75
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Clinical Trial Report

Heat pain detection threshold is associated with the area of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization: a study of healthy male volunteers

, , , &
Pages 265-274 | Published online: 27 Jan 2017

Abstract

Introduction

The area of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization (BTS) of the skin and heat pain detection thresholds (HPDT) may both have predictive abilities in regards to pain sensitivity and clinical pain states. The association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia, however, remains unsettled, and the dissimilarities in physiologic properties suggest that they may represent 2 distinctively different pain entities. The aim of this study was to investigate the association between HPDT and BTS-induced secondary hyperalgesia.

Methods

A sample of 121 healthy male participants was included and tested on 2 separate study days with BTS (45°C, 3 minutes), HPDT, and pain during thermal stimulation (45°C, 1 minute). Areas of secondary hyperalgesia were quantified after monofilament pinprick stimulation. The pain catastrophizing scale (PCS) and hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS) were also applied.

Results

A significant association between HPDT and the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia (p<0.0001) was found. The expected change in area of secondary hyperalgesia due to a 1-degree increase in HPDT was estimated to be −27.38 cm2, 95% confidence interval (CI) of −37.77 to −16.98 cm2, with an R2 of 0.19. Likewise, a significant association between HADS-depression subscore and area of secondary hyperalgesia (p=0.046) was found, with an estimated expected change in secondary hyperalgesia to a 1-point increase in HADS-depression subscore of 11 cm2, 95% CI (0.19–21.82), and with R2 of 0.03. We found no significant associations between secondary hyperalgesia area and PCS score or pain during thermal stimulation.

Conclusion

HPDT and the area of secondary hyperalgesia after BTS are significantly associated; however, with an R2 of only 19%, HPDT only offers a modest explanation of the inter-participant variation in the size of the secondary hyperalgesia area elicited by BTS.

View correction statement:
Heat pain detection threshold is associated with the area of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization: a study of healthy male volunteers [Corrigendum]

Introduction

Clinical pain models may bridge the gap between animal and human research and may be applied in the investigation of pain sensitivity. Sufficient prediction of pain sensitivity, for example prior to surgery, may improve our ability to prevent severe acute and chronic pain following surgery,Citation1 as well improve the inclusion procedure in pharmaceutical drug trials by allowing initial grouping of participants in high- and low-pain responders.Citation2,Citation3

Current evidence suggest that the development of secondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli after a cutaneous heat injury in healthy volunteers is mediated by heat- and mechanosensitive type-I and/or mechanosensitive (heat-insensitive) A-fiber nociceptors, and is due to changes in the central nervous system, that is, central sensitization.Citation4Citation9 Central sensitization encompasses a functional change in neuron properties and nociceptive pathways, with increased membrane excitability and synaptic efficacy, and decreased synaptic inhibition resulting in increased and sometimes pathological responses to mechanical and noxious stimulation.Citation4,Citation9 The transcription-dependent long-lasting phase of central sensitization is assumed to play a key role in several pathological pain conditions, for example, osteoarthritis and fibromyalgia,Citation4,Citation9Citation11 and investigation of secondary hyperalgesia following a standardized burn injury may therefore provide insight into central sensitization.

Studies in healthy volunteers have indicated that the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia following standardized cutaneous sensitization procedures has a large inter-individual to intra-individual variance,Citation12,Citation13 is modifiable by certain analgesics,Citation14Citation18 and may be predictive of individual pain responses.Citation4,Citation9,Citation19,Citation20 The area of secondary hyperalgesia following the cutaneous heat pain model of brief thermal sensitization (BTS)Citation13,Citation14,Citation16,Citation18,Citation21,Citation22 quantified by monofilament stimulationCitation12Citation14,Citation16Citation18,Citation21Citation28 has been demonstrated to be a reproducible phenomenon that may be used in phenotype characterization of healthy volunteers.Citation12,Citation13

Heat pain detection threshold (HPDT) has been applied in several studies,Citation17,Citation18,Citation22,Citation25,Citation29Citation33 and the acute first pain elicited by the rapid heating of the skin is believed to be transmitted in A-fiber type-II mechano- and heat-sensitive nociceptors (in hairy skin), and mechano- and heat-sensitive C fibers.Citation5 HPDT has been proven to be reproducible,Citation34 and evidence suggests that HPDT may have a predictive value when investigating postoperative pain.Citation35,Citation36

However, the dissimilarities in physiologic properties between secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical pinprick stimulation and HPDT suggest that they may represent 2 distinctively different pain entities.

As a first step to explore secondary hyperalgesia following BTS and its potential predictive abilities, we aim to investigate the association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia induced by BTS. We hypothesized that HPDT and areas of secondary hyperalgesia were two predominantly independent entities, and that the area of secondary hyperalgesia was poorly explained by HPDT.

Methods

The study was approved by the local Danish Committee on Health Research Ethics for the Capital Region (Identifier: H-8-2014-012) and the Danish Data Protection Agency (Identifier: 30–1436); the study is also reported on the international database clinicaltrials.gov (Identifier: NCT02527395).

The design and methods of this prospective study is based upon a previous study done by Hansen et al;Citation13 moreover, an extensive description of the design and methods of this study has been published in a preceding methods paper, which is publicly available for review.Citation37

Study participants

Healthy male participants aged >18 and <35 years who could understand and speak the Danish language were included in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to inclusion, and all participants received EUR 20 (USD 27) per hour for their participation in the study. Participants were recruited by advertisement in the medical student magazine at Copenhagen University and online at www.forsøgspersoner.dk. Exclusion criteria were failure to cooperate with the tests, a weekly intake of >21 units of alcohol, consummation of >3 units of alcohol 24 hours before study day, substance abuse, intake of analgesics within 3 days before study day, intake of antihistamines 48 hours before study day, intake of prescription medicine and/or antidepressant medicine within 30 days before study day, neurological illnesses, chronic pain conditions, psychiatric diagnoses, tattoos on the extremities, eczema, wounds or sunburns at the sites of testing, and a body mass index (BMI) of >30 kg/m2 and <18 kg/m2.

Setting

The study was conducted in a quiet secluded room (temperature of 22°C–25°C), where only the investigator and the participant were present. The participants were placed in a supine position, on their back, throughout the assessments. The study was conducted during the time from 8 AM to 6 PM at the Department of Anesthesiology, 4231, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark in the period from October 1, 2015 to December 2, 2015.

Design

The study consisted of 1 screening/information day and 2 separate study days. To avoid a possible carry-over effect of the applied tests, the screening day and the 2 study days were separated with a minimum of 7 days.Citation13 Height, weight, arterial blood pressure, and pulse frequency of all participants were measured; moreover, data on age, right/left-handedness, and parental ethnicity were collected. On the 2 separate study days, the study participants were tested with 3 types of pain models: BTS, HPDT, and pain during 1-minute thermal stimulation (p-TS) in a predefined sequence (see pain models and randomization and allocation concealment). On the information day, the participants were provided with the psychological tests, pain catastrophizing scale (PCS)Citation38Citation40 and hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS)Citation41Citation43 (see psychological testing), which they completed at home and returned on the first study day in sealed opaque envelopes to ensure blinding. Opening of the envelopes was deferred until all participants had completed the study. All other assessments and tests were performed by the same investigator throughout the study (MSH).

Pain models

All pain testing was conducted with a computer-controlled thermode (MSA Thermotester), size 2.5×5 cm

Brief thermal sensitization (BTS)

BTS was induced by placing the computer-controlled thermode on the skin of the participant, centrally on the anterior part of the right thigh in the midline between the anterior superior iliac spine and the base of patella (). The starting temperature of the thermode was 32°C, and with an increase of 1°C/second, the thermode was heated to 45°C. After 3 minutes, the assessment of secondary hyperalgesia (see below) was conducted while the thermode at 45°C was still positioned on the skin of the participant.Citation14,Citation16,Citation18,Citation21,Citation22 The assessment of secondary hyperalgesia took ~1–2 minutes, resulting in a maximum duration of heat stimulation of 5 minutes.

Figure 1 Anatomical location of pain model testing.

Notes: HPDT was performed on the anterior part of the dominant lower arm, p-TS was performed on the anterior part of the non-dominant lower arm, and BTS was performed centrally on the anterior part of the right thigh in the midline between the anterior superior iliac spine and the base of patella.
Abbreviations: BTS, brief thermal sensitization; HPDT, heat pain detection threshold; p-TS, pain during thermal stimulation.
Figure 1 Anatomical location of pain model testing.

Assessment of secondary hyperalgesia

The area of secondary hyperalgesia was quantified after pinprick stimulation with a 19G monofilament (von Frey hair) in 4 linear paths arranged in 90° around the center of the thermode. Stimulation began well outside the area of secondary hyperalgesia, minimum 15 cm from the edge of the thermode, and advanced in steps of 5 mm/second toward the thermode. When the participant stated a clear change in sensation (intense burning, pricking, and tenderness), the spot was marked with a felt pen, and the longitudinal and transverse axes were measured with a pliable measuring tape for rectangular area calculation.Citation12Citation14,Citation16Citation18,Citation21Citation28

Heat pain detection threshold (HPDT)

HPDT was evaluated by placing the thermode on the skin of the participant on the anterior part of the dominant lower arm (). The start temperature of the thermode was 32°C and the temperature was then increased by 1°C/second. When the participant perceived the heat as painful he pressed a button, the temperature was registered, and the thermode returned to a temperature of 32°C. If a temperature of 52°C was reached, the thermode would automatically return to 32°C and 52°C would be registered as the threshold. The HPDT was estimated as an average of 4 separate stimulations with an interval of 6–10 seconds.Citation13,Citation14,Citation16Citation18,Citation22Citation25

Pain during thermal stimulation (p-TS)

The thermode was placed on the participant’s skin centrally on the anterior lower non-dominant arm (). The start temperature of the thermode was 32°C, and with an increase of 1°C/second, the thermode was heated to 45°C and remained 45°C for 1 minute. During the 1 minute heating of the skin the participant evaluated the pain using the electronic visual analog scale (VAS; Somedic USB-VAS), with an index of 0–100 mm, where 0 mm represented “no pain”, and 100 mm represented “worst pain imaginable”. The software provided with the electronic VAS automatically calculated an area under the curve (AUC) and a maximum VAS score for the time period. The participant was not able to see the computer screen during the assessment.Citation17,Citation22Citation25

Psychological testing

Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS)

PCS is 13-point questionnaire on a 5-point Likert scale with values from 0 to 4. The highest achievable score is 52, and the PCS can be subdivided into 3 sections that evaluate 1) rumination, 2) magnification, and 3) helplessness.Citation38Citation40

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)

HADS is a 14-point questionnaire on a 4-point Likert scale with values ranging from 0 to 3. The highest achievable score is 53, and the HADS can be subdivided into 2 sections that evaluate 1) anxiety and 2) depression.Citation41Citation43

Randomization and allocation concealment

The sequence of BTS and HPDT was randomized so that on 1 study day the sequence was: 1) BTS, 2) HPDT, and 3) p-TS, and on the other study day the sequence was: 1) HPDT, 2) BTS, and 3) p-TS. The randomization was performed with a computer-generated random allocation sequence, conducted by the Copenhagen Trial Unit, and stored in sealed opaque envelopes to secure adequate allocation concealment.

Test results and assessments for each study day were entered in a standardized case report form and placed in an opaque sealed envelope to ensure that the investigator was unable to see previous test results. Completed psychological tests were kept in sealed opaque envelopes and the blinding was first broken after all study participants had completed the study.

Outcome measures

Primary analysis

The association between HPDT and area of secondary hyperalgesia induced by BTS.

Secondary analyses

The association between area of secondary hyperalgesia induced by BTS and

  1. VAS-AUC following p-TS

  2. Max VAS-score following p-TS

  3. PCS-score

  4. HADS-score

  5. PCS and HADS subscales (PCS-rumination, PCS-magnification, PCS-helplessness, HADS-anxiety, and HADS-depression)

Sample size

A simulation-based sample size calculation was performed with data from our previous study;Citation13 and with an α of 0.05 and β of 0.01, we estimated that a number of 120 participants were needed in order to provide an empirical power of 99.9% (for further description see the published protocolCitation37). All simulation-based calculations were made using the open-source statistical programming environment R.Citation44

Statistical analysis

Individual levels of areas of secondary hyperalgesia, HPDT, VAS-AUC, and VAS-max were obtained as estimated best linear unbiased predictors (EBLUPS). The association between area of secondary hyperalgesia and HPDT was evaluated by multiple linear regression adjusting for individual body surface area. Models were validated graphically by means of residuals and QQ plots. Normality of residuals was assessed by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. The ability of HPDT to predict the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia was quantified by R2 and illustrated with prediction limits.

In a secondary analysis, we additionally included VAS-AUC, Max VAS-score following p-TS, PCS-score, and HADS-score as predictors in a multiple linear regression on area of secondary hyperalgesia. The importance of these predictors was assessed by backward elimination with a 5% cut-off level.

p-Values corresponded to F tests and were evaluated at a 5% significance level.

Additionally, 3 post hoc sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the robustness of the findings. In the first sensitivity analysis, further adjustment by age, weight, BMI, and mean arterial pressure (MAP) was performed. In the second sensitivity analysis, only right-handed participants were included; and finally, in the third sensitivity analysis, only ethnic Scandinavians were included.

Body surface area was calculated using the Mosteller formula.Citation45 Distributions of variables are summarized by medians and interquartile ranges. All analyses were made using the open-source statistical programming environment R.Citation44

Results

A sample of 131 healthy male volunteers was assessed for eligibility, and a total of 121 were included in the study (). All 121 study participants completed the study, and data from all the participants were analyzed for the primary and secondary outcome measures. Of the 121 participants, 12 had one or more parents with non-Scandinavian ethnicity and 16 were left-handed. The median interval between the 2 study days was 7 days (interquartile range [IQR], 7–8). Relevant data on the included participants’ characteristics are presented in . The median size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia was 447.78 cm2 (IQR, 346.19–528.99) and the median HPDT was 45.57°C (IQR, 43.79–46.61). Results from the p-TS, PCS, and HADS are presented in and . No adverse or serious adverse events were reported.

Figure 2 Flowchart of included study participants.

Figure 2 Flowchart of included study participants.

Table 1 Characteristics of included participants

Table 2 Results from pain model testing

Table 3 Psychological test scores, total, and subscores

Primary analysis

We found a significant association between HPDT and the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia (p<0.0001). We estimated the expected change in area of secondary hyperalgesia due to a 1-degree increase in HPDT to −27.38 cm2 with a 95% confidence interval (CI) of −37.77 to −16.98 cm2. The R2 was calculated to 0.19, and the prediction limits at a given HPDT of 46°C and body surface of 1.99 m2 were estimated to 167.42–656.07 cm2 ().

Figure 3 Predictions of areas of secondary hyperalgesia (following BTS) by HPDT.

Notes: Points correspond to individual participant measurement of secondary hyperalgesia areas. The solid line corresponds to the predictions of secondary hyperalgesia areas and HPDT, and the dashed line corresponds to 95% prediction limits.
Abbreviations: BTS, brief thermal sensitization; HPDT, heat pain detection threshold.
Figure 3 Predictions of areas of secondary hyperalgesia (following BTS) by HPDT.

Secondary analyses

We found a significant association between HADS-depression score and area of secondary hyperalgesia (p=0.046). The estimated expected change in secondary hyperalgesia area to a 1-point increase in HADS-depression was 11 cm2 (95% CI, 0.19–21.82; R2, 0.03). No significant associations were found in any of the other secondary outcome measures.

Post hoc analyses

The 3 post hoc sensitivity analyses did not demonstrate noticeably different results when compared to our primary analysis.

Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to investigate the association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia elicited by BTS. We demonstrated a significant association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia, where increasing levels of HDPT were associated with decreasing sizes of secondary hyperalgesia areas. In addition to the highly significant association, we found an R2 of 19%, illustrating that HPDT only offers a modest explanation of the inter-participant variation in secondary hyperalgesia following BTS. The estimated prediction interval for areas of secondary hyperalgesia at an HPDT of 46°C and a body surface of 1.99 m2 were estimated to 167.42–656.07 cm2 (), indicating that although we find a highly significant result, HPDT and areas of secondary hyperalgesia are only modestly associated. Likewise, we also found a significant association between increasing HADS-depression subscore and increasing size of secondary hyperalgesia area; however, R2 was estimated to 3%, and in this study, HADS-depression subscore only offered a very modest explanation for the variation in the area of secondary hyperalgesia.

We have applied an experimental pain model (BTS) with a high reliability (intraobserver intra-participant correlation of 0.85).Citation13 Moreover, our post hoc sensitivity analyses did not demonstrate noticeable differences compared to our primary analysis, illustrating the robustness of our results. In our study, BMI, age, MAP, left-handedness, and ethnicity did not have any influence on the association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical punctate stimuli. The high number of included participants provides an empirical power of 99.9%, which practically eliminates the risk of type-II errors and once again illustrates the robustness of our results.

The results in this study confirm the results from our previous study where a significant association was demonstrated with an R2 of 20%.Citation13 Likewise, in our current study we find a high inter-participant difference in areas of secondary hyperalgesia ranging from 135 to 788 cm2 (), as well as high inter-participant differences in HPDT ranging from 38.7°C to 51.02°C ().

The weak association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia area is noteworthy because it has been suggested that both parameters may to some extent be important in categorizing pain sensitivity; however, evidence on the predictive value of these parameters is contradicting with diverse results both forCitation20,Citation46Citation48 and againstCitation31,Citation36 HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia areas as predictors of pain sensitivity. The physiologic properties in the neural mediation of HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia may, in part, account for the weak association. HPDT is primarily mediated by A-fiber type-II mechano- and heat-sensitive nociceptors, and mechano- and heat-sensitive C fibers, and secondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli is mediated by heat- and mechano-sensitive type-I and mechanosensitive (heat-insensitive) A-fiber nociceptors.Citation5Citation7 In a recent study, results even suggested that secondary hyperalgesia was mediated only by heat-insensitive mechanosensitive A-fiber nociceptors.Citation8 Finally, it is believed that the development of secondary hyperalgesia is caused by central sensitization due to changes in the central nervous system,Citation4,Citation7,Citation9 which leads to the suggestion that HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical punctate stimuli may be 2 distinctively different pain entities. A biological explanation, although speculative, may be that HPDT represent an acute warning system against nociceptive stimuli, whereas secondary hyperalgesia represents a somewhat later occurrence of sensitization of central neurons, which may serve other purposes in the nociceptive process.

Studies have demonstrated that patients suffering from persistent pain due to rheumatoid arthritis or fibromyalgia display larger areas of secondary hyperalgesia when compared with healthy individuals.Citation49,Citation50 Likewise, clinical studies have indicated that increasing sizes of secondary hyperalgesia areas surrounding surgical wounds are associated with an increased risk of developing chronic pain following surgery.Citation20,Citation51 These findings indicate that a large area of secondary hyperalgesia is found in persons with high levels of central sensitization. Thus, the investigation of secondary hyperalgesia areas may provide insight in individual levels of central sensitization. With Woolf’s description of a central sensitization syndrome,Citation4 where pain hypersensitivity syndromes may share common contributions of central sensitization, the investigation of secondary hyperalgesia may provide insight into already known pain hypersensitivity syndromes, and may also contribute to the phenotyping of pain sensitivity in healthy persons. A recent brain magnetic resonance imaging study of healthy volunteers indicated that participants with differences in areas of secondary hyperalgesia exhibited structural and functional differences when comparing healthy participants with a large vs small area of secondary hyperalgesia,Citation52 suggesting differences in sensory discrimination, pain suppression, and avoidance behavior. However, the practical applicability of secondary hyperalgesia areas is not yet fully understood or described, and thorough investigations of central sensitization, as well as factors influencing individual propensity to develop central sensitization may have a role in the future of analgesic therapy and pain research.

Contrary to our previous study,Citation13 we found a significant association between increasing HADS-depression subscore and increasing size of secondary hyperalgesia area. However, R2 was estimated to 3%, and in this study, HADS-depression subscore only offered a very modest explanation for the variation in secondary hyperalgesia areas. Several clinical studies have demonstrated significant associations between postoperative pain and personality traits, such as depression, anxiety, and pain catastrophizing.Citation53Citation56 Moreover, in a study by Salomons et al,Citation57 it was demonstrated that pain-focused cognitive training reduced the area of secondary hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. However, in a recently published review it was concluded that the influence of psychological variables on experimental pain responses is still largely unclear.Citation58 Our very strict inclusion criteria, that specifically excluded women, chronic pain patients, and persons with prior psychological history, may have resulted in a sampling bias that reduced the inter-individual variance of secondary hyperalgesia areas, PCS (IQR, 7–17), and HADS score (IQR, 3–8.5), and could be responsible for the weak association between HADS, HPDT, and secondary hyperalgesia. A sufficient investigation of psychological variables and pain should attempt to conduct consecutive inclusion of patients prior to, for example, surgery or restricted inclusion of volunteers with high psychiatric vulnerability.

Our study has some limitations. 1) As emphasized before, we applied very strict inclusion criteria, and consequently, included a very homogenous population; inclusion of, for example, females and chronic pain patients could potentially have increased the inter-individual variance and resulted in a higher R2. However, individual characteristics, such as sex,Citation25,Citation59Citation62 obesity,Citation63 and menstrual hormone cycle,Citation64 may potentially influence pain thresholds and sensitivity and to accommodate for all these variables, hereby minimizing the unknown factors of variation, and to focus only on the association between HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia areas, we chose to apply very strict inclusion criteria. Additionally, BTS has only been validated in healthy male volunteers,Citation13 and consequently, the results of this study only apply to young and healthy, male volunteers.

2) We did not evaluate dietary intake, stress and hormone levels, genetics, brain anatomy, or skin receptor density of the included participants. Studies have suggested that diets high on tryptophan,Citation65,Citation66 high stress levels of serum cortisol and testosterone,Citation67 and even certain genetic markersCitation68Citation72 may influence the pain sensitivity. An inter-participant differentiable diet and hormone level, as well as differences in stress levels, genetics, and brain anatomy could be explanatory factors of the high inter-individual variance of HPDT and areas of secondary hyperalgesia.

Finally, 3 patients reported HPDTs well outside the interquartile ranges, 2 patients <40°C and 1 patient >50°C, which may indicate that they misunderstood the procedure.

In our study, HPDT only offered a modest explanation of the inter-individual size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia; and the inter-individual differences in secondary hyperalgesia observed in numerous studies remain largely unexplained. Studies investigating postoperative pain and secondary hyperalgesia before and after surgery could provide insight on the predictive value of secondary hyperalgesia areas, and finally, as secondary hyperalgesia is believed to occur as a result of central neuronal plasticity,Citation4,Citation9 future research should attempt to investigate variables in the central nervous system in both patients and healthy participants, with modalities such as structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography, and magnetoencephalography.

In conclusion, our study demonstrated a statistically significant association between HPDT and the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia. However, with an R2 of only 19%, HPDT offers only a modest explanation of the inter-participant variation in the size of the secondary hyperalgesia area elicited by BTS.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Augustinus foundation (grant number: 14-3907), Toyota Fonden – Denmark (grant number: OH/BG-8610), and the Aase and Ejnar Danielsen’s foundation (grant number: 10-001341). The funders had no role in the conception or design of the study, the collection, analysis or interpretation of data, the writing of the report, or on the decision to publish the results.

The authors would like to thank illustrator Sarah Egbert Eiersholt for her valuable contribution to the artwork in .

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • WernerMUMjoboHNNielsenPRRudinAPrediction of postoperative pain: a systematic review of predictive experimental pain studiesAnesthesiology201011261494150220460988
  • Arendt-NielsenLHoeckHCOptimizing the early phase development of new analgesics by human pain biomarkersExpert Rev Neurother201111111631165122014141
  • ChizhBAPriestleyTRowbothamMSchafflerKPredicting therapeutic efficacy – experimental pain in human subjectsBrain Res Rev200960124325419168094
  • WoolfCJCentral sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of painPain2011152Suppl 3S2S1520961685
  • DubinAEPatapoutianANociceptors: the sensors of the pain pathwayJ Clin Invest2010120113760377221041958
  • TreedeRDChapter 1 Pain and hyperalgesia: definitions and theoriesHandb Clin Neurol20068131018808823
  • ZieglerEAMagerlWMeyerRATreedeRDSecondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli. Central sensitization to A-fibre nociceptor inputBrain1999122Pt 122245225710581220
  • van den BroekeENLenoirCMourauxASecondary hyperalgesia is mediated by heat-insensitive A-fibre nociceptorsJ Physiol2016594226767677627377467
  • LatremoliereAWoolfCJCentral sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticityJ Pain200910989592619712899
  • Arendt-NielsenLYarnitskyDExperimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and visceraJ Pain200910655657219380256
  • EdwardsRRIndividual differences in endogenous pain modulation as a risk factor for chronic painNeurology200565343744316087910
  • WernerMUPetersenKLRowbothamMCDahlJBHealthy volunteers can be phenotyped using cutaneous sensitization pain modelsPLoS One201385e6273323671631
  • HansenMSWetterslevJPipperCBOstervigRAsgharMSDahlJBThe area of secondary hyperalgesia following heat stimulation in healthy male volunteers: inter- and intra-individual variance and reproducibilityPLoS One2016115e015528427167119
  • FrymoyerARRowbothamMCPetersenKLPlacebo-controlled comparison of a morphine/dextromethorphan combination with morphine on experimental pain and hyperalgesia in healthy volunteersJ Pain200781192517113353
  • MathiesenOImbimboBPHilstedKLFabbriLDahlJBCHF3381, a N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist and monoamine oxidase-A inhibitor, attenuates secondary hyperalgesia in a human pain modelJ Pain20067856557416885013
  • PetersenKLIyengarSChappellASSafety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and effects on human experimental pain of the selective ionotropic glutamate receptor 5 (iGluR5) antagonist LY545694 in healthy volunteersPain2014155592993624486883
  • PetersenKLJonesBSegredoVDahlJBRowbothamMCEffect of remifentanil on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat–capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteersAnesthesiology2001941152011135717
  • PetersenKLMeadoffTPressSPetersMMLeComteMDRowbothamMCChanges in morphine analgesia and side effects during daily subcutaneous administration in healthy volunteersPain2008137239540417977662
  • YouDSCreechSKMeagherMWEnhanced area of secondary hyperalgesia in Women with multiple stressful life events: a pilot studyPain Med Epub201647
  • MartinezVBen AmmarSJudetTBouhassiraDChauvinMFletcherDRisk factors predictive of chronic postsurgical neuropathic pain: the value of the iliac crest bone harvest modelPain201215371478148322560289
  • DirksJPetersenKLDahlJBThe heat/capsaicin sensitization model: a methodologic studyJ Pain20034312212814622709
  • DirksJPetersenKLRowbothamMCDahlJBGabapentin suppresses cutaneous hyperalgesia following heat-capsaicin sensitizationAnesthesiology200297110210712131110
  • CavalloneLFFreyKMontanaMCReproducibility of the heat/capsaicin skin sensitization model in healthy volunteersJ Pain Res2013677178424232380
  • DirksJPetersenKLRowbothamMCDahlJBEffect of systemic adenosine on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteersReg Anesth Pain Med200126541441911561260
  • JensenMTPetersenKLGender differences in pain and secondary hyperalgesia after heat/capsaicin sensitization in healthy volunteersJ Pain20067321121716516827
  • MikkelsenSDirksJFabriciusPPetersenKLRowbothamMCDahlJBEffect of intravenous magnesium on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteersBr J Anaesth200186687187311573598
  • PetersenKLBrennumJDahlJBExperimental evaluation of the analgesic effect of ibuprofen on primary and secondary hyperalgesiaPain1997702–31671749150290
  • PetersenKLRowbothamMCA new human experimental pain model: the heat/capsaicin sensitization modelNeuroreport19991071511151610380972
  • ReddyKSNaiduMURaniPURaoTRHuman experimental pain models: a review of standardized methods in drug developmentJ Res Med Sci201217658759523626642
  • StaahlCOlesenAEAndresenTArendt-NielsenLDrewesAMAssessing efficacy of non-opioid analgesics in experimental pain models in healthy volunteers: an updated reviewBr J Clin Pharmacol200968332234119740390
  • WernerMUDuunPKehletHPrediction of postoperative pain by preoperative nociceptive responses to heat stimulationAnesthesiology20041001115119 discussion 115A14695732
  • ManitpisitkulPMayorgaAShalaydaKSafety, tolerability and pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic learnings from a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, sequential group first-in-human study of the TRPV1 antagonist, JNJ-38893777, in Healthy MenClin Drug Investig2015356353363
  • WangHPapoiuADCoghillRCPatelTWangNYosipovitchGEthnic differences in pain, itch and thermal detection in response to topical capsaicin: African Americans display a notably limited hyperalgesia and neurogenic inflammationBr J Dermatol201016251023102920030637
  • KhambamSKNaiduMURaniPURaoTRA simple contact heat experimental pain model for evaluation of analgesic agents in healthy volunteersCurr Ther Res Clin Exp201172623324224648591
  • GottrupHAndersenJArendt-NielsenLJensenTSPsychophysical examination in patients with post-mastectomy painPain200087327528410963907
  • WrightAMossPSloanKAbnormal quantitative sensory testing is associated with persistent pain one year after TKAClin Orthop Relat Res2015473124625425293497
  • HansenMSWetterslevJPipperCBAsgharMSDahlJBIs heat pain detection threshold associated with the area of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization? A study of healthy volunteers – design and detailed plan of analysisBMC Anesthesiol20161612827246322
  • SullivanMJLBishopSRPivikJThe pain catastrophizing scale: development and validationPsychol Assess199574524532
  • LeungLPain catastrophizing: an updated reviewIndian J Psychol Med201234320421723441031
  • Van DammeSCrombezGBijttebierPGoubertLVan HoudenhoveBA confirmatory factor analysis of the pain catastrophizing scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populationsPain200296331932411973004
  • BjellandIDahlAAHaugTTNeckelmannDThe validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. An updated literature reviewJ Psychosom Res2002522697711832252
  • NortonSCoscoTDoyleFDoneJSackerAThe hospital anxiety and depression scale: a meta confirmatory factor analysisJ Psychosom Res2013741748123272992
  • ZigmondASSnaithRPThe hospital anxiety and depression scaleActa Psychiatr Scand19836763613706880820
  • R Core TeamR: A Language and Environment for Statistical ComputingVienna, AustriaR Foundation for Statistical Computing V Available from: http://www.R-project.org/
  • MostellerRDSimplified calculation of body-surface areaN Engl J Med19873171710983657876
  • RudinAErikssonLLiedholmRListTWernerMUPrediction of postoperative pain after mandibular third molar surgeryJ Orofac Pain201024218919620401357
  • RudinAWolner-HanssenPHellbomMWernerMUPrediction of post-operative pain after a laparoscopic tubal ligation procedureActa Anaesthesiol Scand200852793894518477083
  • RavnPFrederiksenRSkovsenAPChristrupLLWernerMUPrediction of pain sensitivity in healthy volunteersJ Pain Res2012531332623055774
  • MorrisVCruwysSKiddBIncreased capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia as a marker of abnormal sensory activity in patients with fibromyalgiaNeurosci Lett199825032052079708868
  • MorrisVHCruwysSCKiddBLCharacterisation of capsaicin-induced mechanical hyperalgesia as a marker for altered nociceptive processing in patients with rheumatoid arthritisPain19977121791869211479
  • SalengrosJCHuybrechtsIDucartADifferent anesthetic techniques associated with different incidences of chronic post-thoracotomy pain: low-dose remifentanil plus presurgical epidural analgesia is preferable to high-dose remifentanil with postsurgical epidural analgesiaJ Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth201024460861620005744
  • AsgharMSPereiraMPWernerMUMartenssonJLarssonHBDahlJBSecondary hyperalgesia phenotypes exhibit differences in brain activation during noxious stimulationPLoS One2015101e011484025615578
  • GranotMFerberSGThe roles of pain catastrophizing and anxiety in the prediction of postoperative pain intensity: a prospective studyClin J Pain200521543944516093750
  • PapaioannouMSkapinakisPDamigosDMavreasVBroumasGPalgimesiAThe role of catastrophizing in the prediction of postoperative painPain Med20091081452145919863742
  • VaughnFWichowskiHBosworthGDoes preoperative anxiety level predict postoperative pain?AORN J200785358960417352896
  • Hinrichs-RockerASchulzKJarvinenILeferingRSimanskiCNeugebauerEAPsychosocial predictors and correlates for chronic post-surgical pain (CPSP) – a systematic reviewEur J Pain200913771973018952472
  • SalomonsTVMoayediMErpeldingNDavisKDA brief cognitive-behavioural intervention for pain reduces secondary hyperalgesiaPain201415581446145224569149
  • HansenMSHorjales-AraujoEDahlJBAssociations between psychological variables and pain in experimental pain models. A systematic reviewActa Anaesthesiol Scand20155991094110226088747
  • AlabasOATashaniOATabasamGJohnsonMIGender role affects experimental pain responses: a systematic review with meta-analysisEur J Pain20121691211122322434689
  • BartleyEJFillingimRBSex differences in pain: a brief review of clinical and experimental findingsBr J Anaesth20131111525823794645
  • CampesiIFoisMFranconiFSex and gender aspects in anesthetics and pain medicationHandb Exp Pharmacol2012214265278
  • MogilJSSex differences in pain and pain inhibition: multiple explanations of a controversial phenomenonNat Rev Neurosci2012131285986623165262
  • OkifujiAHareBDThe association between chronic pain and obesityJ Pain Res2015839940826203274
  • IacovidesSAvidonIBakerFCDoes pain vary across the menstrual cycle? A reviewEur J Pain201519101389140525899177
  • LiebermanHRCorkinSSpringBJGrowdonJHWurtmanRJMood, performance, and pain sensitivity: changes induced by food constituentsJ Psychiatr Res19821721351456764930
  • AbbottFVEtiennePFranklinKBMorganMJSewitchMJYoungSNAcute tryptophan depletion blocks morphine analgesia in the cold-pressor test in humansPsychopharmacology (Berl)19921081–260661410147
  • ChoiJCChungMILeeYDModulation of pain sensation by stress-related testosterone and cortisolAnaesthesia201267101146115122804789
  • DiatchenkoLSladeGDNackleyAGGenetic basis for individual variations in pain perception and the development of a chronic pain conditionHum Mol Genet200514113514315537663
  • NielsenCSStubhaugAPriceDDVassendOCzajkowskiNHarrisJRIndividual differences in pain sensitivity: genetic and environmental contributionsPain20081361–2212917692462
  • NorburyTAMacGregorAJUrwinJSpectorTDMcMahonSBHeritability of responses to painful stimuli in women: a classical twin studyBrain2007130Pt 113041304917932101
  • TegederIAdolphJSchmidtHWoolfCJGeisslingerGLotschJReduced hyperalgesia in homozygous carriers of a GTP cyclohydrolase 1 haplotypeEur J Pain20081281069107718374612
  • WilliamsFMScollenSCaoDGenes contributing to pain sensitivity in the normal population: an exome sequencing studyPLoS Genet2012812e100309523284290