1,768
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Alexithymia, impulsivity and negative mood in relation to internet addiction symptoms in female university students

ORCID Icon, &
Pages 548-556 | Received 10 Jun 2020, Accepted 08 Jun 2021, Published online: 18 Jul 2021

ABSTRACT

Objective

Alexithymia has been implicated as a risk factor for problematic substance use and other excessive behaviours including internet addiction . Impulsiveness has also been identified as a likely predisposing factor for excessive behaviours. However, as impulsivity is often elevated in alexithymia, the degree of independence of these factors in relation to excessive internet use is unclear.

Method

The present study assessed contributions of alexithymia, impulsivity and negative affect to variance in internet addiction symptoms in 116 internet-using female university students. Participants completed the following instruments online: demographics, Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11, Internet Addiction Test and the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21.

Results

Measures were significantly intercorrelated in expected directions. Hierarchical regression indicated that although both alexithymia and impulsivity were highly significant predictors of internet addiction symptoms after controlling for demographic covariates, the contribution of alexithymia became nonsignificant after adding impulsivity to the model. The final model explained 37% of variance in internet addiction symptoms. Multiple mediation modelling indicated that both impulsivity and negative affect fully mediated the association of alexithymia with internet addiction symptoms.

Conclusions

Impulsivity and negative affect may account for the link between alexithymia and internet addiction symptoms in young women at university.

KEY POINTS

What is already known about this topic:

  1. Alexithymia and impulsivity have been linked to a variety of addictive behaviors including internet addiction.

  2. Alexithymia and impulsivity have been reported to independently predict excessive alcohol use in regression models.

  3. Alexithymia may reflect deficient interoception and corresponding poor internal awareness of overconsumption cues in alcohol use.

What this topic adds:

  1. In female university students, alexithymia, impulsivity, and negative affect were significant positive predictors of internet addiction symptoms in a regression model.

  2. Alexithymia was no longer significant after adding impulsivity to the model.

  3. Multiple mediation modelling indicated that impulsivity and negative affect fully mediated the association of alexithymia with internet addiction symptoms.

Alexithymia, a personality trait defined by difficulties identifying and describing feelings and externally oriented thinking (Luminet et al., Citation2018), has been implicated as a risk factor for excessive behaviours including problematic substance use (Cruise & Becerra, Citation2018; Lyvers, Hinton et al., Citation2014; Thorberg et al., Citation2009, Citation2016), compulsive buying (Rose & Segrist, Citation2012), pathological gambling (Marchetti et al., Citation2019; Toneatto et al., Citation2009) and eating disorders (Marsero et al., Citation2011). Alexithymia has also been linked to excessive use of the internet (De Berardis et al., Citation2009; Kandri et al., Citation2014; Lyvers et al., Citation2016; Mahapatra & Sharma, Citation2018), which is often referred to as internet addiction (IA) when such use causes problems for the user. Another personality trait, impulsivity, has been similarly linked to various excessive behaviours (Littlefield & Sher, Citation2010; Lyvers, Hinton et al., Citation2014; MacKillop et al., Citation2011) and is regarded as a manifestation of executive dyscontrol given its associations with neuropsychological and brain imaging indices of prefrontal cortex dysfunction (Lyvers, Citation2000; Lyvers et al., Citation2015; Spinella, Citation2004; Turel et al., Citation2014; Vermeulen et al., Citation2018). Like alexithymia, impulsivity has been linked to IA (De Berardis et al., Citation2009; Cao et al., Citation2007; Dalbudak et al., Citation2013; Mottram & Fleming, Citation2009), with the relationship reportedly paralleling that for pathological gambling (Lee et al., Citation2012; Lumley & Roby, Citation1995).

Alexithymia, a deficit in emotional self-awareness, and impulsivity, the tendency to act rashly without due consideration of consequences, are defined very differently. However, people with higher alexithymia also tend to be more impulsive (Lyvers, Hinton et al., Citation2014; Lyvers, Makin et al., Citation2014; Shishido et al., Citation2013). Findings have thus been mixed regarding the degree to which alexithymia and impulsivity may be regarded as independent risk factors for excessive behaviours. For alcohol use disorder, much evidence suggests that they are largely independent risk factors (see Cruise & Becerra, Citation2018), whereas in other research. the risk associated with alexithymia was attributed to impulsivity (Shishido et al., Citation2013). One recent study (Lyvers, Narayanan et al., Citation2018) compared traits associated with alcohol-related risk to those associated with disordered social media use in young adults, finding that both alexithymia and impulsivity predicted alcohol-related risk, whereas only impulsivity predicted disordered social media use. This suggests that impulsivity may be a more relevant trait than alexithymia in excessive online behaviours, at least in the case of disordered social media use.

IA refers to excessive internet use that leads to negative consequences in life domains such as employment, education and relationships (K.S. Young, Citation2017; K. S. Young, Citation1998). The concept was based on DSM criteria for pathological gambling, currently included under Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). The only internet-related disorder in the DSM-5 (albeit tentatively) is internet gaming disorder. Given the many uses of the internet, the IA concept has been criticized as too heterogenous (Starcevic & Aboujaoude, Citation2017). Nevertheless, the high worldwide prevalence of IA symptoms (Cheng & Li, Citation2014; Durkee et al., Citation2012; Moreno et al., Citation2011) has stimulated research on this apparent phenomenon. A recent study in Australia (Borg & Smith, Citation2018) indicated that of the 93% of a large sample of people contacted by telephone who had internet access, 84% used the internet daily; the heaviest users were young adults, the age group most likely to use the internet excessively (Chakraborty et al., Citation2010). Although IA symptoms have been reported to be more prevalent in males than in females overall (Durkee et al., Citation2012; Wang et al., Citation2017), the gender difference may be narrowing (Dufour et al., Citation2016). Results of a large-scale study in university students indicated that the role of personality traits in IA may be stronger for women than for men (Yen et al., Citation2009). Further, women at university tend to report higher levels of stress and other negative moods than male students do (Rickwood et al., Citation2016), and negative affect has been identified as a proximate factor driving IA (Whang et al., Citation2003). The present study thus assessed trait and mood variables in relation to IA symptoms in a sample of female university students, based on the notion that such variables are especially likely to influence IA symptoms among women attending university given those previous findings.

The present study assessed IA symptoms among internet-using university student women in relation to their levels of impulsivity, alexithymia and negative affect. The goal was to assess whether the reported link of alexithymia to IA symptoms might be explained by impulsiveness and/or negative affect, both of which are commonly associated with alexithymia, in female university students. This hypothesis was tested by regression and multiple mediation modelling.

Method

Participants

Recruitment was from two universities in southeast Queensland, Australia, via campus-wide email at one university and an online research participation notice board at the other, yielding an initial sample of 132. Those recruited from one university were offered the incentive of entry into a random draw for one of two 50 AUD gift vouchers; those recruited from the other were offered an undergraduate psychology credit point. To meet inclusion criteria, participants had to be female and report using the internet at least twice a week for the past 2 years, no use of illicit drugs more than once per month on average, high English proficiency, no current psychiatric medication and no previous brain injury. After removal of multivariate outliers and those who did not meet inclusion criteria, the final sample consisted of 116 female undergraduates aged 18–30 years (M = 21.39, SD = 3.52).

Measures

Demographic questionnaire

Information was requested on age, gender, student status, years of education, use of alcohol and illicit drugs, previous brain injury and English proficiency.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20)

The TAS-20 (Bagby, Parker et al., Citation1994; Bagby, Taylor et al., Citation1994) is the most widely used self-report index of alexithymia. The 20 items encompass three facets of alexithymia, difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feelings (DDF) and externally oriented thinking (EOT). Items are rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree); total scores range from 20 to 100, with higher scores indicating higher alexithymia. Psychometric studies have generally supported the reliability and validity of the TAS-20 (Bagby et al., Citation2007; Sekely et al., Citation2018; Thorberg et al., Citation2010), although alternative models of alexithymia have been suggested (Preece et al., Citation2017). Gignac et al. (Citation2007) supported a five-factor model of the TAS-20; however, their interpretation was subsequently disputed (Bagby et al., Citation2007). Meganck et al. (Citation2008) found support for the three-factor model, but low internal consistency was noted for the externally oriented thinking factor. On the other hand, use of the alpha index of internal consistency is questionable when the number of items is small, as is the case for the TAS-20 subscales (Tavakol & Dennick, Citation2011). Convergent validity of the TAS-20 has been indicated by expected correlations with other indices of alexithymia such as the Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (Zech et al., Citation1999) and the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia (Bagby et al., Citation2006). The total scale score was utilized in this study as recommended by the authors of the original measure (see Sekely et al., Citation2018). The alpha index of internal consistency for the total TAS-20 was acceptable in the present sample (α = .83).

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 (Patton et al., Citation1995) is a widely used 30-item self-report instrument designed to measure trait impulsivity in adults. Questions encompass attentional, motor and non-planning impulsiveness. Items are rated on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost always/always). Total scores range from 30 to 120, with higher scores indicating higher impulsivity. The BIS-11 has been reported to have good psychometric properties (Spinella, Citation2004, Citation2007), with alpha reliability coefficients of .79 – .83 and test–retest stability over 1 year of .60 (Luengo et al., Citation1991). Convergent validity was established with neuropsychological measures that have demonstrated sensitivity to prefrontal cortex dysfunction (Lyvers et al., Citation2015; Spinella, Citation2004). Internal consistency was acceptable at α = .80 in the current sample.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21)

The DASS-21 (Lovibond & Lovibond, Citation1995) is a 21-item self-report instrument assessing negative moods experienced over the previous week. There are three subscales of seven items each: depression, anxiety and stress. Items are scored on a four-point Likert scale from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the time). Total scores can range from 0 to 63, with higher scores indicating more negative mood symptoms experienced during the past week. The DASS-21 has been reported to have excellent psychometric properties (Brown et al., Citation1997) and showed correlations of .72-.85 with other measures of depression and anxiety (Antony et al., Citation1998). The total score was used in the present study as an overall index of negative affect. Internal consistency of the total DASS-21 was excellent at α = .92 in the present sample.

Internet Addiction Test (IAT)

The IAT (K.S. Young, Citation2017; Citation1998) is a 20-item self-report index of IA symptoms based on DSM criteria for pathological gambling. Items consist of statements rated on the degree to which they describe the respondent via a five-point Likert scale from 0 (does not apply) to 5 (always). The total score is the sum of ratings. Items include questions such as “How often do you find that you stay online longer than you intended?” Possible scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating more excessive or problematic internet use. Validity and reliability have been consistently supported across languages and cultures (K.S. Young, Citation2017). Internal consistency was excellent at α = .89 in the present sample.

Procedure

After obtaining approval from the ethics committees of both universities, recruitment was initiated for research on personality and internet use. Respondent anonymity, voluntary nature of participation, time required and incentives were described. Those interested accessed a link to the online questionnaire battery via Qualtrics, a web-based survey hosting company. After completing the demographics questionnaire, instruments were presented in a uniquely randomized order per participant. Upon completion, a final screen reminded participants to email a screenshot to the researchers to receive their incentive. A random draw determined the two participants who received a 50 USD electronic gift voucher.

Results

The DASS-21 index of negative affect was positively skewed, as expected for a nonclinical sample; this was corrected by square root transformation. Descriptive statistics and correlations for the measures are shown in . Total scores on BIS-11 and TAS-20 are shown in the table, as all three BIS-11 subscales showed similar significant correlations with IAT scores (r = .33 – .42), as did all three subscales of the TAS-20 (r = .20 – .31). As expected, scores on the IAT were significantly positively correlated with total TAS-20 alexithymia and BIS-11 impulsivity scores as well as with the transformed DASS-21 scores. Alexithymia, impulsivity and negative mood were significantly positively correlated with each other. IAT scores were significantly negatively correlated with age and education; hence, these demographic variables were included as covariates in the regression and mediation model.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations and intercorrelations of study variables (N = 116)

Regression on internet addiction test scores

Hierarchical multiple regression was conducted on IAT scores. At step 1, the covariates age and education were entered, accounting for a significant 9% of variance, F(2, 113) = 5.59, p = .005. Age was the only significant (and negative) predictor. Alexithymia was entered at step 2 and accounted for a further 9.1% of variance, which was significant, ΔF(1, 112) = 12.38, p = .001; alexithymia was the only significant (and positive) predictor at this step. Impulsivity was entered at step 3 and explained a significant 12.6% of additional variance as a positive predictor, ΔF(1, 111) = 20.27, p < .0001, with impulsivity and age the only significant predictors. Alexithymia was no longer significant; Sobel Test indicated full mediation by impulsivity, z = 3.56, p = .0004. The transformed DASS-21 index of negative mood was entered at the last step based on evidence that negative mood is a proximate driver of excessive internet use (Whang et al., Citation2003); DASS-21 accounted for a significant 6.1% of additional variance as a positive predictor, ΔF(1, 110) = 10.53, p = .002. Impulsivity remained the strongest predictor in the final model, followed by negative mood and age. The final model accounted for 37% of variance in IAT scores, F(5, 110) = 12.79, p < .0001. shows the regression statistics.

Table 2. Hierarchical multiple regression on internet addiction test scores (N = 116)

As discussed in a recent review by Luminet et al. (Citation2021), a number of alexithymia studies have indicated differential behavioural, cognitive, or psychopathological correlates of the EOT component versus the DIF and/or DDF components. Although not a planned analysis, in the present sample, regression results were similar when TAS-20 subscales were used instead of the total score, with the exception of DDF – the shortest subscale with only five items. Both DIF and EOT subscales were significant predictors until impulsivity was added to the model, as was the case for the total TAS-20.

Multiple mediation analysis

Although the hierarchical regression model indicated that the variance in IAT scores explained by TAS-20 alexithymia scores was fully mediated by BIS-11 impulsivity scores before the DASS-21 index of negative affect was entered at the last step, the possibility of dual mediation by both impulsivity and negative affect was tested by multiple mediation modelling via JASP 0.14.1. Mediation was tested using bootstrapping with 1000 bias-corrected replications, controlling for age and education as covariates. As can be seen in , the 95% confidence interval for the indirect effects of alexithymia through impulsivity and negative affect did not include zero, whereas the 95% confidence interval for the direct effect did include zero. This result indicated full mediation; that is, the association between alexithymia and excessive internet use was fully accounted for by impulsivity and negative affect. The model is depicted in showing significant paths.

Table 3. Mediation of the relationship of TAS-20 alexithymia scores with IAT internet addiction scores by both BIS-11 impulsivity scores and DASS-21 negative affect, controlling for age and education

Figure 1. Mediation model showing significant paths after controlling for age and education. The direct effect without mediators is shown in parentheses. ***p < .001

Figure 1. Mediation model showing significant paths after controlling for age and education. The direct effect without mediators is shown in parentheses. ***p < .001

Given the evidence as cited in Luminet et al. (Citation2021) noted earlier, although not a planned analysis another mediation analysis was run with the TAS-20 subscales as predictors instead of the total score and total BIS-11 score as mediator. The results indicated that the relationships of DIF and EOT with excessive internet use were fully mediated by impulsivity, whereas there was neither a direct nor indirect effect of DDF. A second exploratory mediation analysis indicated that the BIS-11 subscales of attentional and motor impulsiveness, but not planning impulsiveness, showed mediation of the relationship between total TAS-20 and excessive internet use.

Discussion

Indices of alexithymia, impulsivity and negative affect were significantly positively correlated with excessive internet use, as indexed by IAT scores, as well as with each other. Alexithymia, impulsivity and negative affect have all been implicated as risk factors for excessive or problematic substance use (Cruise & Becerra, Citation2018; Littlefield & Sher, Citation2010; Lyvers, Hinton et al., Citation2014; Thorberg et al., Citation2009) as well as IA (De Berardis et al., Citation2009; Kandri et al., Citation2014; Lyvers et al., Citation2016; Mahapatra & Sharma, Citation2018) in previous work. However, in the present study, the contribution of alexithymia to variance in IA symptoms became nonsignificant after adding impulsivity to the regression model, suggesting that the contribution of alexithymia reflected high impulsiveness associated with alexithymia. Multiple mediation modelling further indicated that the relationship between alexithymia and internet addiction symptoms was fully mediated by both impulsivity and negative affect. As noted earlier, previous work has been somewhat mixed regarding the degree of independence of alexithymia from other risk factors such as impulsivity or depression in relation to excessive substance use, although much evidence does appear to support alexithymia as an independent risk factor for problematic drinking (Cruise & Becerra, Citation2018). Present findings however suggest that both impulsivity and negative affect may account for the relationship of alexithymia to excessive internet use as indexed by the IAT, presumably reflecting an alexithymic preference for impulsive action over introspection in response to the negative affect often associated with alexithymia and related difficulties with emotional self-regulation.

High impulsivity is considered a behavioural manifestation of deficient executive self-regulation processes mediated by prefrontal cortex (Lyvers, Citation2000; Lyvers et al., Citation2012; Qiu et al., Citation2013; Turel et al., Citation2014). Alexithymia is also linked to impaired self-regulation, as evidenced by positive relationships of alexithymia indices such as TAS-20 with neuropsychological indices of executive function deficits associated with frontal lobe injury (Lyvers, Makin et al., Citation2014; Vermeulen et al., Citation2018), as well as brain imaging research showing deficient frontal activation in alexithymia (Chester et al., Citation2015). Both impulsivity and alexithymia appear to reflect dysfunctionality of brain systems mediating executive control, including self-regulation of negative affect, hence their importance as potential markers of risk for excessive behaviours. However, as emphasised earlier, impulsivity and alexithymia are defined very differently and may contribute to excessive behaviours in different ways. For example, in excessive or problematic drinking, deficient impulse control may drive ongoing alcohol use despite adverse consequences (Dawe & Loxton, Citation2004), whereas the role of alexithymia has been attributed to a deficiency of interoception (Brewer et al., Citation2016) that impedes recognition of internal cues of overconsumption. Independent contributions of both traits to excessive drinking thus seem plausible, as they may promote the disordered behaviour in different ways. By contrast, interoceptive cues of overconsumption are not relevant to self-regulation of internet use. This difference might explain why, in regression models, impulsivity and alexithymia are often found to be independent predictors of excessive drinking (Cruise & Becerra, Citation2018), whereas only impulsivity showed such a role in one form of excessive internet use, social media addiction, in a recent study (Lyvers, Narayanan et al., Citation2018).

Although the present study was undertaken in a non-clinical sample of young women at university, the key finding that both impulsivity and negative affect mediated the relationship of alexithymia with symptoms of internet addiction may have potential implications for clinical practice. If negative affect and impulsivity are underlying mechanisms of how alexithymia is related to internet addiction (and perhaps some other excessive behaviours), then targeting negative affect and impulsivity would be essential. One approach might be to utilise cognitive behavioural therapy to address dysfunctional thought patterns that induce or maintain negative mood states. By identifying dichotomous thinking patterns and applying cognitive restructuring to address and replace such cognitions, a client may over time be able to upregulate depressive moods and downregulate stress and anxiety, reducing dependence on the maladaptive coping strategy of engaging in an excessive behaviour such as IA. Training in relaxation exercises as well as promoting distress tolerance may help alexithymic clients learn to “sit with” as well as cope with negative feelings and moods, instead of trying to escape distress via compulsive behaviour. The tendency to impulsive action may be addressed by learning to do the opposite of what impulsiveness is and focus on “slowing down” to be more open to experiences and get in touch with emotional feelings and somatic sensations. This in turn may assist in gaining access to emotions and being able to better describe, identify and self-regulate feelings, reducing the reliance on excessive internet use or other excessive behaviour for affect regulation.

The present study had several limitations. Although potential influences of age and education were controlled as covariates in the regression model, the sample consisted of female university students given evidence of potentially stronger influences of trait and mood factors on IA symptoms among females at university (Yen et al., Citation2009); thus, the findings cannot be generalised to the wider population of young adults including nonstudents and males. Further, although the IAT assesses symptoms of excessive internet use on a continuum, results in a nonclinical sample may not apply to clinical cases of IA. On the other hand, 19% of the present sample had IAT scores indicative of moderate to severe IA according to suggested cut-off scores (K.S. Young, Citation2017; K. S. Young, Citation1998). This seems high based on overall population estimates (Durkee et al., Citation2012); however, for youth and university students, estimates of IA prevalence have ranged as high as 26% (Moreno et al., Citation2011). Finally, the present findings were cross-sectional and therefore cannot be taken to reflect causal relationships among variables. Although longitudinal research has indicated that alexithymia and impulsivity are relatively stable traits that begin to manifest early in life and predict onset of, and/or relapse to, problematic alcohol use (Littlefield & Sher, Citation2010; Lyvers, Jones et al., Citation2018; Thorberg et al., Citation2009, Citation2016), such longitudinal relationships have not been established for problematic internet use. The role of negative affect is even less clear, as it could promote excessive internet use or represent an outcome of problems related to such use. A longitudinal approach is thus recommended for future research on these issues.

Disclosure Statement

All authors hereby declare that no competing financial interests exist concerning the research reported in this manuscript.

References

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.