753
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Valuing Children: Parents’ Perceptions, Spending Priorities and Children’s Capabilities

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 932-955 | Received 23 May 2023, Accepted 18 Jan 2024, Published online: 25 Feb 2024

Abstract

This paper provides a composite analysis of children’s academic development grounded on the capability approach. The study utilises a panel dataset comprising 8,422 Chinese children and adolescents aged 6 to 16, observed between 2012 and 2018. It introduces a series of innovative indicators, including a parent advantage index to capture how parents influence their children and a ranking indicator for spending priorities to reify the value of children’s education that families have reasoned. To address unobserved heterogeneity, we adopted fixed-effects models, multilevel modelling, and heteroskedasticity-based instrumental variables. Our primary results show that a 1% increase in the parent advantage index yields an increase of 13.85% to 21.31% in children’s academic development, and the biggest leap in prioritising education-relevant spending increases the child outcomes by 2.88% to 6.57%. By highlighting the influence of parents’ beings and doings, particularly the value they assign to education, this research contributes to the existing literature on child development, which often focuses predominantly on material dimensions. In sum, it expands the frontiers of the capability approach and related research on parental practices. It offers novel insights into how policies can be reinforced to equalise educational opportunities and to boost human capital.

JEL CLASSIFICATIONS:

1. Introduction

Debates on children’s capabilities centre on the essence of human development and flourishing lives of humans. Such debates often focus on the instrumental role of education systems (Hanushek, Rivkin, & Schiman, Citation2016; Hanushek & Woessmann, Citation2008; World Bank Group, Citation2018), early environments (Cunha & Heckman, Citation2007; Heckman, Citation2008), parental practices (D. Liu, Chen, & Brown, Citation2020; Vasilyeva, Laski, Veraksa, Weber, & Bukhalenkova, Citation2018), children’s health (Goldhagen et al., Citation2020; Gunnar et al., Citation2020), and children’s nutrition (Black, Trude, & Lutter, Citation2020; Shrestha, Weissman, Thapa, Adhikari, & Perry, Citation2021), among others, in developing their capabilities. Regardless of how big the family is or how constituted, families are predominantly responsible for making necessary arrangements to develop their children’s capabilities (Nussbaum, Citation2000). Particular types of distinctive good, such as a ‘relationship good’, only become possible because of the existence of families (Swift & Brighouse, Citation2014). In addition, families are important for the investments they make in their children (Becker, Citation1974; Becker & Tomes, Citation1976; Nussbaum, Citation2000, Citation2011; Sen, Citation1997, Citation2009, Citation2017).

Within this context, children’s development depends on their capabilities that parents have ‘reason to value’ (Sen, Citation1999), that is, the valuable capabilities that parents wish to foster in their children. Despite this, parents’ priorities have rarely been examined, and only indirectly estimated by children’s academic studies, or simply ignored (Biggeri & Mehrotra, Citation2011). Consequently, these types of studies prevalently assume that parents in general attach a similar level of importance to their children’s development. They ignore the fact that families’ prioritisation of spending in relation to their children could reflect a concerted judgement about the value of education and their children’s future, which, in turn, affects their prioritisation.

This approach applies to some recent studies examining multidimensional poverty (Zhang, Ma, & Wang, Citation2021) and child development in China (Chen, Deng, Zhang, Wang, & Liu, Citation2021; Cui, Liu, & Zhao, Citation2019; Sylvia et al., Citation2022) that focuses on the impact of parenting interventions. However, if family resources are accorded importance based on the use to which they are put, and if the use depends on how different capabilities are valued, it is essential to assess how families prioritise their resources. Given this background, the main objective of this paper is to examine how the academic performance of Chinese children and adolescents depends on household spending priorities vis-à-vis parental practices and other factors. To address these issues, several new indicators have been developed, such as the ‘parent advantage index’ (PAI, modelled on the Human Development Index) and the ‘spending priority ranking’ (SPR, based on a ranking of ten spending category groups assessed as a proportion of a family’s household expenditure). Further, children’s academic development is measured through an assessment of their learning outcomes and learning processes.

The nationally-representative China Family Panel Studies (CFPS) surveys provide a rich database of useful variables, such as school quality, parental reactions to children’s unsatisfactory test scores, and children’s study habits and discipline (SHD). The sample used for this study comprises 8,422 Chinese children and adolescents surveyed during 2012 to 2018. Our study aims to contribute to the literature on children’s development by assessing the place of families in children’s academic performance (Heckman & Mosso, Citation2014), the relevance of traditional Chinese culture that cultivates positive attitudes towards learning (Hsu & Wu, Citation2015), and the significance of parental prioritisation of categories of spending, as an expression of attitudes and values (Nussbaum, Citation2000; Sen, Citation1997, Citation2017). The study does not simply consider the impact of family resources or parental practices on their children’s development, but extends to how families value education, particularly through spending prioritisation.

Overall, our results confirm the effects of parental advantages and a higher spending priority on educational and cultural activities, among others, on children’s academic development. The results proved robust on a series of alternative estimations, including Lewbel (Citation2012) heteroskedasticity-based instrumental variables.

The remainder of this paper is divided into five parts. The first part provides a framework evoked by the capability approach to evaluate children’s capabilities, emphasising the role of informational pluralism and the relevance of comprehensive outcomes in evaluating children’s advantages. The second part presents the data used, including descriptive statistics. The third part illustrates estimation strategies used in this paper. The fourth part discusses the results. The paper concludes by indicating how policy can enhance children’s development.

2. Theoretical framework: children and the capability approach

There are many different approaches to assessing children’s human development, the most-frequently used being the capability approach (Nussbaum, Citation2011; Sen, Citation2017; Yousefzadeh, Biggeri, Arciprete, & Haisma, Citation2019). There are two distinct versions in which this approach can be applied. In its narrower version, it focuses on the use of capability as its main informational space. In its broader version, other features of the approach are relevant (Anand et al., Citation2009; Comim, Citation2021; Comim, Qizilbash, & Alkire, Citation2008), such as its emphasis on informational pluralism, multidimensionality, its use of comprehensive outcomes (where the evaluation of outcomes depends also on how processes unfold), and role of individuals’ agency. In this paper, we adopt the broader version of the approach given that its main contribution is not about providing new measures of capabilities but on employing the general features of the approach to produce more consistent accounts of children’s human development (Burchardt & Vizard, Citation2011; Byskov, Citation2018; Chiappero, Scervini, & Salardi, Citation2018). Within this broader view, its first feature, pluralism, can be seen as an argument for evaluations based on informationally-rich accounts of state of affairs. It is not simply a matter of elaborating multidimensional indicators, but is also concerned with using different informational spaces to construct these indicators. The most widely-used informational spaces in the capability literature are resources, subjective well-being, rights and capabilities (Comim, Citation2021; Sen, Citation1980, Citation2017). Sen’s argument for ‘the impossibility of a Paretian liberal’ was an important milestone in this debate, providing as it does a compelling formulation of rights in the literature of welfare economics (Suzumura, Citation2011).

The pluralist nature of the capability approach implies a valuational exercise that demands, firstly, a consideration of a multiplicity of dimensions and variables in the relevant spaces (that are more often than not heterogeneous), secondly, a solution to the complex issue of the varying importance of different functioning and capabilities and how they are evaluated, and thirdly, an engagement with the ‘agency aspect’ that the approach highlights. In fact, the elaboration of informationally-rich accounts is not the only important element in the capability approach, because these spaces should automatically be part of accounts that attempt to explain how autonomous actions reflect, in different degrees, a person’s freedom to live in a way that they would value (Sen, Citation1999).

Indeed, a second key feature of the approach is how it incorporates references to autonomous actions and reasoned scrutiny as a way of capturing people’s agency (Sen, Citation2009). This means that information should incorporate individual and collective reflective evaluations about what people have reasons to value. In theory, reasoned scrutiny represents a strong critique against assessments based on mechanical judgements, while, in practice, operationalising the scrutiny is often challenging once the exercises of prioritisation attached to the selection of key capabilities are few.

A third key feature of the capability approach, particularly in Sen’s formulation, is a conceptual distinction between comprehensive outcomes (those that include the processes of choice) and culmination outcomes (those that only display the final results of the act of choice). This is because the act of choice also has process significance within which results should be characterised, not only for the final results, but also for all those features of the processes that final results involve (Sen, Citation2002). Thus, different results obtained from different processes cannot receive the same evaluation. Because the capability approach values individual autonomy, as discussed below, it is not sufficient to be concerned only with what an individual receives should they choose, but that they actually get to choose what they receive themselves. Thus, whenever we examine children’s outcomes, we are concerned not only with the marks from their exams, but also with the learning processes involved in achieving those results.

The use of the capability approach for assessing children’s capabilities invites us to look at childhood from a different perspective, integrating key aspects of:

  1. the role of families in promoting human development, focusing on how children are raised,

  2. the path and time-dependent dimension of practices and policies that foster children’s development,

  3. the recognition of children’s agency and autonomy, and

  4. the role of emotions during childhood.

Families are important in promoting children’s capabilities, as networks of love and care (Nussbaum, Citation2006). It is within families that children grow up to become fully-functioning human beings and where they learn to become moral agents, particularly in early childhood. Parental practices can often be categorised as distinct parenting styles that portray certain behavioural and attitudinal patterns towards children. The most influential styles are:

  • authoritative: evident when parents show understanding, open communication, respect and emotional support and considered the most effective and loving parenting style,

  • authoritarian: evident when parents rigorously assess their children’s behaviour, impose rigid norms and punishment without sympathy for children’s difficulties, typically exemplified by an absence of emotional support,

  • permissive: evident when parents acquiesce in actions and behaviours as their children please, still probably being loving and sympathetic yet not responding with discipline and control. This is often the case with absent parents who try to compensate for their absence by indulging their children, and

  • negligent: evident when parents do not show much interest in their children’s development. Their involvement is minimum, with parents spending little time with their children and offering little or no level of support and control.

Different parenting styles entail different prioritisation strategies that parents use to manage their children’s human development. A variety of circumstances can embody the prioritisation, namely, by the amount parents spend on their children’s education, the time they dedicate to play with their children, whether they help with homework or not, and whether parents support their children emotionally. While parenting styles might not be empirically evident in the clear-cut descriptions above, this categorisation is useful in calling attention to the diversity of processes of raising children.

A common mistake in assessing children’s development is viewing childhood as a single discrete period in one’s life, without considering different stages of child development as being unevenly affected by biological and neurological factors (Borghans, Meijers, & ter Weel, Citation2008; Cunha & Heckman, Citation2007). Indeed, children’s receptiveness to language learning is higher by 3 years of age, their IQ scores are often stable by the age of 10, and emotions and self-regulation from the malleability of the pre-frontal cortex lasts until the end of adolescence (Rose & Fischer, Citation2011; Heckman, Citation2008). Correspondingly, investment in early childhood education should be distinguished from that in late childhood, and the impact of parental investment on children’s skills and human development also depend on sensitive (more effective) and critical (unique) periods. Time is of the essence in the matter of being a child, given how their development is uniquely sensitive to different flows of time and timing of particular interventions. Time also matters from another angle – children need time with their parents, time for playing, time for being creative, and time in which they are protected and can flourish.

The use of the capability approach also encourages consideration of how children develop their own capabilities. As much as children require some basic functionings, such as compulsory education, before they can fully exercise their autonomy (Nussbaum, Citation2011), it is important to acknowledge that children have a certain capacity for self-determination that is exercised from a very early age (Ballet, Biggeri, & Comim, Citation2011; Saito, Citation2003). Children will have fully-fledged autonomy when they become adults, with conceptions of what is right and what is good developed with faculties enabled during childhood (Rawls, Citation1971). Whereas a paternalistic view sees children as vulnerable and dependent on their parents, a capability view sees children as an evolving project of human self-determination. The debate is not that children are unable to make choices, but that they may not be able to evaluate and revise the choices they make. Evidence suggests that children start to learn to be independent of their parents from an early age (Lansdown, Citation2004). No one claims that children can display the same level of self-determination as do adults. The flaw is in denying to children a capacity for self-determination that is progressively evolving. In fact, children can persuade adults of what they want, and they can negotiate and renegotiate boundaries imposed by adults (Alderson, Citation2001; Anich, Biggeri, Libanora, & Mariani, Citation2011; Punch, Citation2002). This means that a child’s human development should not be seen merely as a result of his or her parents’ priorities, but rather as an interactive process between parents and the expression of the child’s own agency (Bellanca, Biggeri, & Marchetta, Citation2011).

Finally, we refer to the role of emotions in shaping children’s ethical reasoning (Nussbaum, Citation2006, Citation2011). While emotions help to explain children’s motivation for acting and their endurance (Biggeri, Ballet, & Comim, Citation2011a), they also have an important cognitive role. As Nussbaum (Citation2011) and Cunha and Heckman (Citation2007) demonstrate, emotions can be decisive for the formation of children’s deliberative beliefs, enabling them to perceive critical features in a situation. An example is useful here. A father singing nursery songs to his baby daughter fosters the baby’s moral life and, as such, can be understood as a key practice to be respected and supported in the promotion of the child’s future capabilities. The human sense of value is built upon such interactions within which emotional cognition plays an important role.

Assessing children’s human development through a capability lens means that we should look at it from a multidimensional and pluralist perspective, analysing how families define their priorities about what they have reason to value related to their children’s development. Further, it means going beyond the concept of children as beings without emotions or will. Understanding the formation of children’s capabilities, therefore, entails seeing them as quintessentially dynamic and time-dependent. Of course, it is difficult to emphasise all these different elements in a single analytical discussion. For this reason, we highlight here the links between parental practices and spending priorities, and the impact they might have on children’s cognitive development.

3. Data description and variable definitions

3.1. Data source

We based our empirical analysis of children’s capabilities on the China Family Panel Studies (CFPS), a nationally-representative survey launched in 2010 by the Institute of Social Science Survey, Peking University. The CFPS is modelled on the Panel Study of Income Dynamics of the U.S. to collect data biennially at the individual, household, and community level from 25 provinces, municipalities, and autonomous regions, representing 95% of China’s population. Information on children and adolescents within surveyed households was separately collected.Footnote1 The attrition rate is around 25% biennially. In this study, 4 out of 5 waves were merged to assemble a panel covering data from 2012 to 2018, with child-, adult- and household-level data matched year-to-year. The 2010 survey data was not included because variables provided in the later waves were absent in this initial version. Therefore, our panel includes 8,422 children and adolescents aged 6 to 16 who attended school.

3.2. Child academic development index

Children’s capabilities comprise a rich array of cognitive and socio-emotional dimensions. If we were to follow Nussbaum’s (Citation2018) list of central capabilities, to construct a comprehensive index, we would need indicators of children’s senses, imagination and thought, emotions, practical reasoning, and sense of affiliation or even of play, including the ability to laugh. Unfortunately, data for this kind of evaluation are not normally found empirically. For this reason, we adopted a modest approach, concentrating on a core aspect of children’s human development, namely, their academic development. However, our indicator goes beyond just test scores in literacy and numeracy to include children’s study habits and other non-cognitive elements.

It is important to note that, in the Chinese education system, test scores are decisive information for children to move forward in the education system and the main mechanism for entry into prestigious schools. Consequently, the Gaokao (the national college entrance examination in China) is deemed a major turning point in the life course that determines a person’s career opportunities, earning potential, and even marriage prospects. Studying for the three-day Gaokao can be likened to a marathon in which a variety of cognitive, non-cognitive, and environmental factors connect to shape the outcome. Following Amartya Sen’s distinction between culmination and comprehensive outcomes (Sen, Citation2002), we focus on children’s academic development by structuring it into two parts: one examining learning outcomes (corresponding to culmination outcomes) and the other taking into account learning processes (to reflect comprehensive outcomes).Footnote2

The CFPS provides two ordinal variables scored by parents, describing children’s academic achievement in Mathematics and Chinese based on their performance in the previous semester. We calculated the average point of the two subjects and normalised the result. Likewise, learning processes were measured as the normalised average of seven questions evaluated by parents regarding how good their child’s study habits and discipline (SHD) were.Footnote3 Questions are summarised in Appendix . The children’s academic development index was formulated as follows:Footnote4 (1) Score=Mathematics+Chinese2SHD=QA1+QA2++QA77Norm_Score=ScoreMin(Score)Max(Score)Min(Score)Norm_SHD=SHDMin(SHD)Max(SHD)Min(SHD)(1) (2) CADI=Norm_Score+Norm_SHD2(2)

It is important to note that parents’ understanding of their children’s capabilities might be more relevant than the capabilities per se in this case, given that test scores are always limited in what they test, while parents can have a more comprehensive knowledge of their children’s skills informed by sequences of tests and other indicators that naturally enter into the formation of their views. They are also key observers of their children’s study habits that incorporate a wide range of non-cognitive elements. Moreover, both methods and difficulties of tests varying substantially across schools and regions could also introduce considerable noises to using objective scores as the evaluative information of student achievements.

3.3. Parent advantage index

Socio-cultural factors, particularly family-related, influence the formation and development of children’s mindsets and behaviours. Thus, we created three variables to account for parents’ beings and doings. Among them, the parent advantage index is a unique proxy for functionings. As aforementioned, the PAI is based on the Human Development Index using proxies for the HDI dimensions, such as self-evaluated health status, years of schooling and the natural logarithm of household net income per capita.Footnote5 We adopted household net income per capita to account for intergenerational financial transfers, which commonly occur in Chinese families, that may be driven by the traditional culture of filial piety (see e.g. Sun, Citation2004; H. Zhu, Citation2016).Footnote6 Furthermore, previous studies pointed out that material (housing or financial) support and living arrangements were often intertwined (see e.g. Li & Wu, Citation2019; Yi, Deng, Fan, & Ong, Citation2018; Yin, Citation2010). In this sense, the benefits or burdens placed on adults by their elderly parents need to be considered when assessing their economic advantages.Footnote7

The PAI follows the pluralist approach advocated by Sen and Nussbaum once it combines a subjective variable (self-evaluated health status) with a variable of functionings (years of schooling) and another grounded in resources (household income). It does not differ significantly from the traditional HDI in conception and, as such, cannot be interpreted as an index of capabilities, although it follows a key characteristic of the capability approach. The combination of subjective and objective variables is justified by Sen (Citation2008) and Sen (Citation2009). The problem, as he explains, is not the use of subjective information per se in valuational exercises, but the generalisation of subjective information to analyse all aspects of a problem. This combined use of subjective and objective information can also be found in Burchardt & Vizard, (Citation2011) and Comim et al. (Citation2008).

3.4. Spending priority ranking

Several methods are used in the capability literature to select relevant capability information (Burchardt & Vizard, Citation2011; Byskov, Citation2018). However, these methods often focus on how researchers can identify basic or key capabilities from statistical data without directly tackling the reasoned scrutiny aspect of capabilities, where individuals’ priorities are represented by particular indicators. As much as this is not a trivial task, being able to signal how individuals translate their ‘reasons to value’ into specific priorities is essential under this framework. For this reason, we have built an indicator that attempts to represent individuals’ priorities through their budget allocation choices. We classified 26 expenditure items into ten groups, according to Xie et al. (Citation2017), and calculated spending on items within each group. By calculating the ratio of spending in each group to total expenditure, we then ranked the groups, associating higher percentages of spending with higher levels of priority.Footnote8 In other words, the number ‘10’ represents the highest priority here. Previously, Ratigan (Citation2017) compared social policy priorities of Chinese provinces in a fashion similar to ours.

As shown in , nearly 47% of households gave the highest priority to the ‘diet-relevant’ spending group, while the largest share of spending on the transport-relevant group was 21%, ranked eighth. Similarly, for the group of ‘rent and utilities’, a sixth-level priority given by around 17% of households was the most common, and the prevailing rank seen in the ‘necessity-relevant’ group was ranked fifth. Further, medication, healthcare, and sports items were prioritised fourth by approximately 20% of the families. In contrast, the ‘education-relevant’ group was more significant, ranked ninth and tenth by 30% of the families in total. This is the key variable of interest in our analysis since it reflects how households value investing in their children. In addition, spending on clothing-relevant items was most often ranked sixth and fifth. The remainder of the spending groups had similar distributions, with the donation-, insurance- and all-other-relevant consumption ranked third by 46%, 42%, and 40% of the households, respectively. All these variables are further summarised in Appendix .

Figure 1. Histograms of the SPR indicators

Note: By design, larger numbers here reflect higher priorities.

Source: Authors’ elaboration using CFPS data.

Figure 1. Histograms of the SPR indicatorsNote: By design, larger numbers here reflect higher priorities.Source: Authors’ elaboration using CFPS data.

We also created a group of supplementary SPR indicators in the same way by adding housing mortgage to the utility-relevant group and re-ranked spending groups accordingly. Corresponding statistics are reported in Supplementary Materials Table SA1. However, only 73% observations are retained because the CFPS didn’t survey households’ housing mortgage in 2012. All other covariates are discussed in Appendix A.

4. Econometric modelling and techniques

We undertook a threefold empirical analysis. To alleviate potential endogeneity, we firstly adopted a fixed-effects model. Since some informative variables are time-invariant and our core predictors are slowly-varying household-level variables,Footnote9 a within individual or household estimator is not applicable. Yet all individuals were nested within their households, we thus checked the robustness of the results by considering multilevel modelling, which helped eliminate household-level unobserved heterogeneity. Given that our data contain 6,405 households and 8,422 children, variations estimated in this way were within-child for the majority of the observations, acting as an ideal alternative to individual fixed effects. Finally, we double-checked our analysis with Lewbel (Citation2012) heteroskedasticity-based instruments. This approach is widely used when external instruments are not available (e.g. Chung, Zhang, & Partridge, Citation2020; Z. Liu & Yu, Citation2020). Due to space limitations, further discussion on the hierarchical linear and 2SLS models is provided in Supplementary Material C.

4.1. Fixed effects models

The main specification is as follows:Footnote10 (3) CADIit=α+β1PAIit+β2SPRit+β3Xit+γj+γB+γN+γt+εit(3) where CADIit is the child academic development index of a child i living in province j identified in wave t. PAIit is the average parent advantage index of child i’s parents; SPRit are spending priorities of child i’s household;Footnote11 Xit is a row vector of individual and household characteristics. γj are province fixed effects; γB are child i’s birth year fixed effects; γN are the number of children fixed effects; γt are the survey wave fixed effects. εit is an idiosyncratic error term.

The survey wave fixed-effects allow us to avoid systematic differences imposed by time across the four waves used in this study. The province fixed-effects help eliminate unobserved macro factors existing at the provincial level, such as differences in educational standards and Gaokao policies. Further, the birth year fixed-effects reduce two concerns. Firstly, new education-relevant policies could be issued every few years, altering parents’ attitudes and children’s learning experiences. Secondly, as shown in , one component of the CADI, test scores, tends to decrease for children who become older. Additionally, as China had widely implemented the One-Child Policy for over two decades until late 2015, some invisible but ingrained differences in the families, such as a strong son preference, a lack of contraception or abortion, and religious factors, would be expected between one-child and multiple-child families. Even within multiple-child families, the number of children would most likely have an essential impact on household spending behaviours and the value placed on each expenditure item.

Figure 2. Test scores and study habits and discipline against child age

Source: Authors’ calculation using CFPS data.

Figure 2. Test scores and study habits and discipline against child ageSource: Authors’ calculation using CFPS data.

Beyond the priority ranking, it is of interest to know if conversion factors in our model form differential effects on child outcomes. For instance, for children who attended a key school, it is likely that educational resources were converted into learning outcomes more efficiently than for their ordinary-school counterparts, ceteris paribus. Following Comim, Fennell, and Anand (Citation2018),Footnote12 we estimate conversion rates by including interaction effects between four dummies and the SPR indicators.Footnote13

Therefore, we added the interaction terms, one by one, to the model, as follows: (4) CADIit=α+β1PAIit+β2SPRit+β3Xit+β4(SPRit*Dit)+β5Dit+γj+γB+γN+γt+εit(4) where Dit is the dummy variable accounting for the conversion factor of interest. Other variables remain the same, as above.

5. Empirical analysis

5.1. Main results

In below, we report the results of five models: a baseline model where no covariates but the PAI and SPR indicators are estimated; a FE model where all covariates and fixed-effects are included; a FE model using supplementary SPR indicators; a lagged FE model where SPRit are replaced by SPRit2; two sub-group FE models, where observations are divided into one-child and multi-child families.Footnote14 Here, the PAI is statistically significant at the 1% level across all models, showing that a 1% change yields an increase in the CADI of 13.85% for multi-child families, 20.61% for one-child families, and 17.64%–21.31% in general. Likewise, the key SPR indicator is consistently significant, demonstrating that a change in prioritising education-relevant items from the lowest to the highest rank enhances children’s academic development by 3.42% for one-child families, 6.57% for multi-child families, and 2.88%–5.13% in general.Footnote15 Further, another three SPR indicators are found influential in general, that is, the healthcare-, donation- and clothing-relevant spending priority, despite being negligible in Column (3) or (5).Footnote16 In Supplementary Material C, we present the robustness check of the results using other estimators.

With regard to control variables, several findings are noteworthy. At the child level, girls’ academic development is approximately 4.5%–5.3% better than boys’, and, compared to students at ordinary schools, attending key schools is accompanied by an increase of 1.96%–2.85% in the CADI. At the parent or household level, having savings for educational purposes is found to prompt child outcomes by 1.83% for one-child families, 3.34% for multi-child families, and 2.32%–2.48% in general; children whose parents are divorced or widowed have a 2.78%–4.46% lower CADI than their counterparts; a 1% increase in parental practices yields 8.31%–11.4% higher CADI in general, while the impact for multi-child families is 184% higher than for one-child families; compared to negative reactions, a positive reaction to children’s unsatisfactory test scores increases the CADI by 5.59% in multi-child families, 8.51% in one-child families, and 6.44%–6.79% in general. Additionally, parents’ average age and children’s health status are found to be influential in three other models but not in the multi-child model. In sum, having younger parents and worse health status are associated with the lower CADI.

We examined conversion rates, (Columns (1)–(4) of ), using four conversion factors in order: (a) children’s gender, (b) if either parent received higher education, (c) if households lived in urban areas, and (d) if children attended a key school. Moreover, in Column (5), we replace the PAI with differences in the PAI, i.e. PAIitPAIit2,Footnote17 to examine if parents’ self-advancement over time influences their children. As seen in the table, two conversion factors, i.e. tertiary education and key school, are found to differentiate effects for four spending priorities.Footnote18 Urban residence also plays a role in three aspects, while being born a boy rates as just better at converting insurance-relevant resources into the CADI. More specifically, categories of children who have one highly-educated parent at least, convert utility-, necessity-, education- and transfer-relevant goods and services more efficiently into their development. Noteworthy is that a difference in the CADI can be as large as 21.96% arising from prioritising education-relevant items from the lowest to the highest rank when a highly-educated parent is involved. Similarly, attending a key school further helps children to convert transport-, necessity-, healthcare-, education- and transfer-relevant resources into their academic performance. In contrast, compared to their rural counterparts, children living in urban areas convert healthcare-, education- and transfer-relevant resources relatively inefficiently. Lastly and importantly, as shown in Column (5), a 1% increase in the PAI growth raises the CADI by 7.11%. This finding reveals that what parents achieve for themselves is also influential to their children’s development, at least academically.

Table 1. Spending priorities, parent advantages and child academic development

Table 2. Estimates of conversion factors with interaction effects

5.2. How does family income influence prioritisation?

While poor families may have to prioritise spending on food, utilities, and daily necessities over education, wealthy families have a wider range of options that may diversify their spending. In light of this, we further explore how household net income per capita influences their spending priorities on items related to education. In the first part of Supplementary Materials Table SA4, we present the corresponding results. Column (1) is the simplest specification where only income and SPR variables are included. To account for the non-linearity of income effects widely discussed in previous literature (Cooper & Stewart, Citation2013), we then added a quadratic term since Column (2). More control variables are included in Column (3), and Column (4) utilises the supplementary SPR indicators. As seen, the results are fairly consistent, pointing out a negative relationship between household income and spending priorities on education, while additional household income can mitigate the negative effects, and the relationship eventually reverses among the richest 1%.

This is probably because richest families are more likely to spare no expense for marginal increases in their children’s education quality, while the value for money may still be a concern for better-off families, considering their spending can already grant their children access to sufficient educational resources and services. In contrast, with a commonly held belief that ‘education changes destiny’ (e.g. Chen & Wang, Citation2021; Zhu & Yu, Citation2022), poor families in China often view education as the primary means to escape poverty and enhance social mobility, leading them to prioritise spending on their children’s education. In Supplementary Materials Figure SB1, we aggregated the SPR indicator and visualised its distributions for different decile groups. Despite random variations in distributions between different groups, the top 10% of households show a clear pattern with more families prioritising education-relevant spending at a higher level. In comparison, the distribution of spending priorities in the bottom 10% of households is much more scattered, with similar percentages between groups, except for the two groups with the lowest priority.

The second part of Supplementary Materials Table SA4 examines whether children from better-off families present better academic development. Results based on the computed quartile are in Columns (5)–(6) and on the official quartile are in Columns (7)–(8). Here, to provide a more detailed picture, we utilise the components of the CADI, i.e. test score and SHD, as the DVs. Surprisingly, children from the top 25% of households tend to exhibit, on average, the highest test scores, they however have the worst behavioural development. Possible reasons here include stronger emphasis poorer families put on children’s agency and autonomy and children’s self-development of helpful learning behaviours driven by limited access to educational resources and opportunities. This can be linked to previous findings that family investments could exert a greater influence on children’s cognitive development, while parenting style may have a stronger impact on their behavioural outcomes (Cooper & Stewart, Citation2013).

Furthermore, we performed subgroup regressions using both aforementioned quartile variables and report the results in Supplementary Materials Table SA5. The results show that the top 25% of families have the strongest effects on improving children’s academic development by prioritising spending on education-relevant items. In comparison, no effect is found for the bottom 25%. A plausible reason here is the deprivation of access to good education quality so that increasing expenditure on educational activities is sadly a waste for those families. To gain deeper insights into this finding, we introduced an interaction term between ‘key school’ and the SPR indicator in this subgroup and present the results in Supplementary Materials Table SA6. We only report the results where the DV is the test score because no effect is found for SHD. A visual illustration based on the results in Column (2) can be found in Supplementary Materials Figure SB2. The results demonstrate that, as opposed to ordinary school children, children who attend a key school do benefit from a higher spending priority on education, with a potential difference as large as 12.96% to 21.06%.

5.3. How do gendered breadwinners influence prioritisation?

Mothers tend to allocate income more towards children compared to fathers (Cooper & Stewart, Citation2013). Akee, Copeland, Keeler, Angold, and Costello (Citation2010) demonstrate that the extra income improved children’s educational outcomes only if mothers were the recipients. Building on this premise, we differentiate between families where mothers earn equal or higher incomes, and those where fathers are the primary earners. This, on one hand, allows us to assess whether a breadwinning mother can positively impact children’s academic development. On the other hand, considering the association between women’s bargaining power and their income share in households (Bennett, Citation2013), we therefore examine the impact of mothers versus fathers as the primary earners on household spending prioritisation. These findings are reported in Supplementary Materials Table SA7.

As seen in Columns (1)–(2), children with a breadwinning mother perform on average 2.23–2.44% better than their counterparts, while such a mother does not alter how children convert education spending into their development. In addition, Columns (3)–(4) utilise a dummy to account for scenarios where mothers are the equal or higher earners and Columns (5)–(6) employ another dummy that excludes situations where mothers earn as much as fathers. The results delineate a negative relationship between households led by breadwinning mothers and their spending priorities on education-relevant items.Footnote19 There are two plausible reasons. First, the total household income is on average $2,359 higher when mothers are the equal or higher earners. As evidenced before, wealthier families do not maximally prioritise spending on education until they reach the richest status.

Second, as shown in Supplementary Materials Figure SB4(a), when mothers are the equal or higher earners, the share of their individual income compared to the total household income is often smaller than in scenarios where fathers are the primary earners. It is important to note that individual income here encompasses income from all types of jobs, including side jobs, although there might be other sources of additional earnings. Apart from potentially unidentified sources of income, this reduced share could be linked to living in lager families when mothers are the equal or higher earners. This trend is observable in Supplementary Materials Figure SB4(b), where 4% fewer families of 3–4 persons and 2.4% more families of more than 10 persons are evident when mothers are the equal or higher earners. Co-residing family members provide financial support but may simultaneously diminish mothers’ bargaining power, especially considering the Chinese cultural preference for co-residence with sons (Lei, Strauss, Tian, & Zhao, Citation2015). Nevertheless, Columns (3)–(6) also point out that an increase in the ratio of mothers’ income to total household income positively influences the prioritisation of education spending, consistent with earlier findings.

6. Discussions and concluding remarks

Children’s human development is often evaluated as if the priorities, defined by those who care most about them, did not matter. By using the capability approach, we focus on the concept of ranking as a way of examining spending priorities and the effect of parental advantages on their children’s development. Specifically, we assessed the role of parental spending priorities on their children’s academic development using a sample of 8,422 Chinese children and adolescents surveyed during 2012 to 2018. We found that families with the highest spending priority on their children’s education achieved returns in academic performance ranging from 2.88 to 6.57% higher than those who gave the lowest priority to educational spending. Similarly, prioritising clothing and healthcare could yield maximum returns in child development ranging from 3.24–4.95% and 2.52–3.69%, respectively. When accounting for parent’s higher education attainment, we found that the implied academic performance gains increased to 21.96%. In addition, our results further reveal a negative relationship between family income and the prioritisation of education spending. However, the situation reverses among richest families. Attending key schools particularly matters for children from worse-off families to convert spending priorities on education into their test scores. Moreover, an equally or higher-earning mother can enhance children’s academic development by 2.23% to 2.44%, and an increase in the ratio of mothers’ income to total household income can effectively foster household spending priorities on education.

Although the linkages between household educational expenditures and child development had been previously explored in the literature, this study brings new evidence to light, based on the capability approach. The core outcome in our view is how parents evaluate their children’s academic abilities can be influenced by their spending priorities. We used the capability approach as a way of talking about ‘reasons to value’, linked to parental practices and spending prioritisation. This echoes Sen (Citation2017) social choice work and the determination of the relevant informational spaces for evaluating people’s advantages. The analysis employs a broader version of the capability approach, not restricted to capabilities, and encompasses plural informational spaces. It goes further to consider not simply capabilities per se but how people value those capabilities, and the concept of ranking used for counting these values shows how people order their priorities in this regard. The impact of the rankings is clearly seen in the results reported in this paper.

Thus, our paper provides novel insights into how parents can achieve comprehensive outcomes in their children’s academic development. It goes beyond a consideration of parental practices and children’s test scores. How parents convert their resources to enhance children’s capabilities can be captured, not merely by how much they spend, but by the extent to which parents prioritise their children’s education and culture over other expenditure considerations. Notably, other spending categories, such as medication, healthcare, sports, clothing and beauty, was also found to influence child development outcomes, to some degree.

As China advances to the status of a developed nation, its economy will require increasingly-higher skilled labour and, thus, a better-educated society (Borsi, Valerio Mendoza, & Comim, Citation2022). The state’s long-term development vision and efforts toward boosting social mobility and promoting common prosperity are reflected in the banning of private after-school tutoring and the closure of independent colleges. The former was perceived to give an unfair advantage to wealthier families for whom private tutoring was more affordable, and independent colleges, prone to predatory practices, were considered as a lower-quality alternative to higher education. Nevertheless, the results of this paper suggest that equalising educational expenditure opportunities may not be enough. Households that are more willing to invest in children’s education cannot be simply regarded as wealthier. Rather, as the results here reflect, such households could be those that value education more. Considering Confucianism, which formed the core of traditional Chinese culture, places great emphasis on education and academic achievement (Gu, Citation2006), Chinese parents who overlook or give up cultivating their children’s education are likely to need help far beyond monetary assistance. This finding has significant implications for future educational policy. The reasons behind some households falling behind in prioritising their children’s education may be explained, in part, by (a) traditional gender norms shaping beliefs that spending on a daughter’s education is a waste, which, although changing as China modernises, could still bring lower expectations for girls than for boys (e.g. Chi & Rao, Citation2003; Liu, Citation2006), (b) parents holding a negative attitude towards the usefulness of study, possibly because they themselves are illiterate or less-well educated, and (c) an exaggerated emphasis on children’s agency, in that children are expected to achieve outcomes by themselves through their own diligence and intelligence without the need for parents to engage in developing real opportunities for their children to exercise this agency (Biggeri et al., Citation2011b). Given the newly-unveiled Three-Child Policy, both parents’ perceptions and the actions consequent to their perceptions for the development of children’s capabilities are now even more important for China, especially in view of the finding that the difference in children’s academic performance could be enhanced by up to 6.57% in multiple-child families. The paper suggests that the value placed on children’s development and future can be reflected in the order of relevant resources devoted to it. Policies that aim to reinforce personal and household values will provide additional returns in China’s human capital accumulation.

Table B1. Questions about test scores, SHD, parental practices & absence of parenting

Table B2. Descriptive statistics – spending priority ranking

Table B3. Descriptive statistics – children, parents & households

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (806.6 KB)

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to the editor, Richard Palmer-Jones, and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions, which greatly contributed to the refinement and expansion of this work. We also express our sincere appreciation to Mónica Martínez-Blasco, Vicente Royuela, and Xiaobing Wang for their helpful comments on Chapter 2 of Huaxin Wang-Lu’s doctoral thesis, from which the preceding version of this paper is derived. We extend our gratitude to session participants at the Inequality Conference of the International Centre for Economic Analysis and the 1st International Workshop on the Chinese Development Model for their valuable feedback. Any shortcomings are our own.

Disclosure statement

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Data availability statement

The data used in this study are open to the public on Peking University’s online platform (https://www.isss.pku.edu.cn/cfps/en). The Stata codes can be made available to bona fide researchers upon request.

Additional information

Funding

This research received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 838534 and the Horizon Europe Grant Agreement No. 101086139.

Notes

1 All questionnaires were filled in by parents on behalf of their children under 10 years old at the time of the survey.

2 In addition, Heckman, Stixrud, and Urzua (Citation2006) pointed out that aside from cognitive abilities, socio-emotional skills, such as perseverance, motivation, and self-control, had a direct impact on schooling decisions and test scores. Cunha and Heckman (Citation2007) also demonstrated the interplay of cognitive and non-cognitive skill accumulation.

3 The pairwise correlation rate of the seven dimensions of SHD is between 0.22 and 0.50.

4 Despite the dimensional distinction, test scores and SHD are both an indication of academic development, a latent construct. With this in mind, we also ran a common factor analysis (CFA) to generate an alternate CADI, which accounts for 90.1% of the common variance of the nine variables.

5 The function converting from additional income to enhanced capabilities is likely to be concave (S. Anand & Sen, Citation2000), so the natural logarithm of income is often used in the HDI. A body of literature also demonstrates the non-linearity of family income and child outcomes, see Cooper and Stewart (Citation2021).

6 ‘Filial piety’ refers to Chinese ethics rooted in Confucianism, emphasising attitudes of obedience, respect, care, and love towards parents.

7 In rural China, adult children are more likely to provide financial support to their elderly parents than their urban counterparts (Lee & Xiao, Citation1998). In contrast, due in a large part to sky-rocketing housing prices, adult children living in urban locations are now more likely to receive financial support from their elderly parents (Rosenzweig & Zhang, Citation2014). Further, Silverstein and Zhang (Citation2020) found that grandparents tended to provide economic resources to their grandchildren in rural China.

8 The pairwise correlation rate of the ten dimensions of the SPR is between -0.01 and -0.22.

9 A total of 3476 children were surveyed only once. In addition, half of the rest observations have a difference of numeric value no more than 1 in spending priorities between survey waves.

10 Additionally, we estimated this equation using the CFA-based CADI and two components of the CADI, i.e., test scores and SHD, and report the results in Supplementary Materials.

11 We also estimate supplementary SPR indicators to further check the results.

12 This approach can avoid involving a non-parametric first stage, as do non-linear or frontier models, where functioning and resources cannot be clearly distinguished (e.g., Binder & Broekel, Citation2011; Citation2012a; Citation2012b). Further, compared to a sub-group analysis, it more precisely estimates the differential effect of the resources for each category of conversion factors.

13 The dummies are gender, tertiary education of parents (equals 1 if either of the parents received higher education, and 0 otherwise), urban area, and key school.

14 The same estimation using the CFA-based DV is reported in Supplementary Materials Table SA2. In addition, we replace the CADI with its components and report corresponding main results in Supplementary Materials Table SA3.

15 Since the ranking scale is 1–10, changing from the lowest to highest priority rank is nine-fold, so we multiplied the reported coefficients by 9 to account for an overall difference.

16 The effects of prioritising financial support and social donation on children’s academic development is perhaps a result of parental altruism that some parents are more willing to invest in their children’s human capital formation than others (Das, Citation2007).

17 To ensure sufficient variations, we restricted respondents to those who had been surveyed at least three times.

18 For using the tertiary dummy, they are utility-, necessities-, education- and transfer-relevant spending items, while for using the key school dummy, they are transport-, necessities-, healthcare- and transfer-relevant spending items.

19 We depict histograms of the education SPR in Supplementary Materials Figure SB3. Upon comparison, we notice that the distribution of the education SPR is more diverse when fathers are the higher earners. However, it’s also more probable for the highest spending priority to be selected in this scenario.

20 Questions used to justify this factor are listed as QC1–QC3 in .

21 Chinese secondary schools are divided into ‘key’ and ‘ordinary’ schools. Designated key schools distinguish themselves from ordinary schools by their academic reputation and generally gain more educational resources in areas such as teachers, equipment, and funding. Students need to compete for admission to key schools, meaning that only the best cohort is entitled to study there.

22 The pairwise correlation rate of the six dimensions of parental practices provided by parents themselves is between 0.16 and 0.39.

23 Reaction (b), (c) and (d) are classified as negative. In contrast, reaction (a), (d) and (f) are classified as positive. Only reaction (g) is considered passive.

24 The test result suggests that the disturbance in our first-stage model is heteroskedastic. The levels of all exogenous regressors including fixed effects are involved.

25 ICC is calculated as the ratio of the between-group variance relative to the total variance in the sample. It describes the extent to which observations within province groups are similar to each other.

References

  • Akee, R. K., Copeland, W. E., Keeler, G., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2010). Parents’ incomes and children’s outcomes: a quasi-experiment using transfer payments from casino profits. American Economic Journal. Applied Economics, 2(1), 86–115. doi:10.1257/app.2.1.86
  • Alderson, P. (2001). Research by children. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 4(2), 139–153. doi:10.1080/13645570120003
  • Anand, P., Hunter, G., Carter, I., Dowding, K., Guala, F., & Van Hees, M. (2009). The development of capability indicators. Journal of Human Development and Capabilities, 10(1), 125–152. doi:10.1080/14649880802675366
  • Anand, S., & Sen, A. (2000). The income component of the human development index. Journal of Human Development, 1(1), 83–106. doi:10.1080/14649880050008782
  • Anich, R., Biggeri, M., Libanora, R., & Mariani, S. (2011). Street children in Kampala and NGOs’ actions: Understanding capabilities deprivation and expansion. In Children and the capability approach (pp. 107–136). London: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Arunachalam, S., Bahadir, S. C., Bharadwaj, S. G., & Guesalaga, R. (2020). New product introductions for low-income consumers in emerging markets. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 48(5), 914–940. doi:10.1007/s11747-019-00648-8
  • Ballet, J., Biggeri, M., & Comim, F. (2011). Children’s agency and the capability approach: A conceptual framework. In Children and the capability approach (pp. 22–45). London: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Baum, C. F., & Lewbel, A. (2019). Advice on using heteroskedasticity-based identification. Stata Journal: Promoting Communications on Statistics and Stata, 19(4), 757–767. doi:10.1177/1536867X19893614
  • Becker, G. S. (1974). A theory of social interactions. Journal of Political Economy, 82(6), 1063–1093. doi:10.1086/260265
  • Becker, G. S., & Tomes, N. (1976). Child endowments and the quantity and quality of children. Journal of Political Economy, 84(4, Part 2), S143–S162. doi:10.1086/260536
  • Bellanca, N., Biggeri, M., & Marchetta, F. (2011). An extension of the capability approach: Towards a theory of dis-capability. Alter, 5(3), 158–176. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1875067211000514 doi:10.1016/j.alter.2011.05.001
  • Bennett, F. (2013). Researching within-household distribution: Overview, developments, debates, and methodological challenges. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75(3), 582–597. doi:10.1111/jomf.12020
  • Biggeri, M., Ballet, J., & Comim, F. (2011a). The capability approach and research on children: Capability approach and children’s issues. In Children and the good life (pp. 75–89). Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Biggeri, M., Ballet, J., & Comim, F. (2011b). Children and the capability approach. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Biggeri, M., & Mehrotra, S. (2011). Child poverty as capability deprivation: How to choose domains of child well-being and poverty. In Children and the capability approach (pp. 46–75). London: Palgrave Macmillan UK. doi:10.1057/9780230308374{\_}3
  • Binder, M., & Broekel, T. (2011). Applying a non-parametric efficiency analysis to measure conversion efficiency in Great Britain. Journal of Human Development and Capabilities, 12(2), 257–281. doi:10.1080/19452829.2011.571088
  • Binder, M., & Broekel, T. (2012a). Happiness no matter the cost? An examination on how efficiently individuals reach their happiness levels. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(4), 621–645. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9283-5
  • Binder, M., & Broekel, T. (2012b). The neglected dimension of well-being: Analyzing the development of “conversion efficiency” in Great Britain. Journal of Socio-Economics, 41(1), 37–47. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053535711001302 doi:10.1016/j.socec.2011.10.002
  • Black, M. M., Trude, A. C., & Lutter, C. K. (2020). All children thrive: Integration of nutrition and early childhood development. Annual Review of Nutrition, 40(1), 375–406. doi:10.1146/annurev-nutr-120219-023757
  • Borghans, L., Meijers, H., & ter Weel, B. (2008). The role of noncognitive skills in explaining cognitive test scores. Economic Inquiry, 46(1), 2–12. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2007.00073.x
  • Borsi, M. T., Valerio Mendoza, O. M., & Comim, F. (2022). Measuring the provincial supply of higher education institutions in China. China Economic Review, 71, 101724. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1043951X21001425 doi:10.1016/j.chieco.2021.101724
  • Burchardt, T., & Vizard, P. (2011). Operationalizing’ the capability approach as a basis for equality and human rights monitoring in twenty-first-century Britain. Journal of Human Development and Capabilities, 12(1), 91–119. doi:10.1080/19452829.2011.541790
  • Burton, P., Phipps, S., & Curtis, L. (2002). All in the family: A simultaneous model of parenting style and child conduct. American Economic Review, 92(2), 368–372. doi:10.1257/000282802320191633
  • Byskov, M. F. (2018). Selecting capabilities for development. In F. Comim, S. Fennell, & P. B. Anand (Eds.), New frontiers of the capability approach (pp. 198–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Chen, J., & Wang, D. (2021). Class consciousness of rural migrant children in China. China Quarterly, 247, 814–834. doi:10.1017/S0305741020001083
  • Chen, S. H., Deng, X. F., Zhang, E., Wang, L. K., & Liu, C. H. (2021). Self-regulatory development in children from Chinese immigrant families: Evidence for commonality and specificity. Child Development, 92(6), e1126–e1137. doi:10.1111/cdev.13593
  • Chi, J., & Rao, N. (2003). Parental beliefs about school learning and children’s educational attainment: Evidence from rural China. Ethos, 31(3), 330–356. doi:10.1525/eth.2003.31.3.330
  • Chiappero, E., Scervini, F., & Salardi, P. (2018). From resources to functioning: Rethinking and measuring conversion rates. In New frontiers of the capability approach (pp. 232–245). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Chung, S. H., Zhang, M., & Partridge, M. D. (2020). Positive feedback in skill aggregation across Chinese cities. Regional Studies, 54(11), 1596–1611. doi:10.1080/00343404.2020.1743823
  • Comim, F. (2021). A post-generalizability method for human development indicators. Social Indicators Research, 158(3), 1179–1198. doi:10.1007/s11205-021-02737-0
  • Comim, F., Fennell, S., & Anand, P. B. (Eds.). (2018). New frontiers of the capability approach. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/9781108559881/type/book.
  • Comim, F., Qizilbash, M., & Alkire, S. (2008). The capability approach: Concepts, measures and applications.
  • Cooper, K., & Stewart, K. (2013). Does money affect children’s outcomes?.
  • Cooper, K., & Stewart, K. (2021). Does household income affect children’s outcomes? A systematic review of the evidence. Child Indicators Research, 14(3), 981–1005. doi:10.1007/s12187-020-09782-0
  • Cui, Y., Liu, H., & Zhao, L. (2019). Mother’s education and child development: Evidence from the compulsory school reform in China. Journal of Comparative Economics, 47(3), 669–692. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0147596718305407 doi:10.1016/j.jce.2019.04.001
  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2007). The technology of skill formation. American Economic Review, 97(2), 31–47. doi:10.1257/aer.97.2.31
  • Das, M. (2007). Persistent inequality: An explanation based on limited parental altruism. Journal of Development Economics, 84(1), 251–270. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0304387806001209 doi:10.1016/j.jdeveco.2005.10.006
  • Dooley, M., & Stewart, J. (2007). Family income, parenting styles and child behavioural-emotional outcomes. Health Economics, 16(2), 145–162. doi:10.1002/hec.1142
  • Goldhagen, J. L., Shenoda, S., Oberg, C., Mercer, R., Kadir, A., Raman, S., … Spencer, N. J. (2020). Rights, justice, and equity: A global agenda for child health and wellbeing. Lancet. Child & Adolescent Health, 4(1), 80–90. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S2352464219303463 doi:10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30346-3
  • Gu, M. (2006). An analysis of the impact of traditional Chinese culture on Chinese education. Frontiers of Education in China, 1(2), 169–190. doi:10.1007/s11516-006-0001-8
  • Gunnar, R. J., Kanerva, K., Salmi, S., Häyrinen, T., Haataja, L., Pakarinen, M. P., & Merras-Salmio, L. (2020). Neonatal intestinal failure is independently associated with impaired cognitive development later in childhood. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 70(1), 64–71. doi:10.1097/MPG.0000000000002529
  • Hansen, L. P. (1982). Large sample properties of generalized method of moments estimators. Econometrica, 50(4), 1029. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/1912775?origin=crossref doi:10.2307/1912775
  • Hanushek, E. A., Rivkin, S. G., & Schiman, J. C. (2016). Dynamic effects of teacher turnover on the quality of instruction. Economics of Education Review, 55, 132–148. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S027277571530087X doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2016.08.004
  • Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2008). The role of cognitive skills in economic development. Journal of Economic Literature, 46(3), 607–668. doi:10.1257/jel.46.3.607
  • Heckman, J. J. (2008). Schools, skills, and synapses. Economic Inquiry, 46(3), 289–324. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2008.00163.x
  • Heckman, J. J., & Mosso, S. (2014). The economics of human development and social mobility. Annual Review of Economics, 6(1), 689–733. doi:10.1146/annurev-economics-080213-040753
  • Heckman, J. J., Stixrud, J., & Urzua, S. (2006). The effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. Journal of Labor Economics, 24(3), 411–482. doi:10.1086/504455
  • Hsu, S., & Wu, Y.-Y. (2015). Education as cultivation in Chinese culture. Singapore: Springer.
  • Krumbiegel, K., Maertens, M., & Wollni, M. (2018). The role of fairtrade certification for wages and job satisfaction of plantation workers. World Development, 102, 195–212. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0305750X17303133 doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.09.020
  • Lansdown, G. (2004). Participation and young children. Early Childhood Matters, 103, 4–14.
  • Lee, Y. J., & Xiao, Z. (1998). Children’s support for elderly parents in urban and rural China: Results from a national survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 13(1), 39–62. doi:10.1023/a:1006591608724
  • Lei, X., Strauss, J., Tian, M., & Zhao, Y. (2015). Living arrangements of the elderly in china: Evidence from the charls national baseline. China Economic Journal, 8(3), 191–214. doi:10.1080/17538963.2015.1102473
  • Lewbel, A. (2012). Using heteroscedasticity to identify and estimate mismeasured and endogenous regressor models. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics, 30(1), 67–80. doi:10.1080/07350015.2012.643126
  • Li, L., & Wu, X. (2019). Housing price and intergenerational co-residence in urban China. Journal of Housing Economics, 45, 101596. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1051137717302723 doi:10.1016/j.jhe.2018.08.001
  • Liebenehm, S. (2018). Temporal stability of risk attitudes and the impact of adverse shocks—A panel data analysis from Thailand and Vietnam. World Development, 102, 262–274. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0305750X15305350 doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.07.020
  • Liu, D., Chen, D., & Brown, B. B. (2020). Do parenting practices and child disclosure predict parental knowledge? A meta-analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(1), 1–16. doi:10.1007/s10964-019-01154-4
  • Liu, F. (2006). Boys as only-children and girls as only-children—parental gendered expectations of the only-child in the nuclear Chinese family in present-day China. Gender and Education, 18(5), 491–505. doi:10.1080/09540250600881626
  • Liu, Z., & Yu, L. (2020). Stay or leave? The role of air pollution in urban migration choices. Ecological Economics, 177(June 2019), 106780. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2020.106780
  • Lynch, A. D., Lerner, R. M., & Leventhal, T. (2013). Adolescent academic achievement and school engagement: An examination of the role of school-wide peer culture. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(1), 6–19. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9833-0
  • Nussbaum, M. C. (2000). Women and human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/9780511841286/type/book
  • Nussbaum, M. C. (2006). Frontiers of justice: Disability, nationality, species membership. Cambridge: Belknap Press.
  • Nussbaum, M. C. (2011). Creating capabilities: The human development approach. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Nussbaum, M. C. (2018). The monarchy of fear: A philosopher looks at our political crisis. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  • Pagan, A. R., & Hall, A. D. (1983). Diagnostic tests as residual analysis. Econometric Reviews, 2(2), 159–218. doi:10.1080/07311768308800039
  • Punch, S. (2002). Negotiating autonomy: Childhoods in rural Bolivia. In L. Alanen & B. Mayall (Eds.), Conceptualising child-adult relations (pp. 37–50). London: Routledge.
  • Ratigan, K. (2017). Disaggregating the developing welfare state: Provincial social policy regimes in china. World Development, 98, 467–484. doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.05.010
  • Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. doi:10.4159/9780674042605/html
  • Rose, L. T., & Fischer, K. W. (2011). Intelligence in childhood. In The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 144–174). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Rosenzweig, M., & Zhang, J. (2014). Co-residence, life-cycle savings and inter-generational support in urban China. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/papers/w20057.pdf
  • Saito, M. (2003). Amartya Sen’s capability approach to education: A critical exploration. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 37(1), 17–33. doi:10.1111/1467-9752.3701002
  • Sen, A. (1980). Equality of what?. In The tanner lectures on human values (Vol. 1, pp. 197–220). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sen, A. (1997). Maximization and the act of choice. Econometrica, 65(4), 745. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/2171939?origin=crossref doi:10.2307/2171939
  • Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sen, A. (2002). Rationality and freedom. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Sen, A. (2008). The economics of happiness and capability. In Capabilities and Happiness (pp. 16–27). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sen, A. (2009). The idea of justice. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Sen, A. (2017). Collective choice and social welfare. London: Penguin Books.
  • Shrestha, M., Weissman, A., Thapa, B., Adhikari, R., & Perry, K. (2021). Malnutrition matters: Impact of nutritional status on early childhood development in Nepal. Current Developments in Nutrition, 5(Supplement_2), 688–688. Retrieved from https://academic.oup.com/cdn/article/5/Supplement{\_}2/688/6292934 doi:10.1093/cdn/nzab045_070
  • Silverstein, M., & Zhang, W. (2020). Grandparents’ financial contributions to grandchildren in rural China: The role of remittances, household structure, and patrilineal culture. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(5), 1042–1052. Retrieved from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/75/5/1042/5304471 doi:10.1093/geronb/gbz009
  • Smith, M. D., Rabbitt, M. P., & Coleman-Jensen, A. (2017). Who are the world’s food insecure? New evidence from the Food and Agriculture Organization’s food insecurity experience scale. World Development, 93, 402–412. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0305750X17300086 doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.01.006
  • Sun, R. (2004). Worry about medical care, family support, and depression of the elders in urban China. Research on Aging, 26(5), 559–585. doi:10.1177/0164027504266467
  • Suzumura, K., (2011). Handbook of social choice and welfare (K. J., Arrow, A. K., Sen, K. & Suzumura, (Eds.), 1st ed., Vol. 2, p. 605–685). North-Holland: Elsevier. https://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:eee:socchp:2-23
  • Swift, A., & Brighouse, H. (2014). Family values: The ethics of parent-child relationships. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • Sylvia, S., Luo, R., Zhong, J., Dill, S.-E., Medina, A., & Rozelle, S. (2022). Passive versus active service delivery: Comparing the effects of two parenting interventions on early cognitive development in rural China. World Development, 149, 105686. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0305750X21003016 doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2021.105686
  • Vasilyeva, M., Laski, E., Veraksa, A., Weber, L., & Bukhalenkova, D. (2018). Distinct pathways from parental beliefs and practices to children’s numeric skills. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19(4), 345–366. doi:10.1080/15248372.2018.1483371
  • World Bank Group. (2018). World Development Report 2018: Learning to realize education’s promise. Washington, DC: World Bank. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10986/28340
  • Xie, Y., Zhang, X., Tu, P., Ren, Q., Sun, Y., Lv, P., … Wu, Q. (2017). China Family Panel Studies User’s Manual (3rd ed.). Beijing: Institute of Social Science Survey, Peking University.
  • Yergeau, M.-E. (2020). Tourism and local welfare: A multilevel analysis in Nepal’s protected areas. World Development, 127, 104744. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0305750X19303936 doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2019.104744
  • Yi, D., Deng, X., Fan, G.-Z., & Ong, S. E. (2018). House price and co-residence with older parents: Evidence from China. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 57(3), 502–533. doi:10.1007/s11146-018-9653-4
  • Yin, T. (2010). Parent–child co-residence and bequest motives in China. China Economic Review, 21(4), 521–531. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1043951X10000507 doi:10.1016/j.chieco.2010.05.003
  • Yousefzadeh, S., Biggeri, M., Arciprete, C., & Haisma, H. (2019). A capability approach to child growth. Child Indicators Research, 12(2), 711–731. doi:10.1007/s12187-018-9548-1
  • Zhang, Z., Ma, C., & Wang, A. (2021). A longitudinal study of multidimensional poverty in rural China from 2010 to 2018. Economics Letters, 204, 109912. Retrieved from https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165176521001890 doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2021.109912
  • Zhu, H. (2016). Adult children’s characteristics and intergenerational financial transfers in urban China. Chinese Journal of Sociology, 2(1), 75–94. doi:10.1177/2057150X15624085
  • Zhu, M., Kakwani, N., Li, S., & Wang, X. (2023). Social opportunity function and equity in opportunity in China. In Social inequality in China (pp. 209–233). London: World Scientific.
  • Zhu, Y., & Yu, D. (2022). Education and happiness: Does education expenditure undermine households’ subjective well-being? Evidence from China. Applied Economics, 55(50), 5925–5938. doi:10.1080/00036846.2022.2140772

Appendix A.

Control variables

In addition to the CADI, PAI and SPR variables, we introduced 11 covariates to control for heterogeneity across samples; five at the child level and six at the parent and household level. These variables were derived from the stories that the CFPS data illustrate and assessed from a capability perspective. We elaborate on both these levels here.

Child-level covariates

The CFPS collected rich information concerning children’s development. For instance, it records how many times a child went to hospitals or clinics in cities, towns, communities, or villages in the last 12 months. One can infer the status of children’s health from this information. In addition, the CFPS registered information about whom the actual carers of a child were, and how many times per week, on average, the child met at least one of his or her parents. Based on these questions, we created a dummy specifying children who were not looked after by their parents in person and who saw neither of their parents per week.Footnote20 Furthermore, the quality of teaching, the school atmosphere, and the peer culture can be associated with children’s academic development (Lynch, Lerner, & Leventhal, Citation2013). In the context of China, attending a key school is both a reflection and a determinant of student achievement.Footnote21 Hence, we created another dummy to distinguish students in key schools from those in ordinary schools. We also included gender and sleep time to control for individual heterogeneity.

Parent- and household-level covariates

A substantial body of literature examined the effects of parenting style on child health and student achievement (e.g. Burton, Phipps, & Curtis, Citation2002; Cui et al., Citation2019; Dooley & Stewart, Citation2007). Two variables were created in our dataset to control for parents’ functionings. One accounts for parental practices, measuring the extent to which parents paid attention to and monitored their child’s learning and recreational activities.Footnote22 We also incorporated information based on the observations of home environment by the CFPS’s interviewers, assessing to what extent parents were concerned with their child’s education and actively communicated with their child. Questions in this regard are summarised in Appendix (QB1–QB8).

A further covariate accounts for parents’ reactions to their children’s unsatisfactory test scores, which is closely related to our dependent variable (DV). The possible reactions are summarised as: (a) contact the teacher; (b) physical punishment; (c) scold the child; (d) ask the child to study harder; (e) ground the child; (f) help the child more; (g) take no reaction. We divided these options into three categories, i.e. negative, passive and positive.Footnote23 Further, other covariates are the average age of parents and several dummies that refer to parental marital status, household residence (in urban or rural area), and a family’s savings for children’s education, respectively. Descriptive statistics of other variables are given in Appendix .

Appendix B.

Summative tables and descriptive statistics