463
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Patterns of language and auditory dysfunction in 6-year-old children with epilepsy

, &
Pages 82-89 | Received 29 Oct 2008, Published online: 12 Jul 2009

Abstract

In a previous study we reported difficulty with expressive language and visuoperceptual ability in preschool children with epilepsy and otherwise normal development. The present study analysed speech and language dysfunction for each individual in relation to epilepsy variables, ear preference, and intelligence in these children and described their auditory function. Twenty 6-year-old children with epilepsy (14 females, 6 males; mean age 6:5 y, range 6 y–6 y 11 mo) and 30 reference children without epilepsy (18 females, 12 males; mean age 6:5 y, range 6 y–6 y 11 mo) were assessed for language and auditory ability. Low scores for the children with epilepsy were analysed with respect to speech-language domains, type of epilepsy, site of epileptiform activity, intelligence, and language laterality. Auditory attention, perception, discrimination, and ear preference were measured with a dichotic listening test, and group comparisons were performed. Children with left-sided partial epilepsy had extensive language dysfunction. Most children with partial epilepsy had phonological dysfunction. Language dysfunction was also found in children with generalized and unclassified epilepsies. The children with epilepsy performed significantly worse than the reference children in auditory attention, perception of vowels and discrimination of consonants for the right ear and had more left ear advantage for vowels, indicating undeveloped language laterality.

Introduction

Speech and language ability has previously rarely been comprehensively described in studies of cognitive abilities in children with epilepsy. According to Deonna Citation[1] there are several situations in which a direct causal link between epilepsy and language disorder exists. Both language and the motor command of speech can be affected.

There are recent indications that epilepsy may be more common in language-impaired children than is generally known. A high proportion of Electroencephalogram (EEG) abnormalities and epileptic syndromes has been found in children with severe language impairment Citation[2], and Sillanpää reported a high proportion of speech disorder in children with epilepsy Citation[3]. Speech and language difficulties are known to be associated with specific epilepsy syndromes, such as the Landau Kleffner syndrome (LKS) and benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BCECTS) Citation[1], Citation[4], Citation[5], but they have also been reported in focal epilepsy Citation[6], Citation[7].

Recently, we described speech, language, and cognition in a multidisciplinary study of a regional cohort of preschool children with epilepsy without previously known learning disability, cerebral palsy, and/or autism Citation[8]. Low achievements were found in visuoperception and expressive verbal ability. The study included a heterogeneous group of children with epilepsy, and therefore questions were raised regarding the individual patterns of dysfunction with respect to the different types of epilepsy and sites of epileptiform activity. In the present study the performance of these children is presented individually, and the auditory function of the children is further analysed.

The following specific questions were asked:

  • Do children with the same types of epilepsy or sites of epileptiform activity display deficits in the same speech, language, and communication domains, and do those with different types and localizations display different deficits?

  • Which ear preference for auditory stimuli is found in children with partial and generalized epilepsy and with different locations of epileptiform activity?

  • Is there a difference between the children with epilepsy and the reference children in terms of auditory attention, perception, discrimination, and ear preference?

Method

Participants

The regional cohort Citation[8] of children with epilepsy and without previously known cerebral palsy, learning disability, or autism from the city of Göteborg and eight surrounding municipalities comprised 14 girls and 6 boys, aged 6 years to 6 years 11 months (mean age 6:5 years). Ten children had partial epilepsy with or without secondary generalization, three with focal epileptiform activity in the left hemisphere, three in the right hemisphere, one in the frontal lobes, and one with epileptiform activity alternating between the right and left temporal lobes; in two children, the interictal EEG was normal. Six children had primary generalized seizures; five of them had absence epilepsy and one myoclonic astatic epilepsy. Four children were allocated to the group ‘undetermined’ whether focal or generalized, one of whom had Landau Kleffner syndrome (LKS). All the children were receiving antiepileptic drug treatment. The median full-scale IQ (FSIQ) was 95, verbal IQ (VIQ) 103, and performance IQ (PIQ) 88. Two children had a previously unknown learning disability (FSIQ < 70). Four children were bilingual. All the children had normal hearing within speech frequencies, apart from one child with a hearing level of 25 dB due to a temporary infection. Thirty children without epilepsy, 18 girls and 12 boys, were used as a reference group. Mean age of the reference children was 6:5 years, range 6 years to 6 years 11 months. They were similar in terms of age, sex, place of living, and mono-/bilingualism Citation[8].

Assessment procedure

The assessment battery comprised a wide range of tests for speech, language, and auditory ability, presented in the same order for all the children. The measures used for these tests are shown in . For an overview, the tests are grouped into domains.

  1. Domain of oral motor ability and articulation. The neurolinguistic test battery (Nelli) Citation[9] and the quantitative measures according to Jennische Citation[10] were used to assess oral motor ability and articulation. Oral motor ability is divided into ‘positions’, finding positions with tongue and lips, and ‘movements’, meaning repeated sequences of oral movements. Articulatory positions is a test of the repetition of isolated sounds and syllables and of rapid sequences of equal syllables.

  2. Domain of phonology and literacy. Articulatory patterns refer to phonology-driven sequences of varying syllables, words, and word combinations and non-words from the Nelli test Citation[9]. Phonology was assessed with a repetition task, including the children's representations of all Swedish phonemes. Emerging literacy was assessed with phoneme blending, a subtest of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA) Citation[11], and letter naming.

  3. Domain of grammar and semantics. Expressive grammar was assessed with the grammar subtest of the ITPA, reflecting morphology. Receptive grammar was assessed with the Test for Reception Of Grammar (TROG), reflecting both morphology and syntax Citation[12]. Receptive vocabulary was assessed with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) Citation[13].

  4. Domain of word retrieval and narrative ability. For expressive vocabulary, the word retrieval subtest of the ITPA (retrieval within semantic categories) and Rapid Confrontation Naming (RCFN) (picture naming) were used Citation[14]. In RCFN, the time and number of mistakes were registered. In addition, the auditory analogy ITPA subtest was used. Narrative ability was assessed with the story retelling test of Nelli. The number of events missing from a total of ten was registered.

  5. Domain of pragmatic ability. This domain was assessed with the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC) parental questionnaire Citation[15]. For the pragmatic composite measure, the total scores were given.

  6. Domain of auditory attention and memory. In this domain, the auditory attention measure of the dichotic listening test (DL) is included (see below). Auditory short-term memory was assessed with the digit span test (ITPA).

Table I.  Speech and language test battery for 6-year-old children with epilepsy and reference children.

Auditory attention, level of perception, discrimination, and ear advantage

A dichotic listening test (DL) Citation[16] was used to assess auditory attention, auditory perception level, auditory discrimination, and ear preference, using 108 pairs of consonant-vowel (CV) syllables with randomly varying consonants /p, t, k, b, d, g/ and vowels /a, i, u/. Different CV syllables were presented simultaneously to both ears in a non-forced condition through sound-proof headphones (HD 200, Sennheiser, Tullamore, Republic of Ireland). The children were asked to repeat the pairs of syllables. The results were analysed with respect to the percentage of correctly repeated consonants, vowels, and whole CV syllables for each ear. The level of auditory attention was calculated as the percentage of simultaneously presented pairs of CV syllables correctly repeated. The level of auditory perception was calculated as the average percentage of all correctly repeated consonants, vowels, and CV syllables for each ear. As a measure of auditory discrimination, the number of consonants and vowels that were correctly repeated less than 10% of the times given to each ear was used. The value of vowels could vary between 0 and 3 for each ear (3 corresponding to the lowest discrimination ability), and the value of consonants could vary between 0 and 6 (6 being the lowest ability). The inability to discriminate between the separate consonants and vowels could thus be quantified. The laterality index for consonants, vowels and CV syllables was calculated as (RE + LE)/(RE-LE)×100. Right ear advantage (REA) was established when the laterality index was equal to or greater than +5; left ear advantage (LEA) when it was equal to or below −5; no ear advantage (NEA) when the laterality index was between +5 and −5.

Statistical analysis

The results representing a low score were defined for each speech and language test. A result of the stanine value 1 was defined as a low score for the subtests of the ITPA, and a result corresponding to the 10th percentile or lower in norms for Swedish children was defined as a low score for the TROG and the PPVT. For tests where no Swedish norms were available, the results of the reference group were used as norms. Thus, a result equivalent to, or lower than, the score that corresponded to the 10th percentile of the reference group with the lowest achievements was defined as a low score. The number of low scores within each language domain were counted and presented for each child with epilepsy, together with the type of EEG abnormality, site of epileptiform activity, FSIQ, VIQ and PIQ, and ear preference (). The percentage of the children with epilepsy and the reference children with at least one low score within each domain are given. The average of low scores for speech and language tests for both groups was calculated. Comparisons between the children with epilepsy and the reference group were made for the assessments of the DL test: auditory attention, auditory perception level, and auditory discrimination (Mann-Whitney U-test). Ear preference was compared using the chi-square test. Statistical significance was defined as a P-value of <0.05, two-tailed test.

Table II.  Epilepsy group. Results presented individually for each child: number of variables with low scores (≤ the score which was reached by the 10th percentile of the reference group with the lowest achievements within each domain, intelligence, epilepsy variables and ear preference.

Ethics

Ethical permission for the study was given by the local ethics committee, while written informed consent to participate was received from all parents.

Results

Domains of speech and language dysfunction

Almost all the children with epilepsy had low scores for at least one of the variables within the oral motor domain (19/20, 95%) (Table II). In the domain of phonology and literacy, 14/20 (70%) of the children had low scores for one or more variables. In the domain of grammar and semantics, 8/20 children with epilepsy (40%) had low scores, while, in the domain of word retrieval and narrative ability, 12/20 (60%) obtained low scores. In the domain of pragmatic ability, only three children with epilepsy (15%) were reported to have difficulties, while, in the domain of auditory attention and memory, 13/20 (65%) obtained low scores. The comparable percentages for the reference children are given in Table II. On average, the children with epilepsy had low scores in 6.2 measures, as opposed to 3.0 measures for the reference group.

Speech and language dysfunction, epilepsy variables, and intelligence

In children with partial epilepsy, the following pattern was found: all three children (code nos. 6, 7, and 10) with a left hemispheric site of epileptiform activity had low scores in one or more tests in all the language domains except pragmatics, indicating broad-based language disorders of varying degrees. In contrast, the three children with a right hemispheric site of epileptiform activity (code nos. 4, 8, and 12) did not obtain low scores in the domain of grammar/semantics, and only one of them obtained low scores for word retrieval/narrative ability.

In children with generalized epilepsy, the pattern of dysfunction was less uniform: one child with myoclonic astatic epilepsy (code no. 13) had a dysfunction in all domains, two of the children with absence epilepsy (code nos. 9 and 16) had a dysfunction in only one domain, while the others (code nos. 3, 14, and 20) had varied patterns of dysfunction.

The child with Landau Kleffner syndrome (LKS) (code no. 11) had low scores in all domains except pragmatic ability, and the dysfunction confirmed a diagnosis of expressive language impairment. This child refused to perform some of the tests because of additional severe difficulties with articulation and stuttering.

Despite low scores for speech and language measures, 17/20 children had a higher VIQ than PIQ, with a VIQ of >85 in all but 2 and a PIQ of <85 in 8. One of the two children with an FSIQ of <70 (code no 6) had a VIQ of 90 and a PIQ of 46, indicating a visuoperceptual deficit rather than a general learning disability (Table II).

Ear advantage and epilepsy variables

No clear pattern of association with type of epilepsy, site of epileptiform activity, and ear preference was observed. LEA (right hemispheric dominance) was found in the partial, generalized, and unclassified types of epilepsy (Table II).

Group comparisons of the dichotic listening test

The children with epilepsy obtained statistically significantly lower scores than the reference group in the auditory attention measure of the DL test (P = 0.018) (). In three measures of the DL test, the children with epilepsy obtained significantly lower scores than the reference children, indicating a less dominant left hemisphere for the analyses of speech sounds: the level of auditory perception of vowels presented to the right ear (P = 0.019), the discrimination of consonants presented to the right ear (P = 0.015), and ear preference for vowels (P = 0.042) (Table III), the latter presented in our previous study Citation[8]. Of 19 children with epilepsy, 6 (32%) had a clear LEA (right hemispheric dominance—code no. 17 had NEA only for consonants) and another 4/19 children (20%) had NEA (no hemispheric dominance) in one or more of the CV syllables, vowels, or consonants (Table II). In the reference group, only 4 of 29 children (14%) had a clear LEA, while 13/29 (45%) had NEA in one or more of the CV syllables, consonants, or vowels. As a result, LEA was more common in the children with epilepsy Citation[8].

Table III.  Auditory attention, perception and discrimination measured with the dichotic listening test in 19 6-year-old children with epilepsy and 29 reference children. Comparison between groups (Mann-Whitney U-test).

Discussion

In this cohort of 6-year-old children with epilepsy and previously expected normal development, recruited from a regional sample, the majority obtained low scores for several aspects of speech and language. Almost all the children with epilepsy had low scores in the domain of oral motor ability/articulation, and around two-thirds of the children obtained low scores within the domains of phonology/literacy and auditory attention/memory. Different localizations of epileptiform activity resulted in different patterns of dysfunction. In children with partial epilepsy with a left-sided focus, the dysfunction was most pronounced. Furthermore, children with generalized seizures also displayed a dysfunction in a variety of speech and language areas. Tests of dichotic listening revealed lower scores in auditory attention, perception level, and discrimination, and unclear or different laterality for language in the children with epilepsy compared with the reference group.

Oral motor dysfunction, as found in our study group, has previously been reported in many children with epilepsy. However, it is unclear whether this is an effect of medication or of the epileptic activity Citation[17], and our study group was too small to answer this question. In addition, stuttering in children with epilepsy has recently been highlighted Citation[18], Citation[19]. We also found that stuttering was an obvious impediment in the child with LKS.

Phonological deficits have mainly been reported in children with BCECTS Citation[1] but seldom in a group of children including other types of epilepsy, such as we found. The domain of phonology/literacy included tests of phoneme blending, letter naming, and non-word repetition, and low scores in may be predictors of subsequent reading and writing difficulties Citation[20]. Other studies have also reported reading and writing difficulties in children with epilepsy Citation[1], Citation[21–23].

Deficits in general language-processing capacity is thought to underlie verbal expressive disorder, as in the domain of word retrieval/narrative ability and in auditory attention/short-term memory Citation[17], Citation[20]. Our results indicate that children with epilepsy have a general language-processing disorder, causing the deficits in these domains. However, assessment of auditory attention in small children is difficult to perform, as stated by Hugdahl Citation[24]. We used a procedure of simultaneous attention to competing stimuli, which has not previously been tested, and our results should therefore be interpreted with caution. However, they agree with the results reported by Svoboda, who stated that children with epilepsy often have difficulty listening to competing stimuli against a noisy background Citation[17].

Focal seizures affecting brain areas subserving speech and language are thought to be linked to specific speech and language disability Citation[1]. In the present study, this was apparent in the children with a left hemispheric focus, who had more severe dysfunction. One child with left temporal partial epilepsy had a pattern of dysfunction compatible with severe expressive language disorder, indicating a possible direct effect of the epilepsy on speech and language. In addition, one child with a severe expressive disorder had LKS, known to be a predominantly receptive disorder. This indicates variability in symptoms in LKS. Dysfunction in various language domains was also present in some children with generalized seizures, indicating a general effect on language competence, which is not usually reported.

Our results for language laterality are not clearly linked to a particular type of epilepsy, such as those of Pisano et al., who found a lack of hemispheric specialization for phonological processing and impaired access to stored lexical knowledge in familial lateral temporal lobe epilepsy Citation[25]. Pecini and colleagues found that children with the expressive subtype of specific language impairment had a reduced specialization for language compared with age-matched controls Citation[26]. These findings are comparable to the expressive difficulties in our children with epilepsy, in which several measures revealed a different laterality for the reception and analysis of speech sounds compared with reference children of the same age. It is thus probable that the epileptic activity affects the development of language laterality in these children. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that AED treatment may have influenced this hampered development.

When assessing children with epilepsy, difficulty with attention and memory may affect the results, and this should be taken into consideration when choosing test instruments. The dysfunction in speech, language, and auditory ability found in our study group indicates potential difficulty with subsequent school achievements. The dysfunction in memory and attention may be an obstacle to general performance. Speech and language intervention and extra support for reading and writing acquisition may be needed, as well as additional instruction in the classroom and the opportunity to rest.

Limitations

This study was performed on a small number of children and the results need to be replicated. The dichotic listening test is difficult to perform in 6-year-old children, as the attention ability is not yet fully developed in this age group. The results should be interpreted with caution, as there was great variation in the results for both groups.

Conclusion

Six-year-old children with epilepsy and no previously known cerebral palsy, learning disability, or autism may have a dysfunction in all speech and language domains, in spite of verbal intelligence within the normal range. Children with a left temporal epileptic focus or the Landau Kleffner syndrome are those with the most extensive speech and language dysfunction, and partial epilepsy with either a left- or right-sided focus is more often associated with a dysfunction in phonology. Children with other epileptic foci and with generalized or unclassified epilepsy may also have a dysfunction in speech and language, although less specific. Unusual ear preference and poor auditory perception and discrimination indicate the hampered development of language laterality. The long-term consequences of epilepsy in school-age children need to be studied.

Acknowledgements

We thank the children and their parents who participated in the study and also Dr Anders Hedström at the Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital for supplying EEG results, neuropsychologist Gerd Viggedal for providing neuropsychological data and statistician Anders Odén for expert statistical help. This study was supported by the Linnéa and Josef Carlsson Foundation, Föreningen Margarethahemmet, the Swedish Bureau for Epilepsy, the Foundation of the Queen Silvia Jubilee Fund for Research on Children and Handicaps, and research grants from Sahlgrenska University Hospital. Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

References

  • Deonna T, Roulet-Perez E. Cognitive and behavioural disorders of epileptic origin in children. Mac Keith Press, Cambridge, New York 2005
  • Rejnö-Habte Selassie G, Jennische M, Kyllerman M, Viggedal G, Hartelius L. Comorbidity in severe developmental language disorders: neuropediatric and psychological considerations. Acta Paediatrica 2005; 94: 471–8
  • Sillanpää M. Epilepsy in children: prevalence, disability, and handicap. Epilepsia. 1992; 33: 444–9
  • Metz-Lutz M, Fillipini M. Neuropsychological findings in Rolandic epilepsy and Landau Kleffner syndrome. Epilepsia 2006; 47(Suppl 2)71–5
  • Riva D, Vago C, Franceschetti S, Pantaleoni C, D'Arrigo S, Granata T, Bulgheroni S. Intellectual and language findings and their relationship to EEG characteristics in benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Epilepsy Behav 2007; 10: 278–85
  • Parkinson GM. High incidence of language disorder in children with focal epilepsies. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002; 44: 533–7
  • Cohen H, Le Normand M-T. Langugage development in children with simple-partial left-hemisphere epilepsy. Brain Lang 1998; 64: 409–23
  • Rejnö-Habte Selassie G, Viggedal G, Olsson I, Jennische M. Speech, language and cognition in preschool children with epilepsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 2008; 50: 432–8
  • Holmberg, E, Sahlén, B. Nelli: Neurolinguistic assessment battery for language disordered children. Malmö: Utbildningsproduktion; 1986.
  • Jennische M, Sedin G. Assessment of speech and language skills in children. Ups J Med Sci 1993; 97: 229–50
  • Kirk, SA, McCarthy, JJ, Kirk, WD. ITPA: Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. 4th rev. ed. 1968 ( Swedish version: Holmberg B. Stockholm: Psykologiförl; 2003).
  • Bishop, DVM. Test for reception of grammar. ( Swedish version: Holmberg E, Lundäiv E. Göteborg: SIH läromedel 1998).
  • Dunn, L, Dunn, L. PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997 ( Swedish version: Furberg À, Gustavsson L, Lundaidv E. Go˘teborg: SIH läromedel; 2001).
  • Eklund, H. Ordracet (The Word Race). In:. K Sjöndin, Svensson, L. Rapid confrontation naming: Swedish assessment material for word retrieval. Normative data and evaluation [Bachelor's thesis]. GöteborgSweden: Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg; 1996
  • Bishop DVM. Development of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39: 879–91
  • Hugdahl, K, Engstrand, O, Nordstrand, L. OEA graphicinteractive CAD system for dichotic stimulus alignment (CADDIC). Reports from the Department of Psychology, University of Bergen, Norway. 1986;7:1–19.
  • Svoboda, WB. Childhood epilepsy, language, learning, and emotional complications. Cambridge, UK New York: Cambridge University Press; 2004.
  • Smith L, Reilly S, Parry-Fielder B, Anderson V, Block S, McKay M, et al. Exploring the relationship between epilepsy and stuttering: an EEG investigation. Stem-, Spraak- en Taalpatologie, Nijmegen University Press, Netherlands 2006;14, (Suppl):. 107.
  • Tütüncüoglu S, Serdaroglu G, Kadioglu B. Landau Kleffner syndrome beginning with stuttering: case report. J Child Neurol 2002; 17: 785–8
  • Conti-Ramsden G, Hesketh A. Risk markers for SLI: a study of young language learning children. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2003; 38: 251–63
  • Stores G, Hart JA. Proceedings: reading skills of children with generalized and focal epilepsy attending ordinary school. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1975; 39: 429–30
  • Papavasilou A, Mattheou D, Bazigou H, Kotsalis C, Paraskevoulakos E. Written language skills in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Epilepsy Behav 2005; 6: 50–8
  • Chaix Y, Laguitton V, Lauwers-Cansès V, Daquin G, Cancès C, Démonet J-F, et al. Reading abilities and cognitive functions of children with epilepsy: Influence of epileptic syndrome. Brain Dev 2005; 28: 122–30
  • Hugdahl K, Andersson L, Asbjörnsen A, Dalen K. Dichotic listening, forced attention and brain asymmetry in righthanded and lefthanded children. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1990; 12: 539–48
  • Pisano T, Marini C, Brovedani P, Brizzolara D, Pruna D, Mei D, et al. Abnormal phonologic processing in familial lateral temporal lobe epilepsy due to a new LGI1 mutation. Epilepsia 2005; 46: 118–23
  • Pecini C, Casalini C, Brizzolara D, Cipriani P, Pfanner L, Chilosi A. Hemispheric specialization for language in children with different types of specific language impairment. Cortex 2005; 41: 157–67

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.