216
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric

ABSTRACT

The association between argumentativeness and extraversion, while well established in the United States, has not been examined cross-culturally. Therefore, this study conducts a cross-cultural comparison of this association with nationally representative samples from Australia, Canada, New Zealand, and the United States. The results confirm previous research in the U.S. and showed U.S. participants scored highest on tendency to approach arguments and extraversion, and lowest on tendency to avoid arguments, while New Zealanders scored the lowest on tendency to approach arguments and extraversion. These results expand our understanding of argumentativeness, illustrating the positive association between argumentativeness and extraversion is not a uniquely U.S. phenomenon, and expanding our understanding of argumentativeness to the Canadian context. The differences found between countries suggest future research explore how other factors might explain these differences.

Introduction

Argumentativeness is among the most widely studied constructs in communication. Argumentativeness is a relatively stable trait that “predisposes an individual in communication situation(s) to advocate positions on controversial issues and to attack verbally the positions which other people take on these issues” (Infante & Rancer, Citation1982, p. 72)., Argumentativeness has been associated with many communication constructs, such as verbal aggression (Infante et al., Citation2011; Kotowski et al., Citation2009), organizational dissent (Croucher et al., Citation2009), communication apprehension (Howe & Cionea, Citation2021), conflict (Hample & Anagondahalli, Citation2015), and romantic attachment (Weger, Citation2006). The volume of literature about argumentativeness suggests it is a critical field of research (Rancer & Avtgis, Citation2014).

The argumentativeness construct has been largely explored among U.S. based samples. Non-U.S. based research into argumentativeness has been conducted in Australia (Avtgis & Rancer, Citation2002), China (Rapanta & Hample, Citation2015), France (Croucher et al., Citation2018), Germany (Croucher et al., Citation2021), India (Hample & Anagondahalli, Citation2015), Japan (Suzuki & Rancer, Citation1994), Kyrgyzstan (Subanaliev et al., Citation2018), Nicaragua (Croucher et al., Citation2021), Portugal (Lewiński et al., Citation2018), New Zealand (Hackman et al., Citation1995), the Philippines (Croucher et al., Citation2021), Turkey (Croucher et al., Citation2013), United Arab Emirates (Rapanta & Hample, Citation2015), and the United Kingdom (Croucher et al., Citation2021).

One variable that has consistently shown to predict argumentativeness is extraversion. Extraversion is a trait generally associated with being active, assertive, carefree, dominant, adventurous, sociable, sensation-seeking, and friendly (Eysenck & Eysenck, Citation1963). Extraversion is one of the primary traits, alongside introversion and psychoticism, often associated with Eysenck and Eysenck’s “Big Three” personality traits model. Infante (Citation1987) identified argumentativeness as part of the assertiveness facet of extraversion and Gronostay (Citation2019) explained the active nature of extraverts leads them to be more likely to approach arguments. Research exploring the link between argumentativeness and extraversion has consistently found a positive relationship between extraversion and levels of argumentativeness (Blickle, Citation1995, Citation1997; T. R. Levine & McCornack, Citation1991; McCroskey et al., Citation2001; Tohver, Citation2020). While initially explored in a U.S. context, there is an extensive volume of cross-cultural literature exploring extraversion. Researchers have explored extraversion in more than 70 national contexts, including nations in South America, Africa, Europe, Asia, Oceania, the Middle East, and in former Soviet states (see Lucas et al., Citation2000; Lynn & Martin, Citation1997; Rojas-Méndez & Hine, Citation2017).

The relationship between extraversion and argumentativeness has not yet been explored cross-culturally or in a non-U.S. context. Thus, this study investigated the association between argumentativeness in non-U.S. settings and compared those results with findings from studies conducted in the United States. Specifically, three national contexts were chosen to compare with the U.S.: Canada, Australia, and New Zealand. These four nations were chosen for the following reasons: first, it is critical to compare more than two groups to systematically document cultural influences (Ones et al., Citation2012). Second, these four nations share similar cultural elements that reduce extraneous variables; for example, all four nations are predominantly English-speaking and are economically part of the Global North.

Studies have drawn comparisons among the U.S., Australia, and New Zealand on argumentativeness. Specifically, researchers have found when compared to East-Asian nations, participants from the U.S., Australia, and New Zealand scored higher on argumentativeness (Avtgis & Rancer, Citation2002), with U.S. participants scoring the highest. When compared to U.S. and Australian participants, New Zealand participants score lower, as New Zealanders have been found to have a higher tendency for self-monitoring (Hackman et al., Citation1995). Research has not explored argumentativeness in a Canadian context.

Researchers have extensively explored extraversion and how it differs among the U.S., Australia, Canada, and New Zealand. The consensus has been U.S. participants score higher in levels of extraversion than their Australian, Canadian, and New Zealander counterparts (Lucas et al., Citation2000; Lynn & Martin, Citation1997; Vaughan & Cattell, Citation1976). Previous research linking argumentativeness and extraversion has shown a positive association between tendency to approach argument and extraversion, and a negative relationship between extraversion and tendency to avoid argument (Blickle, Citation1995, Citation1997; T. R. Levine & McCornack, Citation1991; McCroskey et al., Citation2001; Tohver, Citation2020). Thus, we propose the following:

H:

There will be a positive relationship between extraversion and tendency to approach argument and a negative relationship between extraversion and tendency to avoid argument in the United States.

As there is scant research linking argumentativeness and extraversion in a non-U.S. setting, we pose the following research question:

RQ1:

What will be the relationship between extraversion and argumentativeness in Australia, Canada, and New Zealand?

Finally, we seek to compare the results among these samples, and therefore pose the second research question:

RQ2:

How will the relationship between argumentativeness and extraversion differ between Australia, Canada, New Zealand, and the United States?

Method

Participants and procedures

After obtaining ethical approval in 2023, nationally representative samples were collected in Australia (n = 212), Canada (n = 425), New Zealand (n = 453), and the U.S. (n = 879) using Qualtrics. Qualtrics provided financial incentive ($5USD) for participants to complete the survey. All participants were native English speakers and native-born to their respective nation. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted on all data to determine validity following criteria set by Hu and Bentler (Citation1999). Verifying factor structures of a measure is essential when using it in a context for which it was not developed (Croucher & Kelly, Citation2019). As both measures were developed in a U.S. context, it is possible some items may not be culturally relevant (Croucher & Kelly, Citation2019). The AMOS maximum likelihood parameter estimation algorithm was used to create a priori correlation matrices to compare against observed matrices to identify problematic or weak items. Items making up each measure were examined for residual error. Those items causing a significant amount of residual error were systematically removed. Acceptable fit indices were: goodness of fit index (GFI) ≥ .90, a comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ .90, a standard root mean residual (SRMR) ≤ .08, and root mean square error approximation (RMSEA) ≤ .10. Such modification or removal of problematic items is acceptable and recommended (T. Levine et al., Citation2006). “If the factor structure does not fit a particular data set and modifications are made, then one has enhanced the measurement validity within a particular study” (T. Levine et al., Citation2006, p. 313)., provides demographic information.

Table 1. Participant demographics.

Instruments

Argumentativeness Scale

The Argumentativeness Scale is a 20-item scale measuring tendency to avoid and approach argument (Infante & Rancer, Citation1982). A sample item is, “I enjoy defending my point of view on an issue.” Items are rated from 1 (almost never true) to 5 (almost always true). While some research has argued the argumentativeness construct is unidimensional (Kotowski et al., Citation2009), we retained the two factors of avoid and approach, recognizing the arguments of Infante et al. (Citation2011) and for cross-national comparability Hample (Citation2018) and Mamberti and Hample (Citation2022).Footnote1

Eysenck Personality Questionnaire

The 24-item Revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQR) (Francis et al., Citation1992) was used. This scale measures extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism, and tendency to lie. In the current study, only extraversion is included. Each item is scored either yes (0) or no (1). A sample item is, “Are you a worrier?”Footnote2 Means, standard deviations, and correlations for all variables are presented in .

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and correlations for study variables.

Results

Means were compared using analysis of variance (ANOVA) (see for full results). Results revealed significant differences between the sample nations on argumentativeness and extraversion. The U.S. and Canada scored the highest on tendency to approach argument; New Zealand scored the lowest, F(1965) = 84.21, p < .001, η2 = .11. Inversely, New Zealand scored highest on tendency to avoid argument, while the U.S. and Canada scored the lowest, F(1965) = 53.92, p < .001, η2 = .08. The U.S. scored highest on extraversion and New Zealand scored the lowest, F(1965) = 51.51, p < .001, η2 = .07.

The hypothesis predicted a positive relationship between extraversion and tendency to approach argument, and a negative relationship between extraversion and tendency to avoid argument in the United States. To test this hypothesis and answer RQ1, Pearson’s correlation analysis was conducted. The hypothesis was supported (r = .55, p < .001, r = −.29, p < .001). In answering RQ1, correlation analysis revealed tendency to approach argument and extraversion were significantly positively correlated in Australia (r = .40) and New Zealand (r = .27). Extraversion and tendency to avoid argument were significantly negatively correlated in New Zealand (r = −.16) and Australia (r = −.13), but not significantly correlated in Canada (r = .02).

RQ2 asked how the relationships between argumentativeness and extraversion differ among the U.S., Australia, Canada, and New Zealand. Fisher’s z comparisons were conducted to establish whether differences in correlations between samples were significant. In terms of extraversion and ApproachARG, correlations between the U.S. and New Zealand (z = 12.24, p < .01), the U.S and Canada (z = 20.16, p < .001), the U.S. and Australia (z = 6.43, p < .01), Canada and Australia (z = 9.17, p < .01), New Zealand and Canada (z = 6.42, p < .01), New Zealand and Australia (z = 3.70, p = < .05) were significantly different. Thus, based on these results, the highest positive correlations are in the U.S. and Australia. In terms of extraversion and AvoidARG, correlations between the U.S. and Australia (z = 5.54, p < .05), the U.S. and Canada (z = 10.06, p < .01), and the U.S. and New Zealand (z = 5.03, p < .05), were significantly different, while correlations between Canada and Australia (z = 2.80, p = ns), New Zealand and Canada (z = 4.17, p = ns), New Zealand and Australia (z = .76, p = ns) were not significantly different. Thus, based on our results, the highest negative correlations are in the U.S. and New Zealand.

Discussion

Dilbeck et al. (Citation2009) asserted communication in different national contexts should be explored to fully understand the complexity of communication. The current study has done this by expanding our understanding of the link between argumentativeness and extraversion in four national contexts. The following implications can be drawn from this study: first, researchers in communication are increasingly calling for replication studies (Autman & Kelly, Citation2017). Replication helps remove rival hypotheses and diminish the influence of convenience samples, while serving as validity and reliability checks of measures (Kelly & Westerman, Citation2020). This study replicates previous work showing a positive association between argumentativeness and extraversion in the U.S., which should increase our confidence in this relationship. This study also adds to the discussion on the dimensionality and validity of the Argumentative Scale (Infante et al., Citation2011).

Second, this study has cross-culturally expanded understanding of argumentativeness and its relationship with extraversion. Previous research has shown U.S. participants score higher on argumentativeness than Australians and New Zealanders (Avtgis & Rancer, Citation2002). The results of this study confirm previous results; U.S. participants scored the highest on tendency to approach argument and extraversion, while New Zealanders scored the lowest on tendency to approach, highest on tendency avoid, and lowest on extraversion. As with the work of Hackman et al. (Citation1995), the lower score from New Zealanders on tendency to approach argument may be attributed to a higher level of self-monitoring in New Zealand, compared to the U.S. and Australia. Moreover, research has yet to explore argumentativeness in the Canadian context; thus, this study adds to our understanding of argumentativeness in this context. Additionally, while research has consistently shown a positive association between tendency to approach argument and extraversion, this research has only been conducted in a U.S. context. The current research demonstrates this relationship is not uniquely found among U.S. samples.

Third, building off recommendations from Ones et al. (Citation2012) and Croucher and Kelly (Citation2019), the current study compared four national groups that are relatively similar in terms of economic and linguistic systems. Based on these methodological reasons for cross-cultural comparison, it is clear there are differences among the nations in terms of argumentativeness and extraversion, which cannot be explained by traditional comparative means, such as Hofstede’s dimensions (Citation2001). Based on Hofstede’s dimensions, each of these nations score relatively similarly. Thus, future research should explore how other factors might explain these differences, such as regionalism, geography, communication skills training, and use of technology.

This study is not without limitation, which does limit its generalizability. The samples only included native-born individuals in each nation. As each nation has large immigrant populations, generalizability of the findings can only be applied to native-born individuals. Future research should include non-native-born individuals to generate a robust understanding of each nation.

This study is the first to cross-culturally explore the association between argumentativeness and extraversion. The current study not only replicated this association in the U.S., Canada, Australia, and New Zealand, but expanded our understanding of argumentativeness to a Canadian context. This study is a step forward in our overall understanding of argumentativeness and extraversion.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Massey University.

Notes on contributors

Stephen M Croucher

Stephen M Croucher is a Professor and Head of School of Communication, Journalism and Marketing at Massey University. He received his PhD at the University of Oklahoma.

Tommy Yotes

Tommy Yotes is a Doctoral Candidate in the School of Communication, Journalism and Marketing at Massey University.

Doug Ashwell

Doug Ashwell is a Senior Lecturer and Associate Head of School of Communication, Journalism and Marketing at Massey University. He received his PhD at Massey University.

Shawn M Condon

Shawn M Condon is a Senior Tutor in the School of Communication, Journalism and Marketing at Massey University.

Notes

1. CFA results for Argumentativeness Scale – Deleted items = 1, 8, 13, and 16. χ2(103) = 1229.97, p < .0001, CFI = .92, GFI = .94, SRMR .08, RMSEA = .08. Approach α = .92, φ = .92; Avoid α = .87, φ = .87

2. CFA results for EPQR – Deleted items = 1, 9, 11, 15, 16, 20, 22, 23, and 24. - χ2(59) = 771.19, p < .0001, CFI = .95, GFI = .93, SRMR .08, RMSEA = .08. Extraversion α = .72, φ = .72; Neuroticism α = .84, φ = .84; Tendency to Lie α = .88, φ = .88; and Psychoticism α = .70, φ = .70.

References

  • Autman, H., & Kelly, S. (2017). Reexamining the writing apprehension measure. Business and Professional Communication Quarterly, 80(4), 516–529. https://doi.org/10.1177/2329490617691968
  • Avtgis, T. A., & Rancer, A. S. (2002). Aggressive communication across cultures: A comparison of aggressive communication among United States, New Zealand, and Australia. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 31(3), 191–200.
  • Blickle, G. (1995). Conceptualization and measurement of argumentativeness: A decade later. Psychological Reports, 77(1), 99–110. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.1.99
  • Blickle, G. (1997). Argumentativeness and the facets of the big five. Psychological Reports, 81(3_suppl), 1379–1385. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1997.81.3f.1379
  • Croucher, S. M., Braziunaite, R., Homsey, D., Pillai, G., Saxena, J., Saldanha, A., Joshi, V., Jafri, I., Choudhary, P., Bose, L., & Agarwal, K. (2009). Organizational dissent and argumentativeness: A comparative analysis between American and Indian organizations. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 38(3), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.1080/17475759.2009.505057
  • Croucher, S. M., & Kelly, S. (2019). Measurement in intercultural and cross-cultural communication. In E. E. Graham & J. P. Mazer (Eds.), Communication research measures III: A sourcebook (pp. 141–159). Routledge.
  • Croucher, S. M., Kelly, S., Burkey, M., Spencer, A., Gomez, O., Del Villar, C., & Eskiçorapçi, N. (2021). A multi-national validity analysis of the argumentativeness measure. International Journal of Conflict Management, 32(1), 88–101. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCMA-02-2020-0027
  • Croucher, S. M., Kelly, S., Condon, S. M., Campbell, E., Galy-Badenas, F., Rahmani, D., Nshom, E., & Zeng, C. (2018). A longitudinal analysis of the relationship between cultural adaptation and argumentativeness. International Journal of Conflict Management, 29(1), 91–108. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCMA-05-2017-0045
  • Croucher, S. M., Otten, R., Ball, M., Grimes, T., Ainsworth, B., Begley, K., & Corzo, L. (2013). Argumentativeness and political participation: A cross-cultural analysis in the United States and Turkey. Communication Studies, 64(1), 18–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/10510974.2012.727942
  • Dilbeck, K. E., McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, L. L. (2009). Self-perceived communication competence in the Thai culture. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 38(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/17475750903381598
  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1963). The Eysenck personality inventory. Educational & Industrial Testing Service.
  • Francis, L. J. Brown, L. B. & Philipchalk, R.(1992). The development of an abbreviated form of the revised eysenck personality questionnaire (EPQR-A): Its use among students in England, Canada, the U.S.A. and Australia. Personality & Individual Differences, 13(4), 443–449.
  • Gronostay, D. (2019). Wer diskutiert mit? Die Bedeutung von Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen, Argumentierfreude, epistemologischen Überzeugungen und zugewiesenen Diskussionspositionen für die Beteiligung an kontroversen politischen Unterrichtsdiskussionen. Unterrichtswiss, 47(1), 117–135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42010-018-00033-4
  • Hackman, M. Z., Johnson, C. E., & Barthel-Hackman, T. (1995). Correlates of talkaholism in New Zealand: An intracultural analysis of the compulsive communication construct. Communication Research Reports, 12(1), 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824099509362039
  • Hample, D. (2018). Interpersonal arguing. Peter Lang.
  • Hample, D., & Anagondahalli, D. (2015). Understandings of arguing in India and the United States: Argument frames, personalization of conflict, argumentativeness, and verbal aggressiveness. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 44(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/17475759.2014.1000939
  • Hofstede, G.(2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Sage.
  • Howe, W. T., & Cionea, I. A. (2021). Exploring the assumptions between debate participation, communication competence, communication apprehension, and argumentativeness with a global sample. Argumentation & Advocacy, 57(2), 103–122. https://doi.org/10.1080/10511431.2021.1897274
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Infante, D. A. (1987). Aggressive. In J. C. McCroskey & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Personality and interpersonal communication (pp. 157–192). Sage.
  • Infante, D. A., & Rancer, A. S. (1982). A conceptualization and measure of argumentativeness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46(1), 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4601_13
  • Infante, D. A., Rancer, A. S., & Wigley, C. J. (2011). In defense of the argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness scales. Communication Quarterly, 59(2), 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2011.563439
  • Kelly, S., & Westerman, D. (2020). Doing communication science: Thoughts on making more valid claims. Annals of the International Communication Association, 44(3), 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2020.1792789
  • Kotowski, M. R., Levine, T. R., Baker, C. R., & Bolt, J. M. (2009). A multitrait-multimethod validity assessment of the verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness scales. Communication Monographs, 46(4), 443–462. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750903300247
  • Levine, T., Hullett, C. R., Mitchell Turner, M., & Knight Lapinski, M. (2006). The desirability of using confirmatory factor analysis on published scales. Communication Research Reports, 23(4), 309–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824090600962698
  • Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A. (1991). The dark side of trust: Conceptualizing and measuring types of communicative suspicion. Communication Quarterly, 39(4), 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463379109369809
  • Lewiński, M., Hample, D., Sàágua, J., & Mohammed, D. (2018). Arguing in Portugal: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of International & Intercultural Communication, 11(3), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/17513057.2018.1450888
  • Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Sug, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 452–468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.452
  • Lynn, R., & Martin, T. (1997). Gender differences in extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in 37 nations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137(3), 369–373. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549709595447
  • Mamberti, J., & Hample, D. (2022). Interpersonal arguing in Argentina. Argumentation & Advocacy, 58(3), 214–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/10511431.2022.2137984
  • McCroskey, J., Heisel, A., & Richmond, V. (2001). Eysenck’s BIG THREE and communication traits: Three correlational studies. Communication Monographs, 68(4), 360–366. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750128068
  • Ones, D. S., Dilchert, S., Deller, J., Albrecht, A.-G., Duehr, E. E., & Paulus, F. M. (2012). Cross-cultural generalization: Using meta-analysis to test hypotheses about cultural variability. In A. M. Ryan, F. T. L. Leong, & F. L. Oswald (Eds.), Conducting multinational research: Applying organizational psychology in the workplace (pp. 91–122). American Psychological Association.
  • Rancer, A. S., & Avtgis, T. Ä. (2014). Argumentativeness and aggressive communication. Theory, research, and application (2nd ed.). Peter Lang.
  • Rapanta, C., & Hample, D. (2015). Orientations to interpersonal arguing in the United Arab Emirates, with comparisons to the United States, China, and India. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 44(4), 263–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/17475759.2015.1081392
  • Rojas-Méndez, J. I., & Hine, M. J. (2017). Countries’ positioning on personality traits: Analysis of 10 South American national tourism websites. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 23(3), 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1177/1356766716649227
  • Subanaliev, T., Croucher, S. M., Turdabayeva, E., & Kelly, S. (2018). An exploratory analysis of argumentativeness in Kyrgyzstan. Russian Journal of Communication, 10(1), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/19409419.2017.1399084
  • Suzuki, S., & Rancer, A. S. (1994). Argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness: Testing for conceptual and measurement equivalence across cultures. Communication Monographs, 61(3), 256–279. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759409376336
  • Tohver, G. C. (2020). Eysenck giant three. In B. J. Carducci & C. S. Nave (Eds.), The Wiley encyclopedia of personality and individual differences: Personality processes and individual differences (pp. 155–159). Hoboken, NJ.
  • Vaughan, G. M., & Cattell, R. B. (1976). Personality differences between young new Zealanders and Americans. The Journal of Social Psychology, 99(1), 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1976.9924741
  • Weger, H., Jr. (2006). Associations among romantic attachment, argumentativeness, and verbal aggressiveness in romantic relationships. Argumentation & Advocacy, 43(1), 29–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/00028533.2006.11821660