23,795
Views
40
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD: A Delphi consensus study

, , , &

ABSTRACT

Within the clinical evaluation of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), cognitive functions are often assessed to characterize individual cognitive problems and guide treatment planning. However, the composition of an adequate neuropsychological assessment battery remains difficult given the large number of cognitive functions that can be distinguished, the variety of available tools to assess these functions, as well as the cognitive heterogeneity that has been observed between individuals with ADHD. Twenty-seven clinicians and researchers experienced in working with adults with ADHD took part in the present study. This study employed the Delphi methodology in order to compose an assessment battery for the measurement of the most important neuropsychological functions by employing the most suitable measures to assess these functions. Consensus between experts was achieved on a ranking of 16 neuropsychological functions that are important for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. Furthermore, measures were suggested to assess each of the neuropsychological functions. The assessment battery as composed in the present study may support research and clinical practice in the selection of appropriate functions and measures for the determination of the individual level of cognitive functioning and treatment planning of adults with ADHD.

It is estimated that about 30–60% of children diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) still suffer from symptoms of ADHD in adulthood (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013; Barbaresi et al., Citation2013; Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, Citation2000; Mannuzza et al., Citation1991; Seidman, Valera, & Makris, Citation2005). The prevalence of adult ADHD is estimated to be around 3.4% worldwide (Fayyad et al., Citation2007), representing one of the most common psychiatric conditions in adulthood (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). One of the common features of ADHD are pronounced impairments in multiple aspects of cognition (Barkley, Murphy, & Fischer, Citation2007; Kooij et al., Citation2010). These cognitive impairments were shown to be associated with functional impairments in various domains, such as social functioning, academic achievement, occupational attainment, self-concept, as well as general well-being and quality of life (Agarwal, Goldenberg, Perry, & IsHak, Citation2012; Canu & Carlson, Citation2007; Diamantopoulou, Rydell, Thorell, & Bohlin, Citation2007; Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood, Citation1997; Kok, Groen, Fuermaier, & Tucha, Citation2016; Kooij et al., Citation2010).

Given the prominent role of cognitive impairments in adult ADHD, the clinical evaluation of adults with ADHD often comprise a neuropsychological assessment (Fuermaier et al., Citation2015; Hervey, Epstein, & Curry, Citation2004; Schoechlin & Engel, Citation2005; Seidman, Citation2006; Woods, Lovejoy, & Ball, Citation2002). Standardized neuropsychological assessment using psychometric test measures revealed significant group differences between adults with ADHD and healthy individuals in selective attention (L. Tucha et al., Citation2008), divided attention (Woods et al., Citation2002), as well as sustained attention/vigilance (Epstein, Johnson, Varia, & Conners, Citation2001; L. Tucha et al., Citation2017). Furthermore, adult ADHD has been associated with impairments in several aspects of executive control, such as working memory (Alderson, Kasper, Hudec, & Patros, Citation2013; Rohlf et al., Citation2012), planning and problem solving (L. Tucha et al., Citation2011), cognitive flexibility (Halleland, Haavik, & Lundervold, Citation2012), inhibition and impulse control (Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Buitelaar, Citation2005; Boonstra, Kooij, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Buitelaar, Citation2010), risky behavior (Groen, Gaastra, Lewis-Evans, & Tucha, Citation2013), time perception (Barkley, Murphy, & Bush, Citation2001), concept formation (Antshel et al., Citation2010) as well as verbal and design fluency (Boonstra et al., Citation2005; O. Tucha et al., Citation2005; L. Tucha et al., Citation2011). Meta-analyses revealed that inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility and fluency tasks were most robustly impaired in adults with ADHD with effect sizes reaching moderate size (Alderson et al., Citation2013; Boonstra et al., Citation2005; Hervey et al., Citation2004; Schoechlin & Engel, Citation2005). Furthermore, neuropsychological research on adults with ADHD also demonstrated small to medium impairments in several aspects of learning and memory (Fuermaier et al., Citation2016; Kaplan, Dewey, Crawford, & Fisher, Citation1998; Muir-Broaddus, Rosenstein, Medina, & Soderberg, Citation2002; Pollak, Kahana-Vax, & Hoofien, Citation2008; Seidman, Citation2006). Deficits occur particularly in those memory functions that strongly rely on intact executive control, which suggests an involvement of executive dysfunctions in disturbed memory functioning of adults with ADHD (Altgassen, Kretschmer, & Kliegel, Citation2014; Fuermaier, Tucha, Koerts, Aschenbrenner, Weisbrod, et al., Citation2013; Fuermaier, Tucha, Koerts, Aschenbrenner, Westermann, et al., Citation2013; Pollak et al., Citation2008; White & Marks, Citation2004).

Despite the clear evidence of neuropsychological impairment in adults with ADHD, the selection of neuropsychological functions and appropriate assessment tools in a given assessment situation often remains difficult due to several reasons. First, as presented above, neuropsychological impairments in adults with ADHD have been shown in multiple aspects of cognition. A complete assessment of all neuropsychological functions discussed is presumably not possible as such an assessment may considerably exceed the time constraints of any assessment situation. Second, neuropsychologists have numerous tests to choose from in composing an assessment battery. No clear agreement has been established yet on which tools are considered most sensitive and appropriate for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. For example, a review of Seidman (Citation2006) identified more than 70 tests that had been used to compare adults with ADHD with healthy individuals. However, the sensitivity of these tests cannot be determined as the majority of tests were used in one or two studies only. Third, while adult ADHD is clearly associated with cognitive impairment as indicated in group studies, not all patients show deficits on all neuropsychological tests applied (Mostert et al., Citation2015; Seidman, Citation2006; Thome et al., Citation2012). In this context, it has been noted that individuals with ADHD have unique profiles of neuropsychological functioning, with some patients showing impairments in one function while other patients displaying impairments in another function (Thome et al., Citation2012).

In order to address the heterogeneity of approaches as currently used for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD, the present study aims to achieve consensus among an international panel of experts from the field of adult ADHD by means of the Delphi methodology. The Delphi methodology is a structured communication technique, which can facilitate collective decision making in a structured and anonymous way (for a review, see Boulkedid, Abdoul, Loustau, Sibony, & Alberti (Citation2011)). The panel of experts of a Delphi study is requested to complete a series of questionnaires (so called ‘rounds’) interspersed with controlled feedback of the previous rounds at the beginning of each round (Dalkey & Helmer, Citation1963). The use of a Delphi method is of particular use when empirical studies on a certain question or issue are scarce or have not come to an agreement yet (Ziglio, Citation1996). A serial presentation of questionnaires, together with the presentation of group results of the previous round at the beginning of each round, allows the experts to modify their opinions with the aim of reaching group consensus (Sobaih, Ritchie, & Jones, Citation2012). Another strength of the Delphi method is that participation of the panel members is anonymous, which reduces group and/or peer pressure (Powell, Citation2003; Sobaih et al., Citation2012). As such, the influence of authority (e.g., based on the individual’s personality, age and status) on the consensus is minimized. In the present study, the Delphi method is used to achieve consensus on how to compose a neuropsychological assessment battery of adults with ADHD by identifying the most relevant neuropsychological functions as well as the most suitable measures to assess these functions. Such an assessment battery could be useful for a precise description of the cognitive problems in individual patients with ADHD and to guide further treatment planning. The relevance of this assessment battery for research and clinical practice will be discussed.

Method

Delphi methodology

The present study employed the Delphi method with the aim to collect and converge the opinions of experts in the field of adult ADHD. The principles of this method are based on the notion of collective wisdom in decision-making, assuming that the combined opinion of several people converges closer to the truth than the opinion of one individual (Habibi, Sarafrazi, & Izadyar, Citation2014). The Delphi method is designed in a way to minimize or eliminate the problems related to collective decision making in groups, such as (1) the influence of dominant or high-status members on other group members, (2) the group pressure to conform due to lack of anonymity, and (3) the necessity to be in one geographic location at the same time (Boulkedid et al., Citation2011; Dalkey & Helmer, Citation1963). This is achieved by a structured aggregation of independent, anonymous opinions of members of an expert panel by means of online questionnaires (Powell, Citation2003; Sobaih et al., Citation2012). The expert panel is requested to complete a series of questionnaires (in so-called rounds) interspersed with controlled feedback (Dalkey & Helmer, Citation1963). Feedback is given by the presentation of group results of the previous round at the beginning of each round, which encourages the expert panel to reconsider their previous responses and facilitates a conversion of opinions towards a consensus.

In the present study, we used a four-round modified Delphi method to collect the opinions of an expert panel regarding the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD (see for an overview of the rounds). The design of the Delphi method was modified in such a way that the first round started with a list of neuropsychological functions that were to be rated according to their relevance for a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD, instead of open-ended questions as is traditionally done in Delphi studies (Hsu & Sandford, Citation2007). The Delphi rounds were consecutively conducted over the course of eleven months, whereas the collection of data of each of the four rounds took about four weeks. Questionnaires of each round were created and distributed by the use of the online research software Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Citation2016). Correspondence was conducted via e-mail and the responses to the Delphi rounds were confidential (by following a link as provided by email) in order to facilitate honest and unbiased responses. The expert panel was asked to complete the questionnaire of each round within four weeks. During this period, three reminders were sent to those panel members who did not respond yet. After two weeks of the initial invitation to the Delphi round, the first reminder was sent. Two additional reminders in intervals of one week were sent to inform panel member about the termination date of the particular round. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Ethical Committee Psychology affiliated to the University of Groningen, the Netherlands.

Table 1. Overview of the Delphi rounds “Neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD”.

Participants and recruitment

The recruitment of suitable panel members with high expertise on the topic is considered as one of the most important determinants of success when conducting a Delphi study (Hsu & Sandford, Citation2007). Inclusion criteria of potential panel members were willingness to take part in the study as well as at least two scientific publications (peer-reviewed) about neuropsychological functioning of adults with ADHD. Candidates were identified based on publications as determined via scientific databases PsycInfo and PubMed. Moreover, clinical centers with specialized care for adults with ADHD were approached in order to identify suitable candidates.

Potential panel members were informed about the purpose of the study and were encouraged to recommend additional candidates with expertise in the field of adult ADHD. A total of 64 candidates opened the questionnaire and started to fill in responses to the questions of the first round. Twenty-seven of these 64 candidates actually completed the questionnaire. Only these 27 panel members were invited to the subsequent rounds. Twenty panel members completed the second round of the Delphi study, and 22 panel members completed the third round. Finally, in the fourth and last round, 21 panel members took part and completed the questionnaire.

Demographic characteristics of the panel members are presented in . The average age of the initial 27 panel members was 45.9 years with a standard deviation of 10.4 years. Seventeen of the panel members were male and ten were female. Nine panel members currently worked in the United States, eight in Germany, three in the United Kingdom, two in the Netherlands, two in Norway, and one each in Turkey, Canada and Argentina. The panel members indicated various academic backgrounds, including psychology, clinical psychology, neuropsychology, developmental/school neuropsychology, neuroscience or medicine. Seven panel members held a full professorship, five panel members indicated to be associate professor, two panel members worked currently as assistant professor, nine additional panel members hold a doctor title (i.e., PhD), and four panel members had a degree as a master of science (i.e., MSc). The expert panel indicated to have in average 20.9 (SD = 43.1) scientific publications related to neuropsychological functioning of adults with ADHD. On average, panel members spent 39% (SD = 31%) of their working time on clinical work, 43% (SD = 28%) on research and 18% (SD = 24%) on other responsibilities. Other responsibilities entail either teaching, administration, managing, mentoring or a combination of those.

Table 2. Characteristics of the expert panel.

Materials and procedure

Round 1

The first round of the Delphi study had two aims, that is (1) to identify the most relevant neuropsychological functions that should be considered in a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD, and (2) to collect suggestions for the measurement of these functions. Before the start of the first round, participants were requested to give informed consent and to respond to questions regarding demographic information.

In the beginning of the first round, the expert panel was presented with a list of 42 predetermined neuropsychological functions. This list of neuropsychological functions was created by means of a literature review that resulted in a total of 42 neuropsychological functions that were assigned to one of six categories, i.e., attention, executive functions, memory, perceptual functions, visuospatial functions and language functions. Panel members were asked to rate the relevance of each of the presented functions for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD on a 5-point Likert scale, i.e., 1 (very important), 2 (important), 3 (neutral), 4 (not important), to 5 (not important at all) (). Furthermore, panel members were given the possibility to add neuropsychological functions that should be additionally considered in a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. The relevance of the additional neuropsychological functions was rated on the same Likert scale as the functions presented before. After rating the relevance of all neuropsychological functions, the panel members were asked to indicate suitable measures to assess the neuropsychological functions that they have rated as important (score 2) or very important (score 1). Panel members were allowed to indicate multiple measures per neuropsychological function, ordered by their perceived relevance (i.e., starting with the most suitable measure). Panel members were further requested to specify the subtest, scale or variable of each measure that would be most suitable to assess the particular neuropsychological function.

Table 3. Ratings of the predetermined list of neuropsychological functions according to their relevance for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD (Round 1).

Round 2

The second round of the Delphi study aimed to rate the importance of the neuropsychological functions that were additionally suggested by the panel members in the first round. In the beginning of the second round, panel members received an overview of the results obtained by the first round, including the rating of the predetermined list of neuropsychological functions, a list of additional neuropsychological functions that were suggested by panel members during Round 1 as well as an overview of the suggested test measures for the assessment of each of the neuropsychological functions. Subsequently, panel members were presented with 27 functions that were additionally suggested by the expert panel in the first round and were asked to rate the relevance of each of these functions for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD on the same 5-point Likert scale as used in the first round. For each of the functions presented, it was additionally indicated how often each function has been suggested by panel members in the first round. After rating the relevance of all 27 functions, panel members were asked to suggest the most suitable measures to assess those functions that they rated as either important (score 2) or very important (score 1). Analogue to the procedure of the first round, panel members were allowed to indicate multiple measures per function, ordered by their perceived relevance (i.e., starting with the most suitable measure). Panel members were further requested to specify the subtest, scale or variable of each measure that they considered to be most suitable for assessing the particular function.

Round 3

The aim of the third round was to (1) re-evaluate ratings of all neuropsychological functions and to (2) evaluate the measures that were suggested by the expert panel with regard to their suitability for the assessment of particular neuropsychological functions of adults with ADHD. In the beginning of the third round, the expert panel received a list of 46 neuropsychological functions, consisting of the predetermined neuropsychological functions as presented in Round 1 as well as neuropsychological functions that were additionally suggested by the panel members in Round 2. Neuropsychological functions were excluded from further consideration as a potential part of a neuropsychological assessment battery if one of three arguments applied; (1) if the additionally suggested neuropsychological function was already included in the predetermined list of neuropsychological functions, (2) if the additionally suggested neuropsychological function had a large overlap with a function that was already part of the predetermined list (e.g., suggestion to additionally consider timing, even though time estimation was already part of the predetermined list), and (3) if the additionally suggested function did not qualify as a neuropsychological function, but rather represented a clinical symptom (e.g., hyperactivity/overactivity), coexisting problems (e.g., gambling problems), a concept related to other skills or characteristics (e.g., mathematics or personality), or a more general concept or condition (e.g., psychological state). The functions that were attributed to the third category were not entirely excluded from the present study but were separately presented to the expert panel since these entries were obviously regarded as important concepts for the clinical evaluation of adults with ADHD, even though they do not fall within the scope of a classic neuropsychological assessment.

The list of 46 neuropsychological functions provided two more types of information per function, i.e., the individual rating of the particular panel member himself/herself as well as the average rating of the entire expert panel as derived from the previous rounds. Panel members were requested to reconsider their previous rating for each function in the light of the average group rating. For those neuropsychological functions that were rated by the expert panel in the previous rounds on average as either important or very important, panel members were asked to rate the suitability of the suggested measures to assess these neuropsychological functions. Each measure that was suggested by at least two different panel members in the previous rounds was considered for suitability rating. However, in case that only one or two measures had been suggested by the entire expert panel for the assessment of a particular function, these measures were also presented to the expert panel in Round 3, also when they were only suggested by one panel member. The expert panel was asked to indicate for each measure whether they consider it as suitable or not suitable, or whether they do not know the measure. In some cases, when several versions or subtests of a measure have been suggested in Rounds 1 and 2, panel members were asked in Round 3 to first rate the suitability of the general measure, followed by the request to rate the suitability of the specific versions or subtests. These follow-up questions were only presented to those panel members that rated the general measure as suitable. Furthermore, panel members were requested to indicate for each measure whether the measure would be part of their assessment battery if they had to compose a neuropsychological assessment of an adult with ADHD in a given situation. Panel members were also given the opportunity to suggest further measures for the assessment of each function.

Finally, panel members were asked to indicate the time they spend on average on a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment of an adult with ADHD as well as the time they would ideally like to have for such an assessment.

Round 4

The aim of the fourth round was to achieve consensus on an assessment battery for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. In the beginning of the fourth round, the expert panel was presented with a ranked list of neuropsychological functions as derived from ratings of Round 3. For each function that had an average rating of important or very important (score < 2.50), two measures were suggested for its assessment. First, the measure that was most frequently rated as suitable was presented. Second, the measure with the second highest suitable rating and the lowest rate of disapproval (i.e., the fewest not suitable ratings) was presented. In addition, descriptive statistics for the average time spent on a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment of an adult with ADHD, and the time panel members would ideally like to spend on such an assessment was presented.

Panel members were further asked whether they agreed with the suggested assessment battery. In case of disagreement, panel members were asked to suggest changes that would lead to their agreement.

Results

Round 1

The ratings of the predetermined neuropsychological functions as well as the additionally suggested functions by the expert panel are presented in and , respectively. In total, 27 functions were suggested by the panel members in addition to the 42 functions as mentioned on the list of predetermined neuropsychological functions For each neuropsychological function a panel member rated as important or very important, the panel member was asked to suggest one or more measures to assess this function (see Table of the supplementary information for a complete presentation of all suggested measures for the assessment of the predetermined neuropsychological functions). Interquartile ranges of ratings were determined as an objective measure of consensus between panel members in their evaluation of the importance of each neuropsychological function (Scheibe, Skutsch, & Schofer, Citation1975). For a Likert-scale with four to five answer options, an interquartile range of 1 or less is considered as a high level of consensus (Raskin, Citation1994; Rayens & Hahn, Citation2000). In the ratings of the predetermined list of neuropsychological functions of Round 1, high consensus, as indicated by interquartile ranges of 0 or 1, was achieved for 30 of the 42 neuropsychological functions.

Round 2

In the second round of the study all functions that were additionally suggested by the expert panel in the first round were rated regarding their importance for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD (see ). The level of consensus among panel members on their ratings was again addressed by interquartile ranges. This calculation revealed consensus (i.e., interquartile range of 0 or 1) for 17 of the 27 suggested functions. Furthermore, panel members suggested measures to assess those functions that they rated as important or very important (see Table S2 of the supplementary information for a complete presentation of all measures that were suggested for the assessment of the additional functions).

Table 4. Ratings of functions that were additionally suggested by panel members for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD (Round 2).

Round 3

Round 3 of the study resulted in a final ranking of 46 neuropsychological functions as derived from a re-evaluation of the ratings of all neuropsychological functions from Round 1 and 2 (). This final ranking of neuropsychological functions contained both functions from the predetermined list of neuropsychological functions, as well as functions that were additionally suggested by panel members in the previous rounds. Consensus among members of the expert panel was high across all neuropsychological functions in the final ranking as indicated by an interquartile range of 0 or 1 for the ratings of all functions (Raskin, Citation1994; Rayens & Hahn, Citation2000). As described above (see materials and procedure section), not all functions that were additionally suggested by the expert panel were considered in the final rating of neuropsychological functions in order to avoid redundancy and to restrict the list to typical neuropsychological functions. Functions that were suggested by the expert panel but that do not fall within the narrower focus of neuropsychology were listed separately as they are perceived by the panel members as important concepts within the clinical evaluation of adults with ADHD ().

Table 5. Final ratings of all neuropsychological functions for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD (Round 3).

Table 6. Ratings of functions that were suggested for the clinical evaluation of adults with ADHD that do not fall within the scope of a neuropsychological assessment (Round 2).

Moreover, an assessment battery was composed for the examination of those functions that received an average rating score of 2.5 or lower (i.e., important or very important). This battery suggests the measure that was rated most often as suitable by the expert panel for each neuropsychological function. As an ‘alternative measure’, the battery suggests the measure that was rated second most often as suitable for each individual neuropsychological function, while having the lowest rate of not suitable ratings (see for a complete presentation of the assessment battery). Alternative measures could not be suggested for all neuropsychological functions of the assessment battery. This was the case if either no alternative measure was suggested by panel members, or the suggested measure was considered not suitable by at least one third of all panel members who indicated to know the measure. The information obtained from the question whether the measure at hand would be part of the assessment battery if the panel member had to perform an individual neuropsychological assessment of an adult with ADHD, was not considered for the final ranking. The reason was that when panel members answered this question, they primarily considered those measures that are available in their own work context instead of the general usefulness of the measures suggested. Moreover, responses to this question did not take the utility of other measures that may be equally, or even more suitable than the one they use in their daily work, into account. Thus, we considered the suitability rating, that allowed panel members to indicate several suitable measures as well as measures that are unknown to the panel members, as more informative for a valid ranking of neuropsychological measures.

Table 7. Test battery for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD as suggested by the expert panel.

Finally, panel members were requested to indicate the time they spend on average on a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD as well as the time they ideally like to spend on such an assessment. As presented in , the median time panel members spend on average on a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD is 2 h 15 min (median absolute deviation 1 h 15 min). The median time panel members ideally like to spend on a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD, however, is 3 h (median absolute deviation 1 h 15 min).

Table 8. Duration of a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD according to panel members.

Round 4

Fifteen (71.4%) of the 21 panel members that participated in the fourth round agreed with the assessment battery without further changes, five panel members (23.8%) disagreed with the assessment battery in such a way that they suggested further changes, and one panel member (4.8%) generally disagreed with the approach. More specifically, the five panel members disagreed with the battery in specific details, that is the lack of consideration of other functions or measures (three panel members), the length of the battery (one panel member), or an overlap between functions and/or measures as contained in the battery (one panel member).

Discussion

Based on the knowledge and expertise of an international panel of experts in the field of adult ADHD, the present study derived a ranking of neuropsychological functions with regard to their importance for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. A high level of consensus was attained in the evaluation of neuropsychological functions as indicated by interquartile ranges of 0 or 1 for all functions, which is considered as an objective and rigorous measure of group consensus (Scheibe et al., Citation1975). The ratings revealed that 16 neuropsychological functions were considered by the panel members to be important for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. On this basis, a neuropsychological assessment battery was composed. The majority of the panel members (71.4%) agreed with the assessment battery without further changes. Five experts (23.8%) disagreed only in a specific aspect, while only one panel member (4.8%) generally disagreed with the approach. Given that the assessment battery contains measures that are commonly used in research and clinical practice, it may have the potential to give both clinicians and researchers a toolkit for the selection of widely accepted measures for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. In this context, it must be stressed that the assessment battery is not meant to serve as a diagnostic tool to diagnose ADHD in adults, but provides a selection of functions and measures for the cognitive assessment of adults that are already diagnosed with ADHD. Such a battery is useful to characterize neuropsychological strengths and weaknesses and offer specific information for individualized treatment planning (Lange et al., Citation2014). For example, a clinical neuropsychological assessment can increase awareness of patients and relatives about individual strengths and weaknesses and can be used to design and implement individual compensation strategies, e.g., using external memory aids, decreasing environmental distraction, using personal organizers, or developing daily routines. In addition to support treatment planning, a neuropsychological assessment can be useful to objectify subjectively reported complaints and thus increase individual compliance and adherence to treatment. By suggesting an alternative measure in addition to the most suitable measure for the assessment of the majority of neuropsychological functions, the compiled battery takes the availability of measures across different institutes into account, as well as the differences in priorities when selecting functions and measures for the assessment of individual patients. Thus, by providing a complete ranking of all neuropsychological functions discussed, as well as the suggestion of two measures for most of the relevant functions (best suited and alternative measure), the suggested assessment battery can be flexibly adapted based on individual needs of clinicians and researchers (e.g., by selecting specific functions and/or measures only). The use of the suggested assessment battery may also depend on the time that is available for an assessment. The present study revealed that there is considerable variation across experts in the field with regard to the time spent on neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. The present findings also indicate that thorough neuropsychological assessment is considered relevant as additional test time is desired by the panel members. Furthermore, it was shown that panel members wish to perform neuropsychological assessments of adults with ADHD more elaborately than they are currently able to perform in their given context.

Of note, the functions that were rated by the expert panel as being of importance for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD largely correspond to the results of empirical research that identified robust impairments in several aspects of cognition in adults with ADHD as reported in reviews, meta-analyses, and studies using comprehensive neuropsychological test batteries (Alderson et al., Citation2013; Boonstra et al., Citation2005; Fuermaier et al., Citation2015; Hervey et al., Citation2004; Mostert et al., Citation2015; Schoechlin & Engel, Citation2005; Woods et al., Citation2002). Nevertheless, ratings of experts and results of previous empirical research did not overlap in all components, as for example verbal memory and fluency functions have repeatedly been shown to be compromised in adults with ADHD (Fuermaier et al., Citation2015; Fuermaier et al., Citation2016; Skodzik, Holling, & Pedersen, Citation2017; L. Tucha et al., Citation2011; O. Tucha et al., Citation2005), but were not rated by the expert panel of the present study for inclusion in the assessment battery. As a possible explanation for this discrepancy, it must be considered that memory problems in adults with ADHD were described to arise particularly in those memory functions that are strongly associated with executive control (Fuermaier, Tucha, Koerts, Aschenbrenner, Westermann, et al., Citation2013; Muir-Broaddus et al., Citation2002; Pollak et al., Citation2008). Thus, it can be speculated that panel members assume that memory problems of adults with ADHD occur secondary to executive dysfunctions, and therefore did not prioritize the assessment of memory in the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. Concerning fluency functions, it can be speculated that panel members did not assign this function a high rank as test performance in fluency functions might clinically be more difficult to interpret with regard to its implications for daily life functioning compared to neuropsychological functions with more obvious associations to tasks of daily living, such as distractibility or task planning. In contrast, the expert panel considered also functions as important which did not play a central role in the majority of research on the clinical neuropsychology of adults with ADHD, such as time estimation. There is indeed evidence of altered time estimation and reproduction in individuals with ADHD (Barkley, Edwards, Laneri, Fletcher, & Metevia, Citation2001; Mette et al., Citation2015; Noreika, Falter, & Rubia, Citation2013; Pollak, Kroyzer, Yakir, & Friedler, Citation2009; Prevatt, Proctor, Baker, Garrett, & Yelland, Citation2011), nevertheless, measures of time estimation and reproduction have not been routinely used in the assessment of adults with ADHD. Taken together, our conclusion is twofold. While the results of the present study are consistent with major findings of empirical neuropsychological research of the last decades, they also shed light on the topic from a more clinical perspective. This perspective weights the importance of individual neuropsychological functions in the assessment of adults with ADHD differently than they have been treated in previous research (e.g., fluency functions or time estimation).

With regard to the measures of the composed assessment battery, it was noted that the expert panel selected several computerized tests for one or more aspects of attention, such as variants of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) or subtests of the Test battery of Attention Performance (TAP). The CPT and TAP are indeed prominent and widely used tests in the clinical assessment of children and adults with ADHD (Epstein, Conners, Sitarenios, & Erhardt, Citation1998; Epstein et al., Citation2001; Fuermaier et al., Citation2015; Huang-Pollock, Karalunas, Tam, & Moore, Citation2012; Marchetta, Hurks, De Sonneville, Krabbendam, & Jolles Citation2008, O. Tucha, Mecklinger, et al., Citation2006; O. Tucha, Walitza, et al., Citation2006); however, their clinical use have also been criticized for a number of reasons (Berger, Slobodin, & Cassuto, Citation2017; Hall et al., Citation2016). For example, diagnostic precision of CPTs (and related tests for attention) to determine ADHD was mostly inaccurate. While sensitivity towards ADHD was shown to be reasonable in several studies, specificity towards normal controls and individuals with other psychiatric disorders was often low (Arble, Kuentzel, & Barnett, Citation2014; Edwards et al., Citation2007; Forbes, Citation1998; Park et al., Citation2016; Riccio & Reynolds, Citation2001; Schatz, Ballantyne, & Trauner, Citation2001). Thus, CPT test failure is not disorder specific, as there are usually multiple possible reasons why someone would show poor performance on a CPT. Furthermore, only moderate correlations were found between CPT performance and parent or teacher ratings of individuals with ADHD (Forbes, Citation1998; Rielly, Cunningham, Richards, Elbard, & Mahoney, Citation1999), which is related to the critique of low ecological validity of CPTs (i.e., inability to simulate difficulties of patients in everyday life). Several authors stressed the need to improve the ecological validity by assessing the individual’s behavior in more natural settings, because most CPTs do not contain external and environmental distracting stimuli (Negut, Jurma, & David, Citation2016; Parsons, Bowerly, Buckwalter, & Rizzo, Citation2007). As another concern about CPTs, test-retest reliability varied considerably across studies and CPT measures used. In general, omission errors tended to have lower reliability, while commission errors and reaction time variables had higher reliability (Berger & Cassuto, Citation2014; Ogundele, Ayyash, & Banerjee, Citation2011). If the CPT does not prove to fulfill sufficient requirements on reliability, it may also not be appropriate for monitoring treatment effects. Further critiques on the CPT concerned that developmental changes in ADHD symptomatology were not consistently reflected in changes in CPT performance (Vaughn et al., Citation2011), and that many current versions of CPTs have ceiling effects, which appeared to be associated to the simplicity of CPTs including visual stimuli (Berlin, Bohlin, Nyberg, & Janols, Citation2004, Mahone, Pillion, & Hiemenz, Citation2001). Because of these serious limitations associated to the CPT, multiple expert groups concluded in consensus statements and clinical practice guidelines that computerized testing of sustained attention, such as with variants of the CPT, is not useful for clinical diagnosing of ADHD (American Academy of Pediatrics, Citation2001; Gualtieri & Johnson, Citation2011; Seixas, Weiss, & Muller, Citation2012; Wolraich et al., Citation2011).

Representativeness of the expert panel

One of the most important determinants of success of a Delphi study is the qualification of the panel members as experts in the field (Hasson, Keeney, & McKenna, Citation2000). Several characteristics of panel members of the present study give a good indication that they indeed constitute a group with high expertise in the field of adult ADHD. First, the academic degree/position of the expert panel was high, with most panel members being professors and/or having a doctor title. Second, the number of publications in the field of neuropsychological functioning of adults with ADHD was high and considerably exceeded the minimum inclusion criterion of two publications per panel member (see ). Other characteristics of an expert panel that may distort data analysis and interpretation are panel size, response rate, response bias and drop-out bias. It has been argued that for homogeneous panels a group size of 10–15 members is in most cases sufficient (Delbecq, Van de Ven, & Gustafson, Citation1975). The expert panel of the present study (20–27 members) exceeded this number, leading to the conclusion that panel size was adequate. Regarding the response rate, it was noted that 64 potential candidates opened the questionnaire and started to fill in information, whereas only 27 panel members actually completed the first round. Nonetheless, more than 70% of panel members remained part of the expert panel throughout the Delphi process (27 panel members in the first round to 21 panel members in the fourth round), which has been noted by several authors to be necessary to achieve a rigor Delphi process (e.g., Sumsion, Citation1998; Walker & Selfe, Citation1996). Conclusions regarding a response bias are difficult to draw as it remains unknown why some invited experts agreed to participate in the study while others did not. However, considering the acceptable sample size and the high expertise of the panel members, it may be assumed that the expert panel forms a representative group of experts in the field of clinical neuropsychology of adult ADHD. Finally, conclusions regarding a bias due to drop out can be drawn when comparing panel characteristics of the different rounds. While many characteristics were comparable across panel members of the different rounds, such as age, geographic location, academic background, or type of work, other characteristics differed considerably between panels of the four rounds, such as gender distribution or the number of publications. Most obviously, the mean number of publications dropped from about 21 publications in the first round to about 9 publications in the second round, and rose back to a level comparable to the first round in Rounds 3 and 4. While it cannot be excluded that a response bias might have occurred due to these differences in panel characteristics, distortions of the Delphi process might have been little given that this bias was caused by a very high number of publications of single panel members, and given that all information that was contained in round 2 (evaluation of functions that were additionally suggested by panel members) was evaluated by the panel in Round 3.

Limitations

The present study must be seen in the context of limitations. First, not all panel members agreed with the assessment battery as presented in Round 4. More specifically, six panel members disagreed with the approach because of specific reasons (five panel members) or general disagreement (one panel member). However, it can be argued that the specific aspects of the assessment battery to which panel members disagreed might not be a major threat to the study in most cases. For example, disagreement with specific functions, measures, or the total length of the battery (four panel members) could be compensated by considering the suggested battery as a toolkit that does not need to be performed in its entire length in all cases, but instead can be applied in parts only in order to accommodate the respective situation in the individual clinical setting. In contrast, the critique of a large overlap between functions and measures (one panel member) is a valid point that must be considered when interpreting the use of the present assessment battery, as evidently some of the measures assess more than one function (e.g., panel members suggested the CPT for the assessment of sustained attention, distractibility, and focused attention). We regret that it was not possible to take this rather complex issue into account when composing the battery. Considering the already substantial time investment of panel members over four rounds of the Delphi process, additional time investment of panel members was not requested in order to avoid a large drop-out rate.

Furthermore, the present study failed to suggest an alternative measure for the assessment of each function of the assessment battery as an acceptable suitability rating was not achieved for an alternative measure of all functions (for about one third of functions, no alternative measure could be provided). Given the differences in availability and preferences of clinical tools across institutes, an alternative measure for the assessment of each aspect of functioning would certainly be an advantage.

Finally, further limitations occurred that are associated with the Delphi methodology. For example, the guided process towards group consensus may eliminate divergent but potentially important opinions of single panel members. Thus, the results of a Delphi study represent the solutions with largest overlap between panel members, but do not necessarily represent optimal solutions or objective truth on a specific topic. Therefore, the results should rather be seen as the compromise on the design of a neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD as it is applied by an international panel of experts in research and clinic. Moreover, the characteristics of a Delphi process do not allow direct communication and discussion between panel members. In the present context, the selection of tests, subtests, or variables of specific measures might have been facilitated by personal discussion between panel members. However, personal communication was not allowed in order to ensure crucial characteristics of a Delphi process, such as the avoidance of group pressure toward conformity and the influence of authority.

Conclusion

The present study employed a Delphi methodology to perform a ranking of 46 neuropsychological functions regarding their importance for the neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD. This process led to the composition of a test battery suggesting measures for the assessment of 16 functions that were rated as either important or very important. This study was the first of its kind among experts in the field of ADHD and provides a toolkit of functions and measures that can be used for an objective assessment of cognitive functions of adults with ADHD. While it must be emphasized that a neuropsychological assessment is neither sensitive nor specific enough to diagnose ADHD, a neuropsychological assessment is very useful to complement diagnostic assessment and serves as an objective indication of cognitive dysfunctioning of individuals diagnosed with ADHD. Results of such a clinical neuropsychological assessment can increase awareness of individual strengths and weaknesses and can be used to design and implement compensation strategies. Furthermore it can be useful to objectify subjectively reported complaints and thus to increase individual compliance and adherence to treatment. The observed differences between the results of the present Delphi study and previous neuropsychological research on adults with ADHD underline the importance of the Delphi methodology and highlight the clinical relevance of the present results.

Disclosure of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Supplemental material

hapn_a_1429441_sm7753.docx

Download MS Word (28.6 KB)

References

  • Agarwal, R., Goldenberg, M., Perry, R., & IsHak, W. W. (2012). The quality of life of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 9(5–6), 10–21.
  • Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Hudec, K. L., & Patros, C. H. G. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 27(3), 287–302. doi:10.1037/a0032371
  • Altgassen, M., Kretschmer, A., & Kliegel, M. (2014). Task dissociation in prospective memory performance in individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18(7), 617–624. doi:10.1177/1087054712445484
  • American Academy of Pediatrics. (2001). Subcommittee on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and committee on quality improvement. Clinical practice guideline: Treatment of the school-aged child with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 108, 1033–1044.
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
  • Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., Maglione, K., Doyle, A. E., Fried, R., Seidman, L. J., & Biederman, J. (2010). Executive functioning in high-IQ adults with ADHD. Psychological Medicine, 40(11), 1909–1918. doi:10.1017/s0033291709992273
  • Arble, E., Kuentzel, J., & Barnett, D. (2014). Convergent validity of the Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA + Plus): Associations with working memory, processing speed, and behavioral ratings. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29, 300–312. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu006
  • Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., Voigt, R. G., Killian, J. M., & Katusic, S. K. (2013). Mortality, ADHD, and psychosocial adversity in adults with childhood ADHD: A prospective study. Pediatrics, 131(4), 637–644. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-2354
  • Barkley, R., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 541–556.
  • Barkley, R., Murphy, K., & Bush, T. (2001). Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15(3), 351–360. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.15.3.351
  • Barkley, R., Murphy, K. R., & Fischer, M. (2007). ADHD in adults: What the science says. New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Berger, I., & Cassuto, H. (2014). The effect of environmental distractors incorporation into a CPT on sustained attention and ADHD diagnosis among adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 62–68. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.012
  • Berger, I., Slobodin, O., & Cassuto, H. (2017). Usefulness and validity of continuous performance tests in the diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(1), 81–93.
  • Berlin, L., Bohlin, G., Nyberg, L., & Janols, L. (2004). How well do measures of inhibition and other executive functions discriminate between children with ADHD and controls? Child Neuropsychology, 10, 1–13. doi:10.1076/chin.10.1.1.26243
  • Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 816–818. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816
  • Boonstra, A. M., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). Executive functioning in adult ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 35(8), 1097–1108. doi:10.1017/s003329170500499x
  • Boonstra, A. M., Kooij, J. J. S., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). To act or not to act, that’s the problem: Primarily inhibition difficulties in adult ADHD. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 209–221. doi:10.1037/a0017670
  • Boulkedid, R., Abdoul, H., Loustau, M., Sibony, O., & Alberti, C. (2011). Using and reporting the Delphi method for selecting healthcare quality indicators: A systematic review. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20476. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020476
  • Canu, W. H., & Carlson, C. L. (2007). Rejection sensitivity and social outcomes of young adult men with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(3), 261–275. doi:10.1177/1087054706288106
  • Dalkey, N., & Helmer, O. (1963). An experimental application of the Delphi method to the use of experts. Management Science, 9(3), 458–467. doi:10.1287/mnsc.9.3.458
  • Delbecq, A. L., Van de Ven, A. H., & Gustafson, D. H. (1975). Group techniques for program planning: A guide to nominal and Delphi processes. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and Co.
  • Diamantopoulou, S., Rydell, A., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2007). Impact of executive functioning and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on children’s peer relations and school performance. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 521–542. doi:10.1080/87565640701360981
  • Edwards, M. C., Gardner, E. S., Chelonis, J. J., Schulz, E. G., Flake, R. A., & Diaz, P. F. (2007). Estimates of the validity and utility of the Conner’s CPT in the assessment of inattentive and/or hyperactive impulsive behaviors in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 393–404. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9098-3
  • Epstein, J. N., Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., & Erhardt, D. (1998). Continuous performance test results of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 155–168. doi:10.1076/clin.12.2.155.2000
  • Epstein, J. N., Johnson, D. E., Varia, I. M., & Conners, C. K. (2001). Neuropsychological assessment of response inhibition in adults with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(3), 362–371. doi:10.1076/jcen.23.3.362.1186
  • Fayyad, J., De Graaf, R., Kessler, R., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Demyttenaere, K., … Jin, R. (2007). Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 402–409. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034389
  • Fergusson, D., Lynskey, M., & Horwood, L. (1997). Attentional difficulties in middle childhood and psychosocial outcomes in young adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(6), 633–644. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01690.x
  • Fogel, B. S., & Greenberg, D. B. (2015). Psychiatric care of the medical patient (3rd ed.). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/med/9780199731855.001.0001
  • Forbes, G. B. (1998). Clinical utility of the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) in the diagnosis of attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, 461–476. doi:10.1002/(sici)1097-4679(199806)54:4<461::aid-jclp8>3.3.co;2-s
  • Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., Lange, K. W., & Tucha, O. (2013). Source discrimination in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e65134. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065134
  • Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Aschenbrenner, S., Westermann, C., Weisbrod, M. … Tucha, O. (2013). Complex prospective memory in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e58338. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058338
  • Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Kaunzinger, I., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M. … Tucha, O. (2015). Cognitive impairment in adult ADHD - perspective matters!. Neuropsychology, 29(1), 45–58.
  • Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Koerts, J., Weisbrod, M., Lange, K. W. … Tucha, O. (2016). Effects of methylphenidate on memory functions of adults with ADHD. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 24(3), 199–211. doi:10.1080/23279095.2015.1124108
  • Groen, Y., Gaastra, G. F., Lewis-Evans, B., & Tucha, O. (2013). Risky behavior in gambling tasks in individuals with ADHD - A systematic literature review. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74909. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074909
  • Gualtieri, C. T., & Johnson, L. (2011). ADHD: Is objective diagnosis possible? Psychiatry, 2(11), 44–53.
  • Habibi, A., Sarafrazi, A., & Izadyar, S. (2014). Delphi technique theoretical framework in qualitative research. The International Journal of Engineering and Science, 3(4), 8–13.
  • Hall, C. L., Valentine, A. Z., Groom, M. J., Walker, G. M., Sayal, K., Daley, D., & Hollis, C. (2016). The clinical utility of the continuous performance test and objective measures of activity for diagnosing and monitoring ADHD in children: A systematic review. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 677–699. doi:10.1007/s00787-015-0798-x
  • Halleland, H. B., Haavik, J., & Lundervold, A. J. (2012). Set-shifting in adults with ADHD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(4), 728–737. doi:10.1017/s1355617712000355
  • Hasson, F., Keeney, S., & McKenna, H. (2000). Research guidelines for the Delphi survey technique. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 32(4), 1008–1015. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.2000.t01-1-01567.x
  • Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., & Curry, J. F. (2004). Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 485–503. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485
  • Hsu, C., & Sandford, B. A. (2007). The Delphi technique: Making sense of consensus. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation, 12(10), 1–8.
  • Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: a meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 360–371. doi:10.1037/a0027205
  • Kaplan, B. J., Dewey, D., Crawford, S. G., & Fisher, G. C. (1998). Deficits in long-term memory are not characteristic of ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(4), 518–528. doi:10.1076/jcen.20.4.518.1477
  • Kok, F. M., Groen, Y., Fuermaier, A. B. M., & Tucha, O. (2016). Problematic peer functioning in girls with ADHD: A systematic literature review. PLoS ONE, 11(11), e0165119. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0165119
  • Kooij, S. J. J., Bejerot, S., Blackwell, A., Caci, H., Casas-Brugue, M., Carpentier, P. J. … Asherson, P. (2010). European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European network adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry, 10(67), 1–24. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-67
  • Lange, K. W., Hauser, J., Lange, K. M., Makulska-Gertruda, E., Takano, T., Takeuchi, Y. … Tucha, O. (2014). Utility of cognitive neuropsychological assessment in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders, 6, 241–248. doi:10.1007/s12402-014-0132-3
  • Mahone, M. E., Pillion, J. P., & Hiemenz, J. R. (2001). Initial development of an auditory continuous performance test for preschoolers. Journal of Attention Disorders, 5, 93–106. doi:10.1177/108705470100500203
  • Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bonagura, N., Malloy, P., Giampino, H., & Addali, K. A. (1991). Hyperactive boys almost grown up: Replication of psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 77–83. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810250079012
  • Marchetta, N. D. J., Hurks, P. P. M., De Sonneville, L. M. J., Krabbendam, L., & Jolles, J. (2008). Sustained and focused attention deficits in adult ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11, 664–676. doi:10.1177/1087054707305108
  • Mette, C., Grabemann, M., Zimmermann, M., Strunz, L., Scherbaum, N., Wiltfang, J., & Kis, B. (2015). No clear association between impaired short-term or working memory storage and time reproduction capacity in adult ADHD patients. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0133714. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133714
  • Mostert, J. C., Onnink, A., Klein, M., Dammers, J., Harneit, A., Schulten, T. … Hoogman, M. (2015). Cognitive heterogeneity in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. A systematic analysis of neuropsychological measurements. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 11, 2062–2074.
  • Muir-Broaddus, J. E., Rosenstein, L. D., Medina, D. E., & Soderberg, C. (2002). Neuropsychological test performance of children with ADHD relative to test norms and parent behavioral ratings. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17(7), 671–689. doi:10.1093/arclin/17.7.671
  • Negut, A., Jurma, A. M., & David, D. (2016). Virtual-reality-based attention assessment of ADHD: Clinical VR: Classroom-CPT versus a traditional continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 1–21. doi:10.1080/09297049.2016.1186617
  • Noreika, V., Falter, C. M., & Rubia, K. (2013). Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 235–266. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.036
  • Ogundele, M. O., Ayyash, H. F., & Banerjee, S. (2011). Role of computerised continuous performance task tests in ADHD. Progress in Neurology and Psychiatry, 15, 8–13. doi:10.1002/pnp.198
  • Park, J., Kim, C., Ahn, J., Joo, Y., Shin, M., Lee, H., & Kim, H. (2016). Clinical use of continuous performance tests to diagnose children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 108705471665812. (Epub). doi:10.1177/1087054716658125
  • Parsons, T. D., Bowerly, T., Buckwalter, J. G., & Rizzo, A. A. (2007). A controlled clinical comparison of attention performance in children with ADHD in a virtual reality classroom compared to standard neuropsychological methods. Child Neuropsychology, 13, 363–381. doi:10.1080/13825580600943473
  • Pollak, Y., Kahana-Vax, G., & Hoofien, D. (2008). Retrieval processes in adults with ADHD: A RAVLT study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(1), 62–73. doi:10.1080/87565640701729789
  • Pollak, Y., Kroyzer, N., Yakir, A., & Friedler, A. (2009). Testing possible mechanisms of deficient supra-second time estimation in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 679–686. doi:10.1037/a0016281
  • Powell, C. (2003). The Delphi technique: Myths and realities. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 41(4), 376–382. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02537.x
  • Prevatt, F., Proctor, B., Baker, L., Garrett, L., & Yelland, S. (2011). Time estimation abilities of college students with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(7), 531–538. doi:10.1177/1087054710370673
  • Qualtrics. (2016). Qualtrics. Provo, UT, USA.
  • Raskin, M. (1994). The Delphi study in-field instruction revisited - expert consensus on issues and research priorities. Journal of Social Work Education, 30(1), 75–89.
  • Rayens, M. K., & Hahn, E. J. (2000). Building consensus using the policy Delphi method. Policy, Politics, & Nursing Practice, 1, 308–318. doi:10.1177/152715440000100409
  • Riccio, C. A., & Reynolds, C. R. (2001). Continuous performance tests are sensitive to ADHD in adults but lack specificity. A review and critique for differential diagnosis. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 931, 113–139. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05776.x
  • Rielly, N. E., Cunningham, C. E., Richards, J. E., Elbard, H. J., & Mahoney, W. J. (1999). Detecting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a communications clinic: Diagnostic utility of the Gordon Diagnostic System. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 685–700. doi:10.1076/jcen.21.5.685.866
  • Rohlf, H., Jucksch, V., Gawrilow, C., Huss, M., Hein, J., Lehmkuhl, U., & Salbach-Andrae, H. (2012). Set shifting and working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 119(1), 95–106. doi:10.1007/s00702-011-0660-3
  • Schatz, A. M., Ballantyne, A. O., & Trauner, D. A. (2001). Sensitivity and specificity of a computerized test of attention in the diagnosis of attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Assessment, 8, 357–365. doi:10.1177/107319110100800401
  • Scheibe, M., Skutsch, M., & Schofer, J. (1975). Experiments in Delphi methodology. In H. A. Linstone & M. Turoff (Eds.), The Delphi method — techniques and applications (pp. 262–287). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Schoechlin, C., & Engel, R. R. (2005). Neuropsychological performance in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Meta-analysis of empirical data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(6), 727–744. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.005
  • Seidman, L. J. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(4), 466–485. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.004
  • Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., & Makris, N. (2005). Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1263–1272. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.019
  • Seixas, M., Weiss, M., & Muller, U. (2012). Systematic review of national and international guidelines on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 26, 753–765. doi:10.1177/0269881111412095
  • Skodzik, T., Holling, H., & Pedersen, A. (2017). Long-term memory performance in adult ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(4), 267–283. doi:10.1177/1087054713510561
  • Sobaih, A. E. E., Ritchie, C., & Jones, E. (2012). Consulting the oracle? Applications of modified Delphi technique to qualitative research in the hospitality industry. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 24(6), 886–906. doi:10.1108/09596111211247227
  • Sumsion, T. (1998). The Delphi technique: An adaptive research tool. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61(4), 153–156. doi:10.1177/030802269806100403
  • Thome, J., Ehlis, A., Fallgatter, A. J., Krauel, K., Lange, K. W., Riederer, P. … Gerlach, M. (2012). Biomarkers for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A consensus report of the WFSBP task force on biological markers and the world federation of ADHD. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 13(5), 379–400. doi:10.3109/15622975.2012.690535
  • Tucha, L., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., Buggenthin, R., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M. … Tucha, O. (2017). Sustained attention in adult ADHD: Time-on-task effects of various measures of attention. Journal of Neural Transmission, 124(1), 39–53. doi:10.1007/s00702-015-1426-0
  • Tucha, L., Tucha, O., Laufkoetter, R., Walitza, S., Klein, H. E., & Lange, K. W. (2008). Neuropsychological assessment of attention in adults with different subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(2), 269–278. doi:10.1007/s00702-007-0836-z
  • Tucha, L., Tucha, O., Sontag, T. A., Stasik, D., Laufkoetter, R., & Lange, K. W. (2011). Differential effects of methylphenidate on problem solving in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(2), 161–173. doi:10.1177/1087054709356391
  • Tucha, O., Mecklinger, L., Laufkötter, R., Kaunzinger, I., Paul, G. M., Klein, H. E., & Lange, K. W. (2005). Clustering and switching on verbal and figural fluency functions in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(3), 231–248. doi:10.1080/13546800444000047
  • Tucha, O., Mecklinger, L., Laufkoetter, R., Klein, H. E., Walitza, S., & Lange, K. W. (2006). Methylphenidate-induced improvements of various measures of attention in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 113(10), 1575–1592. doi:10.1007/s00702-005-0437-7
  • Tucha, O., Walitza, S., Mecklinger, L., Sontag, T., Kuebber, S., Linder, M., & Lange, K. W. (2006). Attentional functioning in children with ADHD - predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type and children with ADHD - combined type. Journal of Neural Transmission, 113(12), 1943–1953. doi:10.1007/s00702-006-0496-4
  • Vaughn, A., Epstein, J. N., Rausch, J., Altaye, M., Langberg, J., Newcorn, J. H. … Wigal, T. (2011). Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test and ADHD symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 853–864. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9501-y
  • Walker, A. M., & Selfe, J. (1996). The Delphi method: A useful tool for the allied health researcher. International Journal of Therapy and Rehabilitation, 3(12), 677–681. doi:10.12968/bjtr.1996.3.12.14731
  • White, H. A., & Marks, W. (2004). Updating memory in list-method directed forgetting: Individual differences related to adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(7), 1453–1462. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.002
  • Wolraich, M., Brown, L., Brown, R. T., DuPaul, G., Earls, M., Feldman, H. M. … Visser, S. (2011). Subcommittee on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; steering committee on quality improvement and management. ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128, 1007–1022.
  • Woods, S. P., Lovejoy, D. W., & Ball, J. D. (2002). Neuropsychological characteristics of adults with ADHD: A comprehensive review of initial studies. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(1), 12–34. doi:10.1076/clin.16.1.12.8336
  • Ziglio, E. (1996). The Delphi method and its contribution to decision making. In M. Adler & E. Ziglio (Eds.), Gazing into the oracle: The Delphi method and its application to social policy and public health (pp. 3–26). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.