1,990
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Understanding how unfamiliar faces become familiar: Introduction to a special issue on face learning

&

The identity information that is carried by faces allows us to recognize the people around us, thereby providing fundamental structure to our interpersonal interactions. In the eye of the beholder, this facial identity information is carried on a continuum (see Clutterbuck & Johnston, Citation2002, Citation2004). At one end of this continuum are the faces of unfamiliar people, whom we have seen only briefly—perhaps in only a single photograph. At the other end of this continuum lies the recognition of highly familiar people, such as family, friends, and colleagues, or famous people to whose faces we are exposed extensively through various media. These familiar faces were, of course, at some point also unfamiliar to an observer. And, as the people who are familiar to one person are inevitably unfamiliar to someone else, both familiar and unfamiliar face recognition can be performed on exactly the same visual stimuli (see, e.g., Armann, Jenkins, & Burton, Citation2016; Ritchie et al., Citation2015). In this sense, familiar and unfamiliar face processing are clearly linked. However, increasing familiarity with a face exerts a transformational effect on the cognitive representations that underpin this process. As a consequence, the identification of unfamiliar and familiar faces is characterized by different properties (for reviews, see Hancock, Bruce, & Burton, Citation2000; Johnston & Edmonds, Citation2009).

To illustrate, familiar faces can be recognized quickly and accurately (see, e.g., Bruce, Carson, Burton, & Kelly, Citation1998; Bruce & Valentine, Citation1985), and over intervals of many years (Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittlinger, Citation1975). They can also be recognized without conscious awareness (Morrison, Bruce, & Burton, Citation2000) or explicit memory (Jenkins, Burton, & Ellis, Citation2002), in the visual periphery (Bindemann, Burton, & Jenkins, Citation2005; Bindemann, Jenkins, & Burton, Citation2007), and from partial (Brunas, Young, & Ellis, Citation1990; Johnston, Barry, & Williams, Citation1996), degraded (Demanet, Dhont, Notebaert, Pattyn, & Vandierendonck, Citation2007; Lander, Bruce, & Hill, Citation2001), and distorted images (Bindemann, Burton, Leuthold, & Schweinberger, Citation2008; Hole, George, Eaves, & Rasek, Citation2002). The sum of evidence therefore suggests that the recognition of familiar faces is remarkably robust, even under challenging conditions.

By contrast, unfamiliar face identification is highly error-prone, even under seemingly good conditions. When observers are asked to identify a target face out of a concurrent lineup of ten possible matches, accuracy is typically at approximately 70% (Bruce et al., Citation1999; Bruce, Henderson, Newman, & Burton, Citation2001). Performance remains error-prone when this task is reduced to a simple pairwise comparison, in which observers decide whether two faces side-by-side depict the same person or different people (e.g., Bindemann, Avetisyan, & Rakow, Citation2012; Burton, White, & McNeill, Citation2010). This pattern is observed with high-quality images that depict the compared faces in the same frontal view, with a neutral expression, and under good lighting. Moreover, this difficulty is not restricted to photographs, but persists when observers match a person seen live to a face photograph (Kemp, Towell, & Pike, Citation1997; Megreya & Burton, Citation2008; White, Kemp, Jenkins, Matheson, & Burton, Citation2014) or moving video images (Davis & Valentine, Citation2009). Thus, unfamiliar face identification appears to be difficult even under optimized conditions.

These differences between familiar and unfamiliar face processing are striking, and the process of the transition from an unfamiliar face to a familiar one—how it is learned—is the topic of this special issue. Understanding face learning requires insight into the nature of the changes that cognitive representations undergo as faces progress along the familiarity continuum. Observers’ initial cognitive representations of unfamiliar faces often represent only a “snapshot”, or visual pattern, that is restrained by the limited experience with a new face (Longmore, Liu, & Young, Citation2008; Megreya & Burton, Citation2006). With increasing exposure to a person across different views, lighting conditions, emotional expressions, and so forth, observers can extract more information about their facial appearance. This must include the stable identity-defining characteristics of a face that are shared across different encounters (Burton, Jenkins, Hancock, & White, Citation2005; Jenkins & Burton, Citation2011), but also how a person can vary in appearance (Burton, Kramer, Ritchie, & Jenkins, Citation2016; Jenkins, White, Van Montfort, & Burton, Citation2011). This information must then be applied, in turn, to disentangle which aspects of an incoming face stimulus reflect identity information and which are more reflective of the conditions under which a person is encountered. A key attribute of this process must be that a cognitive identity representation emerges with increasing familiarity that is generalizable across many different encounters with a person (Burton et al., Citation2005; Jenkins & Burton, Citation2011).

This transition from image-bound to stable and generalizable face representations is addressed in several papers in this special issue. Longmore et al. (Citation2017) explore observers’ ability to generalize recognition to novel images of a learned face by manipulating apparent age. Etchells, Brooks, and Johnston (Citation2017) also focus on generalization by studying the recognition of newly learned faces across different views. One important aspect of these studies is that initial exposure to a face and its subsequent recognition is assessed across different exemplars. While the other-race effect in face recognition has been researched extensively, studies that contrast learning of same- and other-race faces across such different exemplars are limited. In this special issue, Hayward, Favelle, Oxner, Chu, and Lam (Citation2017) provide such a demonstration, across naturalistic images that were taken from Facebook photo albums.

Whereas these three papers focus on face recognition across different exemplars, other reports in this special issue examine the benefit of providing such variability at the learning stage. Ritchie and Burton (Citation2017) investigate whether exposure to sets of images that depict people in conditions of high variability, by providing variation in lightning, head angle, expression, and age, facilitates face learning compared to image sets in which a person’s appearance does not vary as greatly. Jones, Dwyer, and Lewis (Citation2017) provide an interesting extension of this research, by exploring whether computer-generated views can provide additional images for face learning when multiple naturalistic photographs of a person are not available. Butcher and Lander’s (Citation2017) research report then provides insight into whether a similar effect is apparent for familiar faces, by investigating correlations between the amount and distinctiveness of faces’ motion and their recognition.

The remaining articles in this special issue focus on a variety of aspects of face learning. Millen, Hope, Hillstrom, and Vrij (Citation2017) use eye-tracking to examine familiarity, by comparing eye movements to newly learned, famous, and personally known faces. A specific aim here is to determine whether eye movements can expose deception, by revealing memory of a face even when observers are overtly lying about its recognition. Estudillo and Bindemann (Citation2017) then use eye movements to examine how observers might update representations of their own faces. This is explored with a gaze-contingent procedure in which an onscreen face mimics changes in observers’ eye direction to create an effect akin to looking at oneself in a mirror.

We complete this special issue by focusing on our earliest learning experiences of faces, in infants that are only 1 and 3 months old. Sugden and Moulson (Citation2017) explore this issue with a neat procedure, in which infants wear head-mounted video cameras to capture their perspective of faces in the visual field. This footage is then examined to determine how frequently faces are seen alone and up close in the visual field, and in frontal and upright views. Webb, Neuhaus, and Faja (Citation2017) close this special issue with a review of face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. This review is wonderfully structured to compare typical and atypical observers in early development, childhood, adolescence and adulthood, and provides an insightful breakdown of face processing into attention, perception, and learning and memory.

References

  • Armann, R. G. M., Jenkins, R., & Burton, A. M. (2016). A familiarity disadvantage for remembering specific images of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 571–580. doi:10.1037/xhp0000174
  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittlinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 54–75. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.104.1.54
  • Bindemann, M., Avetisyan, M., & Rakow, T. (2012). Who can recognize unfamiliar faces? Individual differences and observer consistency in person identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 277–291. doi:10.1037/a0029635
  • Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., & Jenkins, R. (2005). Capacity limits for face processing. Cognition, 98, 177–197. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.004
  • Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., Leuthold, H., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Brain potential correlates of face recognition: Geometric distortions and the N250r brain response to stimulus repetitions. Psychophysiology, 45, 535–544. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00663.x
  • Bindemann, M., Jenkins, R., & Burton, A. M. (2007). A bottleneck in face identification: Repetition priming from flanker faces. Experimental Psychology, 54, 192–201. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.3.192
  • Bruce, V., Carson, D., Burton, A. M., & Kelly, S. (1998). Prime time advertisements: Repetition priming from faces seen on subject recruitment posters. Memory & Cognition, 26, 502–515. doi:10.3758/BF03201159
  • Bruce, V., Henderson, Z., Greenwood, K., Hancock, P. J. B., Burton, A. M., & Miller, P. (1999). Verification of face identities from images captured on video. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 339–360. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.5.4.339
  • Bruce, V., Henderson, Z., Newman, C., & Burton, A. M. (2001). Matching identities of familiar and unfamiliar faces caught on CCTV images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 207–218. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.7.3.207
  • Bruce, V., & Valentine, T. (1985). Identity priming in the recognition of familiar faces. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 373–383. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01960.x
  • Brunas, J., Young, A. W., & Ellis, A. W. (1990). Repetition priming from incomplete faces: Evidence for part to whole completion. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 43–56. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02344.x
  • Burton, A. M., Jenkins, R., Hancock, P. J. B., & White, D. (2005). Robust representations for face recognition: The power of averages. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 256–284. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.06.003
  • Burton, A. M., Kramer, R. S. S., Ritchie, K. L., & Jenkins, R. (2016). Identity from variation: Representations of faces derived from multiple instances. Cognitive Science, 40, 202–223. doi:10.1111/cogs.12231
  • Burton, A. M., White, D., & McNeill, A. (2010). The Glasgow Face Matching test. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 286–291. doi:10.3758/brm.42.1.286
  • Butcher, N., & Lander, K. (2017). Exploring the motion advantage: Evaluating the contribution of familiarity and differences in facial motion. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1138974
  • Clutterbuck, R., & Johnston, R. A. (2002). Exploring levels of face familiarity by using an indirect face-matching measure. Perception, 31, 985–994. doi:10.1068/p3335
  • Clutterbuck, R., & Johnston, R. A. (2004). Demonstrating the acquired familiarity of faces by using a gender-decision task. Perception, 33, 159–168. doi:10.1068/p5115
  • Davis, J. P., & Valentine, T. (2009). CCTV on trial: Matching video images with the defendant in the dock. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 482–505. doi:10.1002/acp.1490
  • Demanet, J., Dhont, K., Notebaert, L., Pattyn, S., & Vandierendonck, A. (2007). Pixelating familiar people in the media: Should masking be taken at face value? Psychologica Belgica, 47, 261–276. doi:10.5334/pb-47-4-261
  • Estudillo, A. J., & Bindemann, M. (2017). Can gaze-contingent mirror-feedback from unfamiliar faces alter self-recognition? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1166253
  • Etchells, D. B., Brooks, J. L., & Johnston, R. A. (2017). Evidence for view-invariant face recognition units in unfamiliar face learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1248453
  • Hancock, P. J. B., Bruce, V., & Burton, A. M. (2000). Recognition of unfamiliar faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 330–337. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01519-9
  • Hayward, W. G., Favelle, S. K., Oxner, M., Chu, M. H., & Lam, M. L. (2017). The other-race effect in face learning: Using naturalistic images to investigate face ethnicity effects in a learning paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1146781
  • Hole, G. J., George, P. A., Eaves, K., & Rasek, A. (2002). Effects of geometric distortions on face-recognition performance. Perception, 31, 1221–1240. doi:10.1068/p3252
  • Jenkins, R., & Burton, A. M. (2011). Stable face representations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1671–1683. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0379
  • Jenkins, R., Burton, A. M., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Long-term effects of covert face recognition. Cognition, 86, B43–B52. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00172-5
  • Jenkins, R., White, D., Van Montfort, X., & Burton, A. M. (2011). Variability in photos of the same face. Cognition, 121, 313–323. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.001
  • Johnston, R. A., Barry, C., & Williams, C. (1996). Incomplete faces don’t show the whole picture: Repetition priming from jumbled faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49, 596–615. doi:10.1080/027249896392513
  • Johnston, R. A., & Edmonds, A. J. (2009). Familiar and unfamiliar face recognition: A review. Memory, 17, 577–596. doi:10.1080/09658210902976969
  • Jones, S. P., Dwyer, D. M., & Lewis, M. B. (2017). The utility of multiple synthesized views in the recognition of unfamiliar faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1158302
  • Kemp, R., Towell, N., & Pike, G. (1997). When seeing should not be believing: Photographs, credit cards and fraud. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 211–222. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199706)11:3<211::AID-ACP430>3.0.CO;2-O
  • Lander, K., Bruce, V., & Hill, H. (2001). Evaluating the effectiveness of pixelation and blurring on masking the identity of familiar faces. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 101–116. doi:10.1002/1099-0720(200101/02)15:1<101::AID-ACP697>3.0.CO;2-7
  • Longmore, C. A., Liu, C. H., & Young, A. W. (2008). Learning faces from photographs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 77–100. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.77
  • Longmore, C. A., Santos, I. M., Silva, C. F., Hall, A., Faloyin, D., & Little, E. (2017). Image dependency in the recognition of newly learnt faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1236825
  • Megreya, A. M., & Burton, A. M. (2006). Unfamiliar faces are not faces: Evidence from a matching task. Memory & Cognition, 34, 865–876. doi:10.3758/BF03193433
  • Megreya, A. M., & Burton, A. M. (2008). Matching faces to photographs: Poor performance in eyewitness memory (without the memory). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 364–372. doi:10.1037/a0013464
  • Millen, A. E., Hope, L., Hillstrom, A. P., & Vrij, A. (2017). Tracking the truth: The effect of face familiarity on eye fixations during deception. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1172093
  • Morrison, D. J., Bruce, V., & Burton, A. M. (2000). Covert face recognition in neurologically intact participants. Psychological Research, 63, 83–94. doi:10.1007/s004260000037
  • Ritchie, K. L., & Burton, A. M. (2017). Learning faces from variability. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2015.1136656
  • Ritchie, K. L., Smith, F. G., Jenkins, R., Bindemann, M., White, D., & Burton, A. M. (2015). Viewers base estimates of face matching accuracy on their own familiarity: Explaining the photo-ID paradox. Cognition, 141, 161–169. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.002
  • Sugden, N. A., & Moulson, M. C. (2017). Hey baby, what’s “up”? One- and 3-month-olds experience faces primarily upright but non-upright faces offer the best views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1154581
  • Webb, S. J., Neuhaus, E., & Faja, S. (2017). Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059
  • White, D., Kemp, R. I., Jenkins, R., Matheson, M., & Burton, A. M. (2014). Passport officers’ errors in face matching. PloS One, 9(8), e103510. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0103510

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.