However, there is still little empirical evidence on the topics alternative media and populist politicians use to evoke emotions in their audiences, especially anger. In this study, we explore research question 2 (RQ 2): Which type of content of Facebook posts by alternative/hyper-partisan media or populist politicians (candidates/parties) evoke particularly high levels of angry emotions among users?

Data and Methods

We examined the posts and user engagement levels of 148 Facebook pages of different actors and outlets from April 14, 2020, to June 30, 2020, in six countries, namely Belgium, France, Germany, Switzerland, the UK, and the US. During this period, when lockdowns were still in place in most countries studied, a particularly large number of people mostly stayed at home and used Facebook to obtain information (Newman et al., Citation2021). At the time of the study, countries were affected by the pandemic to varying degrees. Confirmed infections per day were highest in France (172.70) and Belgium (109.39), followed by the US (89.55), the UK (63.22), Germany (60.20), and Switzerland (44.33).Footnote1 The countries also differed in terms of the political situation at the time of the study. Due to the different political and health systems, crisis management varied, ranging from top-down approaches (e.g., France), top-down with operational bottom-up (Italy) to negotiated and coordinated top-down with significant bottom-up approaches in multi-party systems (e.g., Belgium and Germany) (Bouckaert et al., Citation2020). The U.S. exhibited a mix of top-down approaches, such as federal directives and vaccine distribution strategies, and bottom-up responses, characterized by varied state policies, local initiatives and grassroot community actions (OECD, Citation2022). In almost all countries studied, populist parties assumed an opposition function and criticized the measures imposed as too far-reaching (e.g., Switzerland, Germany, Belgium) or not far-reaching enough (France) (Bolleyer & Salát, Citation2021). To compare the potentially exceptional period under study, we replicated our study in three countries (BE, FR, US) for two periods before the pandemic (four weeks in January and February 2019 and 2020; see Appendix).

We collected data from a wide range of public accounts, including media outlets (alternative/hyper-partisan, public broadcasters, up-market, mass market, and tabloid news media), politicians, and political parties (government, opposition, populist; see ). The selection of media accounts followed an audience-based approach in that we selected functional equivalents in terms of audience and reach (according to which Fox News, for example, is more similar to audience- and opinion-rich tabloids like Daily Mail and Bild than it is to niche papers in the alternative media sector; see Peck (Citation2022).To ensure that we selected functional equivalents in all countries, we sampled the Facebook accounts based on 1) importance (rank of the actor or reach of the news outlet) and 2) the current literature (Engesser et al., Citation2016; Ernst, Esser, et al., Citation2019). In addition, we selected hyper-partisan/alternative news outlets based on data from the Digital News Report 2020 (Newman et al., Citation2020) and on their self-descriptions as “alternative” (e.g., on their websites). To reflect the diversity and heterogeneity of these outlets in the countries under study, we sampled five to eight Facebook accounts per country. We understand populism as a communication phenomenon that is expressed in content and style (de Vreese et al., Citation2018). Therefore, to sample functional equivalents of populist actors across different countries, we drew on studies that used communication content from an international set of actors to categorize populism. Ernst, Blassnig, et al. (Citation2019) define core populist messages on the dimensions of anti-elitism, people-centrism, and preservation of sovereignty. Timbro (Citation2019) uses similar dimensions and focuses on authoritarianism for categorization. Furthermore, we draw on data from the Global Populism Database, which contains over 1100 speeches by political actors. Various elements of populist communication were coded (with reference to text, tonality, quality of arguments, style, etc.), elements that can also be attributed to concepts such as popular centrism and anti-elitism. Although we could not identify a populist party in the United States based on our sources, we did identify a populist politician (i.e., Donald Trump). By applying a mixed-methods approach of automated and qualitative text analysis, we analyzed how various countries and actor groups differ in terms of emotional user reactions and what kind of content triggers anger on Facebook.

Table 1. Sample and number of posts.

Emotional Reactions

To create the dataset, we accessed public Facebook data (posts and comments) through the CrowdTangle API and the Facebook Graph API using the Facepager application (Jünger & Keyling, Citation2013) from April 14 to June 21, 2020. CrowdTangle is a Meta owned analytic platform that tracks posts published by verified public profiles and groups. The resulting dataset consisted of N = 175,539 Facebook posts (BE: n = 18,501; CH: n = 10,542; DE: n = 24,663; FR: n = 36,611; U.K: n = 43,1211; U.S: n = 46,011). The metadata included emotional reactions (love, haha, wow, sad, and angry) as well as numbers of likes, shares, and comments for each post. We compared emotional reactions across countries and actor groups. Prior to the analysis, we normalized the counts of emotions against the number of followers of each Facebook account to allow for comparability between accounts, some of which varied greatly in their number of followers. This approach was chosen to holistically capture the diverse emotional reactions across various account sizes and ensure that smaller accounts, which might have fewer responses to individual posts, were accurately represented. It is vital to note that while the numbers may not intuitively align with an absolute scale, they are indicative of the relative engagement efficiencies of posts within and across countries. Additionally, the normalization was based on the number of followers an account had at the time of our data sampling. Given platform constraints, this method offers the closest representation of potential post engagement. A high normalized number of emotion expressions indicates what proportion of an account’s followers have actively left emojis. This value has the advantage over the total count that the accounts can be compared directly, even though their followers (and thus the engagement potential) differ greatly. A normalized count of .7, for example, means that 70% of the followers have expressed emotions.

Posts

In accordance with our theoretical expectations regarding favorable opportunity structures, we were particularly interested in posts circulated by accounts of populist political actors and alternative/hyper-partisan news media that generated the highest levels of angry reactions. To analyze the data, we relied on relative measurements. First, we filtered for all posts with angry reactions and subsequently selected the highest quartile in terms of the normalized number of angry reactions (i.e., angry reactions divided by the number of followers). To pinpoint the content that elicits notably high angry reactions, we adopted a relational approach, focusing on the identification of “overused” words. These are terms that a specific actor or media outlet uses far more frequently than the reference group’s average. By blending ’corpus-based’ and ’corpus-driven’ techniques (Rayson, Citation2008), we spotlight essential words that can be compared across individual accounts. Our method involved contrasting a chosen post’s content against all other posts from the same actor category (e.g., traditional politicians vs. news media). For each word or symbol (referred to as ’tokens’), we assessed its observed versus expected appearance rate, focusing on the most anger-inducing posts. To refine our analysis, we removed common words, punctuation, single letters, numbers, and URLs. This process yielded a “weight” for each token, revealing its prominence in anger-evoking posts as either ’overused’ or ’underused.’ Importantly, our metric also integrated the absolute frequency of each token, adjusted logarithmically for scale. Through this method, we could discern which content elements – expressed as specific words – were most associated with heightened angry responses.

In a second step, we conducted a qualitative content analysis of the 10 most anger-provoking posts per account to be able to interpret the results of the automated analysis (for similar approaches, see Hellmueller et al., Citation2021). We inductively identified emerging themes by a close reading of the posts, summarized the posts into groups of themes, and interpreted the themes. This step was conducted independently for each country, arriving at a set of granular themes that were summarized and refined until data saturation was reached. We then compared themes and communication styles based on Engesser et al. (Citation2016) study of populist rhetoric. Those authors have defined five ideological key elements of populist rhetoric (advocating for the people, attacking the elites, ostracizing others, invoking the heartland, and emphasizing the sovereignty of the people). We used these elements as heuristic categories for our text analysis and classified the posts accordingly. Finally, we compared the distribution of identified themes across actor types and countries.

Results

To answer RQ1, we first compared different emotional reactions between the countries under study (see ). Regarding emotions, “angry” is the most common and is especially widespread in Belgium, Germany, and the United States. In most countries, “angry” reactions represent the most common form of user engagement, except for the United Kingdom, where “love” is about as common as anger. In Switzerland, Germany, and France, “haha” is the second most frequently used emotion, while in Belgium, the United Kingdom, and the United States, “love” is significantly more frequent than “haha.” “Sad” and “wow,” in contrast, occur comparatively rarely in all countries. Overall, we found that the use of emojis differs greatly between countries. While the most used emoji, “angry,” has a normalized count of .07 in Switzerland, it reaches a normalized count of .38 in Belgium. The use of “love” also varies from .03 in Germany to .33 in Belgium (normalized values). Belgian users use all available emojis comparatively often, while Swiss users are generally more reserved in their use of emojis. In the remaining countries, only “angry” and “love” or “haha” are used more often (see ).

Figure 1. Proportion of emotional user reactions in traditional vs populist politicians per country.

Figure shows normalized values of individual accounts per country. Normalized values indicate the number of emotions per outlet in relation to the number of followers of the respective account.
Figure 1. Proportion of emotional user reactions in traditional vs populist politicians per country.

Figure 2. Proportion of emotional user reactions in traditional vs populist parties per country.

Figure shows normalized values of individual accounts per country. Normalized values indicate the number of emotions per outlet in relation to the number of followers of the respective account.
Figure 2. Proportion of emotional user reactions in traditional vs populist parties per country.

Figure 3. Proportion of emotional user reactions in mainstream vs alternative media per country.

Figure shows normalized values of individual accounts per country. Normalized values indicate the number of emotions per outlet in relation to the number of followers of the respective account.
Figure 3. Proportion of emotional user reactions in mainstream vs alternative media per country.

Table 2. Means of Facebook reactions per country.

Next, we compared the levels of emotional reactions among various actor types. We assumed that populist politicians (H1a) and parties (H1b) receive more angry reactions compared to traditional politicians and parties. As and show, populist politicians in most countries under study provoked significantly higher levels of “angry” reactions relative to the number of their followers compared with traditional politicians. Donald Trump in the United States was an exception, as he received mostly “love” reactions from his followers. Traditional politicians also received mostly “love” reactions, except for Germany, where “angry” reactions predominated (but not as strongly as on the Facebook account of the German populist politician).

Figure 4a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist politicians per country (Belgium, France, Germany).

Boxes show the distribution per actor group. Points indicate means. Scales adjusted per country due to widely varying follower and user reactions.
Figure 4a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist politicians per country (Belgium, France, Germany).

Figure 4b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist politicians per country (Switzerland, UK, US).

Figure 4b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist politicians per country (Switzerland, UK, US).

We found a similar pattern for the Facebook accounts of populist parties, where “angry” reactions predominated (see and ). Since we could not sample a distinct populist party in the United States, we compared the Democratic and the Republican Parties instead. While on the account of the Democratic Party “angry” was the most common emotional reaction, “love” was found most frequently on the account of the Republican Party. On the accounts of populist parties in the remaining countries, “angry” was again the dominant emotional reaction, particularly in Germany and Switzerland. Traditional parties elicited mostly “love” reactions, except for Germany, where those parties provoked more “haha” reactions. Two-sided t-tests showed that populist politicians and parties provoked significantly more “angry” reactions than other politicians and parties across all countries under study (p = .000). Based on these findings, we accept our hypotheses H1a and H1b.

Figure 5a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist parties per country (France, Belgium, Germany).

Boxes show the distribution per actor group. Scales adjusted per country due to widely varying follower and user reactions.
Figure 5a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist parties per country (France, Belgium, Germany).

Figure 5b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist parties per country (Switzerland, UK, US).

Figure 5b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of traditional vs. populist parties per country (Switzerland, UK, US).

In addition, we assumed that hyper-partisan and alternative news media would elicit more “angry” reactions from their Facebook followers compared with traditional news media. As and show, this is true for all countries under study. In Germany, France, and Belgium, alternative and hyper-partisan news media provoked the highest levels of “angry” reactions. Sadness, in contrast, was more frequent in the accounts of traditional news media. Finally, “love” did not occur as an emotional user reaction in either traditional or alternative/hyper-partisan media accounts – unlike in the accounts of political actors. Based on these findings, we accepted H2.

Figure 6a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of mainstream vs. alternative media per country.

Figure 6a. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of mainstream vs. alternative media per country.

Figure 6b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of mainstream vs. alternative media per country.

Boxes show the distribution per actor group. Scales adjusted per country due to widely varying follower and user reactions.
Figure 6b. Emotional user reactions (angry and love) of mainstream vs. alternative media per country.

Further, we were interested in the posts that elicited the highest levels of angry reactions (RQ2). To answer this question, we first identified the quartile of posts from populist actors and alternative/hyper-partisan media that received the highest number of angry reactions. In a second step, we examined which words occurred more frequently in these posts than in other posts within the respective actor group. In a third step, we examined the 10 posts with the highest number of angry reactions per account using qualitative content analysis to identify dominant themes.

shows the 10 most overused words for populist politicians and parties per country. After a close reading of all messages, we assigned the most overused words to two categories: immigration and the blaming of political opponents. While in the European countries under study, immigration seems to be a highly important topic for populist actors, the accounts of Donald Trump and the GOP focus more on blaming political enemies. That right-wing populists actors focus on immigration is far from new (Bene et al., Citation2022), but this topic was pivotal and provoked much anger even in a period when a global health-crisis was affecting people all over the world.

Table 3. Top 10 key words in anger-provoking Facebook posts by media outlet.

Similar patterns can be observed regarding alternative/hyper-partisan media (). These outlets also focus on immigration and blaming national governments, for example, accusing migrants of being responsible for the spread of the coronavirus or accusing governments of trying to force the population to undergo a supposedly dangerous vaccination against COVID-19. In the United States, the hyper-partisan news outlet Breitbart focuses exclusively on attacking and blaming the Democrats for current problems. In Belgium and the United Kingdom, the alternative/hyper-partisan media combine the topics of immigration and elite blaming. These media outlets attack leftist or liberal parties and politicians and report on the alleged aggressive or illegal behavior of migrants. In Switzerland, Germany, and France, however, the COVID-19 pandemic was more present. These posts were vaccine-skeptical and included allegations of possible planned forced vaccinations of the general population. The German outlet is an outlier because of its sensationalist style. It started almost every post with the words “unbelievable” or “incredible” written in capital letters and asked its followers to “please share the article.”

Table 4. Top 10 key words in anger-provoking Facebook posts by politicians (top) and parties (bottom).

To test whether our findings were influenced by the exceptional period of the pandemic, we conducted a replication analysis using data from 2019 and 2020, as documented in the Appendix. This analysis was performed in three countries (BE, FR, U.S.) prior to the outbreak of the pandemic (three weeks in January 2019 and in January 2020). We focused on two reaction types (love and anger) and four actor types (alternative media, traditional media, populist politicians, and traditional politicians). Our results confirm our initial findings, specifically regarding the patterns observed across media accounts. The data demonstrates consistently high levels of angry reactions generated by alternative media in comparison to traditional media in all three countries. The differences in emotional reactions to posts by political actors were even more pronounced. Notably, at the outset of the pandemic, reactions to posts made by traditional politicians were generally more positive. In France, emotional reactions were at a comparatively low level in 2019. In January 2020, especially the love reactions increased, which was probably related to the upcoming important local elections in March 2020. During the pandemic (May 2020), anger reactions also increased sharply, especially in the accounts of populist politicians. In Belgium, anger reactions increased significantly at the beginning of 2020, especially on the account of populist politicians, and, in addition, love reactions toward traditional politicians increased during the pandemic. In the U.S., love reactions to accounts of traditional politicians in particular had increased in the May 2020 study period compared to the two comparison periods.

Overall, the findings of our replication analysis indicate that the type and amount of emotional reactions are influenced by the political context and concrete events. There is little doubt that the pandemic has led to an increase in the emotional intensity of reactions, particularly in terms of anger and love. This trend was observed across all three countries, with heightened emotional reactions recorded in 2020, particularly during the pandemic.

Our in-depth qualitative analysis confirmed these patterns. The analysis showed that the 10 posts per outlet that received the highest numbers of “angry” reactions consisted of populist elements, as described by Engesser et al. (Citation2016), such as anti-elitism (e.g., blaming the government or political opponents for current problems) and exclusion (e.g., discrediting specific social groups, such as immigrants). In one of her posts, which received more than 9,000 “angry” reactions, the German populist politician Alice Weidel claimed that a German pensioner couple was thrown out of their apartment so that asylum seekers could move in. Weidel used an emotional style in her posts, for example, by calling reported events “outrageous.” This style is adopted in the users’ reactions, which points to spill-over effects between post content and user engagement. Further, the French far right populist party Rassemblement National (RN) posted a message claiming that illegal immigrants would be allowed to demonstrate in France and called for support for a petition against refugees. A similar rhetoric can be found in the posts of the German far right populist party AfD (Alternative für Deutschland). One post, which received more than 8,000 reactions, referred to an increase in broadcasting fees for public media in Germany. In this context, the AfD spoke of a “compulsory fee” and called on users to donate to a counter-campaign. Similarly, the United Kingdom’s Brexit Party posted a video claiming that the mainstream media would deliberately hide criminal actions by refugees, which elicited 2,142 angry user reactions. In the United States, former President Donald Trump addressed similar topics as populist politicians in other countries but received mostly “love” from his followers. For example, his post stating that “The United States of America will be designating ANTIFA as a Terrorist Organization” generated 122,646 love reactions. We will elaborate on the remarkable difference in terms or reactions in the next section.

Regarding alternative and hyper-partisan media, we also find populist elements, such as exclusion and anti-elitism. For example, in one of its posts with 1,088 “angry” reactions, the German alternative news media Tichys Einblick attacked then German Chancellor Angela Merkel by accusing her of buying her power retention at the European level with German taxpayers’ money. The Belgian alternative outlet SCEPTR falsely claimed in a post that former minister Bert Anciaux wanted to introduce Arabic as an official language in Belgium, which provoked 1,696 “angry” reactions.

In summary, we found that while the frequency of various emotional user reactions on Facebook differs between countries, anger dominates across most countries (see ). In some countries (Belgium, the United Kingdom, and the United States), “love” is also a frequently used emotional reaction. These country differences can be attributed primarily to a few actors who provoke a high number of emotional reactions, particularly anger, namely populist candidates, parties, and alternative/hyper-partisan news media. Our analyses of the most anger-eliciting posts showed that these messages frequently contain populist rhetoric, including elements such as anti-elitism and exclusion, focusing on themes such as immigration and blaming political opponents.

Table 5. Comparisons of normalized mean scores for “love” and “angry” emotions between account types (media, politicians, parties) across countries.

Conclusion

This study uses a comparative analytical approach to improve our understanding of the favorable opportunity structures that facilitate emotionally charged political discourse. Specifically, we examine the systematic differences in emotional responses exhibited by users and how these differences are related to country of origin, types of political actors, types of media, and characteristics of messages. Our research takes into account the event environment, specifically a pandemic health crisis, and how differently configured political systems and diverse constellations of political actors, media actors, and social media users respond to this crisis.

To establish the robustness of our observations, we conducted a replication analysis using data from 2019, which shows that the underlying patterns persist over time. However, it is noteworthy that the pandemic has exacerbated the situation in certain countries. Specifically, we find that populist politicians and alternative media have skillfully used their established narrative frameworks and applied them to the health crisis to elicit emotional responses, particularly anger and affection (love), from their supporters. This finding underscores the adaptability of these actors in applying their communication strategies even in times of crisis, aligning the crisis context with their core themes, such as blaming the government, the opposition, and foreigners, to reinforce their ideological narratives.

In some countries, alternative and hyper-partisan media have also contributed to the spread of misinformation and conspiracy theories about the COVID-19 vaccine. Nevertheless, our study shows that emotional expressions are more prevalent in certain countries, such as Belgium and the United States, than in others, regardless of the contextual background of the pandemic.

In light of these observations, we conclude that the emotionalization of national Facebook spheres is primarily driven by a few high-profile accounts characterized by predominantly hyper-partisan and populist content. The anger generated by these accounts can generate uncivil forms of polarization and facilitate the spread of ideologically motivated misinformation (Rossini, Citation2022; Staender et al., Citation2021). The anger-driven engagement by their followers provides an impetus for alternative and hyper-partisan news outlets and populist figures to disseminate further contentious content, increasing the potential to translate misleading and hostile information into tangible political action.

It is important to recognize that not all forms of anger-inducing messaging produce the same results. Some actors are able to evoke positive emotions, even “love,” through seemingly hostile messages because of their loyal and like-minded followers. Former US President Donald Trump, for example, stands out as a notable outlier in this context. His messages predominantly elicited positive responses from his followers, especially when he targeted political opponents. This finding is consistent with previous research suggesting that attacking opponents is a successful strategy for pleasing populist supporters (Martella & Bracciale, Citation2022). Furthermore, Trump held the position of an incumbent at the time of our data collection, which significantly influenced the emotional responses of his followers. This may explain why he received numerous positive responses in support of his policies, despite criticism from his opponents. As a leader, Trump bore more resemblance to traditional politicians than to oppositional populist figures in Europe. Encouragingly, our replication analysis shows the persistence of this pattern across two pre-pandemic time points.

Future research would be well-served by examining how the emotional tenor of these responses evolved as Trump lost the 2020 election and assumed an “outsider” role. Unfortunately, this investigation was not feasible due to Meta’s prolonged suspension of Trump’s account, which was only reinstated (in February 2023) after the completion of this study.

What does the frequency of angry emotions tell us about political communication and the quality of public discourse in different countries?

First, it is imperative to acknowledge that polarizing issues, assigning blame, and using attack rhetoric are commonly used strategies to captivate audiences and elicit emotional responses in different countries. As Ihlebæk and Holter (Citation2021) argue, the anger generated by alternative media often becomes an integral part of users’ identities as oppositional truth-seekers. This anger is fueled by their confrontation with mainstream sources and validated by alternative sources. This phenomenon highlights the complex relationship between anger, identity, and political communication.

Second, it is crucial to consider the role of cultural and political contexts in explaining the variation in the prevalence of hostile emotions, such as anger, in online discourse across countries (Hellmueller et al., Citation2021; Humprecht et al., Citation2020). The extent to which populist political figures and alternative, hyper-partisan news outlets achieve success and visibility in a given country appears to correlate directly with the prominence of hostile emotions, particularly anger, in social media discourse. In such a national environment, the pervasive presence of angry emotions can contribute to the degradation of public discourse and distort citizens’ perceptions of prevailing public opinion. Furthermore, hostile conversations can undermine trust in the political process (Mutz, Citation2015) and lead individuals to disengage from political information (Kim et al., Citation2021; Majó-Vázquez et al., Citation2020).

Nonetheless, it is worth noting that negative emotions, including anger, can be intertwined with more positive feelings of perseverance and resilience among populist voters and alternative media readers. These individuals often find affirmation of their beliefs and camaraderie with like-minded users.

In sum, the prevalence of angry emotions in political communication has profound implications for the quality of public discourse in different countries. By examining the strategies employed by political actors, the cultural and political contexts, and the potential interplay of emotions, our opportunity-structure approach can be helpful for developing targeted strategies to improve the quality of public discourse and promote informed and constructive political engagement in diverse societies.

There are several limitations to our study. Most importantly, based on our analysis, we cannot determine whether user reactions are related to the content of the post or to the author. For example, a populist post mostly leads to angry reactions because of the claim (e.g., that immigrants misbehave), but the anger could also be directed to the populist politicians because of the offensive language used. Reactions like “haha” and “wow” can also be ambiguous and express irony or cynicism, for example. To better understand the underlying motivations behind these reactions, future studies should examine the effects of different types of content by various actor groups. In addition, for the categorization of populist actors, we drew on the current research literature, which understands populism as political communication that manifests itself in content and style. However, this approach ignores the fact that a populist communication style consists of elements that are also sporadically used by traditional politicians. Such nuances are particularly important for in-depth analyses of individual countries or case studies and should be considered in further research. Another limitation relates to our sample of alternative and hyper-partisan media outlets. This category is very broad, and their Facebook accounts vary widely. To address this issue, we based our sampling decisions on several indicators, including reach and self-description of the outlets. Future research should collect more data on these media outlets, develop typologies, and identify functional equivalents across countries. Finally, more cross-national research is needed on the nature of Facebook reactions. In particular, the question arises as to why users in some countries generally express emotional reactions more often than users in other countries and how emotionalization on Facebook is perceived in different countries. Thus, future research on the emotionalization of social media spheres should not only focus on negative emotions in posts and comments to describe information environments but also examine discourse cultures and the general emotional expression of citizens in various countries on- and offline.

Our analysis of users’ emotional reactions belongs to a recent line of comparative research that understands social media environments as opportunity structures that favor or disfavor certain consumption patterns and media effects. Our findings provide a more nuanced view of the conditions that foster political misinformation and emotional contagion on social media (Kramer et al., Citation2014). The similarities across countries show how some actors with extreme positions effectively utilize the mechanisms Facebook provides. Through evidence from different nations, actor types, media accounts, and message characteristics, our study contributes to a better understanding of these mechanisms.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (2 MB)

Disclosure Statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Supplementary Material

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website at https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2024.2350416

Additional information

Funding

The work was supported by the Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung [100017L_182253].

Notes on contributors

Edda Humprecht

Edda Humprecht is professor of Digitalization & the Public Sphere at the Institute of Communication Science at the University of Jena. Her research focuses on digital political communication from a cross-national perspective.

Michael Amsler

Michael Amsler is a postdoc in the Department of Communication and Media Research at the University of Zurich. His research focuses on computational methods.

Frank Esser

Frank Esser is professor of International & Comparative Media Research in the Department of Communication and Media Research at the University of Zurich. His research focuses on cross-national studies of news journalism and political communication.

Peter Van Aelst

Peter Van Aelst is a research professor in the Department of Political Science at the University of Antwerp and a founding member of the research group ‘Media, Movements and Politics’ (M2P). His research focuses on political communication.

Notes

1. Data obtained from: https://ourworldindata.org/, numbers refer to daily new confirmed COVID-19 cases per million people as of April 14, 2020.

References

  • Anderson, A., Brossard, D., Scheufele, D., Xenos, M. A., & Ladwig, P. (2014). The “nasty effect:” online incivility and risk perceptions of emerging technologies. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 19(3), 373–387. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12009
  • Bene, M. (2017). Go viral on the Facebook! Interactions between candidates and followers on Facebook during the Hungarian general election campaign of 2014. Information Communication and Society, 20(4), 513–529. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2016.1198411
  • Bene, M., Ceron, A., Fenoll, V., Haßler, J., Kruschinski, S., Larsson, A. O., Magin, M., Schlosser, K., & Wurst, A.-K. (2022). Keep them engaged! Investigating the effects of self-centered social media communication style on user engagement in 12 European countries. Political Communication, 39(4), 429–453. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2022.2042435
  • Bhat, P., & Chadha, K. (2020). Anti-media populism: Expressions of media distrust by right-wing media in India. Journal of International & Intercultural Communication, 13(2), 166–182. https://doi.org/10.1080/17513057.2020.1739320
  • Blassnig, S., & Wirz, D. S. (2019). Populist and popular: An experiment on the drivers of user reactions to populist posts on facebook. Social Media + Society, 5(4), 205630511989006. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305119890062
  • Boberg, S., Quandt, T., Schatto-Eckrodt, T., & Frischlich, L. (2020). Pandemic populism: Facebook pages of alternative news media and the corona crisis—A computational content analysis. http://arxiv.org/abs/2004.02566
  • Bolleyer, N., & Salát, O. (2021). Parliaments in times of crisis: COVID-19, populism and executive dominance. West European Politics, 44(5–6), 1103–1128. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2021.1930733
  • Boomgaarden, H. G., & Song, H. (2019). Media use and its effects in a cross-national perspective. KZfSS Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie, 71(S1), 545–571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11577-019-00596-9
  • Bouckaert, G., Galli, D., Kuhlmann, S., Reiter, R., & Van Hecke, S. (2020). European coronationalism? A hot spot governing a pandemic crisis. Public Administration Review, 80(5), 765–773. https://doi.org/10.1111/puar.13242
  • D’Errico, F., & Paciello, M. (2018). Online moral disengagement and hostile emotions in discussions on hosting immigrants. Internet Research, 28(5), 1313–1335. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-03-2017-0119
  • de Vreese, C. H., Esser, F., Aalberg, T., Reinemann, C., & Stanyer, J. (2018). Populism as an expression of political communication content and style: A new perspective. The International Journal of Press/politics, 23(4), 423–438. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161218790035
  • Eberl, J. M., Tolochko, P., Jost, P., Heidenreich, T., & Boomgaarden, H. G. (2020). What’s in a post? How sentiment and issue salience affect users’ emotional reactions on facebook. Journal of Information Technology & Politics, 17(1), 48–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/19331681.2019.1710318
  • Engesser, S., Ernst, N., Esser, F., & Büchel, F. (2016). Populism and social media: How politicians spread a fragmented ideology. Information, Communication & Society, 0(0), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2016.1207697
  • Ernst, N., Blassnig, S., Engesser, S., Büchel, F., & Esser, F. (2019). Populists prefer social media over talk shows: An analysis of populist messages and stylistic elements across six countries. Social Media + Society, 5(1), 205630511882335. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305118823358
  • Ernst, N., Esser, F., Blassnig, S., & Engesser, S. (2019). Favorable opportunity structures for populist communication: Comparing different types of politicians and issues in social media, television and the press. The International Journal of Press/politics, 24(2), 165–188. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161218819430
  • Figenschou, T. U., & Ihlebæk, K. A. (2019). Media criticism from the far-right: Attacking from many angles. Journalism Practice, 13(8), 901–905. https://doi.org/10.1080/17512786.2019.1647112
  • Freelon, D. (2013). Discourse architecture, ideology, and democratic norms in online political discussion. New Media & Society. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813513259
  • Gerstlé, J., & Nai, A. (2019). Negativity, emotionality and populist rhetoric in election campaigns worldwide, and their effects on media attention and electoral success. European Journal of Communication, 34(4), 410–444. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267323119861875
  • Hameleers, M., Bos, L., & de Vreese, C. (2016). “They did it”: The effects of emotionalized blame attribution in populist communication. Communication Research, 44(6), 870–900. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650216644026
  • Hasell, A., & Weeks, B. E. (2016). Partisan provocation: The role of partisan news use and emotional responses in political information sharing in social media. Human Communication Research, 42(4), 641–661. https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12092
  • Heiss, R., Schmuck, D., & Matthes, J. (2019). What drives interaction in political actors’ Facebook posts? Profile and content predictors of user engagement and political actors’ reactions. Information Communication and Society, 22(10), 1497–1513. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2018.1445273
  • Hellmueller, L., Lischka, J. A., & Humprecht, E. (2021). Shaping (non)-discursive social media spaces: Cross-national typologies of news organizations’ heavy commenters. New Media & Society, 23(11), 3249–3267. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444820946454
  • Hiaeshutter-Rice, D., & Weeks, B. (2021). Understanding audience engagement with mainstream and alternative news posts on facebook. Digital Journalism, 9(5), 519–548. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.1924068
  • Humprecht, E., Castro Herrero, L., Blassnig, S., Brüggemann, M., & Engesser, S. (2022). Media systems in the digital age: An empirical comparison of 30 countries. Journal of Communication, 72(2), 145–164. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqab054
  • Humprecht, E., Hellmueller, L., & Lischka, J. A. (2020). Hostile emotions in news comments: A cross-national analysis of Facebook discussions. Social Media + Society, 6(1), 205630512091248. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305120912481
  • Ihlebæk, K. A., & Holter, C. R. (2021). Hostile emotions: An exploratory study of far-right online commenters and their emotional connection to traditional and alternative news media. Journalism, 22(5), 1207–1222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1464884920985726
  • Jünger, J., & Keyling, T. (2013). Facepager Ein Programm zur automatisierten Datenerhebung im Netz. University of Greifswald. https://www.univie.ac.at/digitalmethods/wp-content/uploads/2013/10/Facepager_2013.pdf
  • Kalogeropoulos, A., Negredo, S., Picone, I., & Nielsen, R. K. (2017). Who shares and comments on news?: A cross-national comparative analysis of online and social media participation. Social Media & Society, 3(4), 205630511773575. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305117735754
  • Kim, J. W., Guess, A., Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2021). The distorting prism of social media: How self-selection and exposure to incivility fuel online comment toxicity. Journal of Communication, 71(6), 922–946. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqab034
  • Kim, Y., & Kim, Y. (2019). Incivility on Facebook and political polarization: The mediating role of seeking further comments and negative emotion. Computers in Human Behavior, 99(May), 219–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.022
  • Kramer, A. D. I., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(29), 10779. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1412583111
  • Kühne, R., & Schemer, C. (2015). The emotional effects of news frames on information processing and opinion formation. Communication Research, 42(3), 387–407. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650213514599
  • Kühne, R., Schemer, C., Matthes, J., & Wirth, W. (2011). Affective priming in political campaigns: How Campaign-induced emotions prime political opinions. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 23(4), 485–507. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edr004
  • Larsson, A. O. (2019). News use as amplification: Norwegian national, regional, and hyperpartisan media on facebook. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 96(3), 721–741. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699019831439
  • Lecheler, S., Bos, L., & Vliegenthart, R. (2015). The mediating role of emotions: News framing effects on opinions about immigration. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 92(4), 812–838. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699015596338
  • Majó-Vázquez, S., Nielsen, R. K., Verdú, J., & Rao, N. (2020). Volume and patterns of toxicity in social media conversations during the covid-19 pandemic ( Factsheet, p. 12). Reuters Institut for the Study of Journalism.
  • Martella, A., & Bracciale, R. (2022). Populism and emotions: Italian political leaders’ communicative strategies to engage Facebook users. Innovation: The European Journal of Social Science Research, 35(1), 65–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/13511610.2021.1951681
  • Mason, L. (2018). Uncivil agreement: How politics became our identity. University of Chicago Press.
  • Moffitt, B. (2018). Populism 2.0: Social media and the false allure of ‘unmediated’representation. In P. Taggart, G. Fitzi J. Mackert, & B. S. Turner (Eds.), In Populism and the crisis of democracy (pp. 30–46). Routledge.
  • Mudde, C. (2009). Populist radical right parties in Europe. Cambridge University Press Cambridge.
  • Müller, P., & Freudenthaler, R. (2022). Right-wing, populist, controlled by foreign powers? Topic diversification and partisanship in the content structures of German-language alternative media. Digital Journalism, 10(8), 1363–1386. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2022.2058972
  • Mutz, D. C. (2015). In-your-face politics: The consequences of uncivil media. Princeton University Press.
  • Neuman, W. R., Marcus, G. E., & MacKuen, M. B. (2018). Hardwired for news: Affective intelligence and political attention. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 62(4), 614–635. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838151.2018.1523169
  • Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Schulz, A., Andı, S., & Nielsen, R. K. (2020). Reuters institute digital news report 2020. Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism.
  • Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Schulz, A., Andı, S., Robertson, C. T., & Nielsen, R. K. (2021). Digital news report 2021. Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism.
  • OECD. (2022). First lessons from government evaluations of COVID-19 responses: A synthesis (OECD policy responses to coronavirus (COVID-19)) [OECD policy responses to coronavirus (COVID-19)]. https://doi.org/10.1787/483507d6-en
  • Peck, R. (2022). Comparing populist media: From Fox News to the Young Turks, from cable to YouTube, from right to left. Television & New Media, 24(6), 599–615. https://doi.org/10.1177/15274764221114349
  • Rathje, S., Bavel, J. J. V., & Linden, S. V. D. (2021). Out-group animosity drives engagement on social media. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(26), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2024292118/-/DCSupplemental.Published
  • Rayson, P. (2008). From key words to key semantic domains. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 13(4), 519–549. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.13.4.06ray
  • Rhodes-Purdy, M., Navarre, R., & Utych, S. M. (2021). Populist psychology: Economics, culture, and emotions. The Journal of Politics, 83(4), 1559–1572. https://doi.org/10.1086/715168
  • Rico, G., Guinjoan, M., & Anduiza, E. (2017). The emotional underpinnings of populism: How anger and fear affect populist attitudes. Swiss Political Science Review, 23(4), 444–461. https://doi.org/10.1111/spsr.12261
  • Ringe, N., & Rennó, L. (2022). Populists and the pandemic. Populists and the pandemic, 1.
  • Rossini, P. (2022). Beyond incivility: Understanding patterns of uncivil and intolerant discourse in online political talk. Communication Research, 49(3), 399–425. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650220921314
  • Salminen, J., Hopf, M., Chowdhury, S. A., Jung, S., Almerekhi, H., & Jansen, B. J. (2020). Developing an online hate classifier for multiple social media platforms. Human-Centric Computing and Information Sciences, 10(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13673-019-0205-6
  • Shin, W., Kim, C., & Joo, J. (2021). Hating journalism: Anti-press discourse and negative emotions toward journalism in Korea. Journalism, 22(5), 1239–1255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1464884920985729
  • Skonieczny, A. (2018). Emotions and political narratives: Populism, Trump and trade. Politics & Governance, 6(4), 62–72. https://doi.org/10.17645/pag.v6i4.1574
  • Staender, A., Humprecht, E., Esser, F., Morosoli, S., & Van Aelst, P. (2021). Is sensationalist disinformation more effective? Three facilitating factors at the national, individual, and situational level. Digital Journalism, 0(0), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.1966315
  • Timbro. (2019). Timbro Authoritarian Populism Index. https://populismindex.com/
  • Trilling, D., Tolochko, P., & Burscher, B. (2017). From newsworthiness to Shareworthiness. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 94(1), 38–60. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699016654682
  • Tuomola, S., & Wahl-Jorgensen, K. (2022). Emotion mobilisation through the imagery of people in Finnish-language right-wing alternative media. Digital Journalism, 11(1), 61–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2022.2061551
  • Wirz, D. S. (2018). Persuasion through emotion? An experimental test of the emotion-eliciting nature of populist communication. International Journal of Communication, 12(25), 1114–1138. https://doi.org/10.1166/jnn.2014.8184
  • Yarchi, M., Baden, C., & Kligler-Vilenchik, N. (2020). Political polarization on the digital sphere: A cross-platform, over-time analysis of Interactional, positional, and affective polarization on social media. Political Communication, 38(1–2), 98–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2020.1785067
  • Zollo, F., Novak, P. K., Del Vicario, M., Bessi, A., Mozetič, I., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., Quattrociocchi, W., & Preis, T. (2015). Emotional dynamics in the age of misinformation. PLOS One, 10(9), e0138740. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138740