549
Views
61
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

A systematic review of sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect

&
Pages 1831-1839 | Published online: 31 Jul 2017

Abstract

Objectives

The present review investigated whether there are systematic sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect.

Methods

A literature search was conducted in multiple electronic databases. Studies were included if the study compared a group or condition where a placebo was administered to a natural history group or similar cohort.

Results

Eighteen studies were identified – 12 on placebo effects and 6 on nocebo effects. Chi-square tests revealed that 1) males responded more strongly to placebo treatment, and females responded more strongly to nocebo treatment, and 2) males responded with larger placebo effects induced by verbal information, and females responded with larger nocebo effects induced by conditioning procedures.

Conclusion

This review indicates that there are sex differences in the placebo and nocebo effects, probably caused by sex differences in stress, anxiety, and the endogenous opioid system.

Introduction

The placebo effect is a psychological and/or physiological response that follows administration of inert substances or treatments.Citation1 For example, an inactive medication administered together with information that it is an analgesic medication has been found to decrease pain and pain-related physiological reactions.Citation2 The nocebo effect is defined as increased pain or other symptoms after administration of an inactive treatment purported to increase pain or unpleasant symptoms.Citation3

Placebo and nocebo effects have mostly been studied in the field of pain, and sex differences in placebo analgesiaCitation4,Citation5 and nocebo hyperalgesiaCitation3,Citation6 have been reported repeatedly. There are sex differences in pain, with males often reporting lower pain to a standard stimulus.Citation7 Furthermore, males often have a higher pain thresholdCitation8 and tolerance than females,Citation9 and several pain conditions and pain-related symptoms are more frequent among femalesCitation10,Citation11 – for example, musculoskeletal painCitation12 and fibromyalgia.Citation13 Thus, because there are sex differences in experimental and clinical pain, we hypothesize that sex differences exist also in placebo and nocebo responses.

Sex differences in placebo and nocebo responses could be due to sex differences in the underlying psychological or physiological processes mediating placebo and nocebo effects. Placebo analgesia is partly due to activation of a descending pain-modulatory system that involves endogenous opioid activation,Citation14 and nocebo hyperalgesia is partly mediated through activation of endogenous pronociceptive systems involving cholecystokinin (CCK)Citation15,Citation16 and HPA hyperactivity.Citation17 CCK has an inhibitory effect on placebo analgesia and an excitatory effect on nocebo hyperalgesia.Citation16 Therefore, any sex differences in placebo and nocebo responses may reflect sex differences in these systems.

Endogenous pain modulation obtained by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNICs) – where pain to one stimulus is reduced by application of a second painful stimulus – has been found to reduce pain more in males than in females.Citation18 Thus, data suggest that activation of endogenous pain modulation reduces pain more in males than in females, which is consistent with stronger placebo analgesic responses in males compared to females. Stress-induced analgesia (SIA) is also mediated by activation of the descending inhibitory pain pathway, and SIA may take place during or following exposure to stress- or fear-inducing situations.Citation19 Males exhibit stronger SIA than females,Citation20 and animal studies have shown that SIA may be partially reversed by opioid blockade in males, but not in females.Citation21

Aslaksen et al reported that placebos reduced stress more in males than in females, and, when controlling for the effect of stress, the sex differences in placebo analgesia disappeared.Citation4 Thus, sex differences in placebo analgesia may be explained by sex differences in stress levels. Potential explanations are that there are sex differences in the pain modulatory system involving endogenous opioids and CCK as well as sex differences in stress responses.

Sex differences in placebo and nocebo responding have also been found in domains other than pain, such as in cognitive performance,Citation22 dopaminergic function,Citation23 nausea,Citation24 and mental distress.Citation25 Thus, the observed sex differences in placebo and nocebo responding could also be due to sex differences in information processing. Klosterhalfen et al investigated differences in nocebo responding due to the induction method.Citation24 The results revealed that females responded with larger nocebo responses than males when a conditioning procedure was used, whereas males responded with larger nocebo responses than females when a verbal procedure was used. All types of placebo and nocebo effects are mediated through the same psychological processes, namely through expectancies and/or via conditioning procedures. Therefore, the present study investigated whether there are sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect, and whether sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect are due to the induction method.

The present review hypothesized that 1) placebo responses would be stronger or more frequently observed in males than in females, 2) nocebo responses would be stronger or more frequently observed in females than in males, 3) verbally induced placebo responses would be more frequently observed in males than in females, and 4) conditioned nocebo responses would be more frequently observed in females than in males.

Methods

Search procedure

Searches in the Medline, PsycINFO, Embase and ISI databases (Web of Science) were conducted until July 29, 2016. “Sex” refers to biological differences between males and females. “Gender,” on the other hand, refers to socially created differences between males and females. However, the terms have not been used consistently across studies, and both terms were included in the searches, although sex differences are the focus of this review. Two searches that included Boolean search terms were conducted in each of the chosen databases. One search was conducted for the placebo and nocebo effects, and one search was conducted for placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia (). A review protocol does not exist, but a list of the excluded articles can be forwarded by contacting the corresponding author (SMV).

Table 1 Search terms used in the literature search

Data extraction

One author (SMV) extracted data from the included studies. The other author (MAF) checked these data. The search resulted in 1,635 hits. Only studies with a natural history control group or condition were included, to ascertain that any changes in symptoms after placebo administration were placebo or nocebo effects. A placebo response was defined as the difference in the response in a condition where information was provided that effective treatment had been administered and the response in a condition where this information was not presented and symptoms were only monitored. Studies were excluded if a placebo response was defined only as the difference between a pretest and a posttest in the same group. Studies which 1) included a group or condition where placebo was administered with information that the placebo had therapeutic effects and a natural history group or condition where no treatment was provided and the symptom was only observed, or 2) administered the same amount of drug to all participants, but where different types of information about the drug were provided to different groups or conditions were included. Studies where the placebo response or the placebo effect was not the primary outcome were also included. Studies reporting significant sex differences in the placebo or the nocebo effect were included. Thus, “a larger effect,” henceforth, refers to significant sex differences in the placebo or the nocebo effect.

Included studies were categorized according to design, number of participants, healthy volunteers or patients, induction method, target disorder or symptom, and outcome. The induction method was differentiated into verbal information and conditioning. Conditioning as induction method was defined as placebo or nocebo treatments induced through 1) response conditioning, for example, by surreptitiously reducing the intensity of a painful stimuli, or 2) response conditioning by social observational modeling.

Statistics

Chi-square tests were conducted to test the association between 1) placebo and nocebo effects and sex, and 2) induction method and sex.

Results

Eighteen studies satisfied the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Further details about the identification, inclusion, and exclusion process are displayed in . References for the included studies are presented in Box S1. Eight studies investigated placebo analgesia, and two studies investigated nocebo hyperalgesia. The remaining eight studies investigated cognitive performance, mental distress, dopaminergic functioning, irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) symptoms, nausea, and reverse reactions to pharmacological treatments. Variables such as emotions and stress were included when they were relevant for the interpretation of the results. The studies are summarized in and presented with full references in Appendix 1.

Figure 1 PRISMA flow diagram.

Figure 1 PRISMA flow diagram.

Table 2 Included studies

Among the 12 placebo studies, only males responded with a placebo effect, or with a larger placebo effect than females, in eight studies.

The association between sex and placebo and nocebo responding was significant (χ2 [1, N=18], p=0.046), indicating that more studies showed a placebo effect in males compared to females, and more studies showed a nocebo effect in females compared to males.

Additionally, the association between induction method and sex in placebo and nocebo responding was significant (χ2 [1, N=18], p=0.023). Thus, verbal information induced placebo effects more frequently in males, whereas classical conditioning more frequently induced nocebo effects in females.

Discussion

The results showed that placebo responses were more often seen in males compared to females, whereas nocebo responses were more often observed in females than in males. Moreover, verbally induced placebo responses were more frequent in males than in females, whereas conditioned nocebo responses were more frequent in females than in males. There are several potential explanations for these sex differences. In the following text, we argue that sex differences in placebo and nocebo responding are due to sex differences in 1) affective responses to placebo and nocebo treatment, and 2) underlying psychophysiological mechanisms.

Sex differences in the placebo effect

The finding that males displayed placebo responses more frequently than females may be due to sex differences in psychophysiological mechanisms involved in stress and anxiety. Increased negative emotions may induce increased painCitation26 and result in nocebo responding. Activation of endogenous pain modulatory processes by stress, as in SIA,Citation19 or by DNICCitation18 have been reported to reduce pain more in males than in females. Both these procedures involve increased stress. Placebo analgesia, on the other hand, is partly due to decreased stress.Citation4 The association between reduced stress and reduced pain report in males, but not in females, after placebo administration could suggest that males are more responsive to stress-related endogenous pain modulatory processes than females. McCubbin et al showed that the effect of relaxation training was blocked by administration of naloxone, indicating that the effect of relaxation training was mediated by opioids.Citation27 Thus, opioids are activated by both increased and decreased stress. Koepp et al used positron emission tomography and observed reduced binding of a μ-receptor agonist in subjects that experienced positive emotions.Citation28 The finding indicated that positive emotions were related to increased opioid activity. Thus, decreased stress and increased positive emotions have been associated with increased opioid activity, and this could be part of the mechanism underlying placebo analgesia. Accordingly, sex differences in this mechanism may partly explain the sex differences in placebo responding reported in this review.

The finding that vasopressin enhances the magnitude of the placebo effect and decreases stress levels in females, but not in males,Citation29 supports the hypothesis that sex differences in the placebo effect are explained by sex differences in stress and the underlying neurophysiological and neurobiological mechanisms. Vasopressin is involved in processing and regulation of social behaviors, and it has been reported that males and females respond with different brain activation and behavior after vasopressin administration. Colloca et alCitation29 administered intranasal vasopressin as placebo treatment and uncovered that females with the lowest cortisol and anxiety levels responded with the largest vasopressin-induced placebo effects. This suggests that high levels of stress and negative emotions in females are central for the explanation of sex differences in the placebo effect. Kessner et al included only males in a study investigating the effect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia and reported that oxytocin increased the placebo analgesic response in males.Citation30 As vasopressin, oxytocin is involved in regulation of social behaviors. Pharmacological interventions for investigating placebo and nocebo effects represent a novel and promising approach, also for the understanding of sex differences in placebo and nocebo effects.

Sex differences in the nocebo effect

The relationship between high levels of anxiety and high levels of pain is stronger in females than in malesCitation31,Citation32 and may explain the observations that females respond more often and stronger to nocebo hyperalgesic treatment than males. Swider and Babel showed that social observational modeling induced larger nocebo hyperalgesic responses in females than in males and suggested that this difference was due to larger increases in anxiety in females than in males.Citation6

Previous negative drug experiences may enhance nocebo responding. Liccardi et al investigated adverse reactions after oral administration of placebo in patients that previously had reported adverse reactions to different medications.Citation33 It was found that adverse reactions to placebo were significantly more frequent in females compared to males. Ströhle investigated sex differences in responses to placebo and sodium lactate in patients diagnosed with panic disorder.Citation34 Infusion of sodium lactate increases anxiety levels and may generate panic attacks in patients with panic disorder. Females diagnosed with panic disorder responded with higher anxiety to infusion of placebo, compared to males with panic disorder. However, the studies by Liccardi et alCitation33 and StröhleCitation34 did not include a natural history control condition and only suggest that females display larger increases in anxiety after a nocebo has been administered, compared to males.

Sex differences in the placebo and nocebo effect due to the induction method

Klosterhalfen et al showed that females responded more strongly than males to conditioned stimuli, whereas males responded more strongly than females toward verbal suggestions.Citation24 This hypothesis fits well with findings from the present review. Activation of the endogenous opioid system mediates placebo analgesia, and naloxone inhibits or abolishes placebo analgesic responses. However, naloxone does not reduce all types of placebo responses. Therefore, not all placebo responses are mediated through the endogenous opioid system. Amanzio and Benedetti reported that placebo analgesia induced by verbal information was reduced by naloxone, and that placebo analgesia induced by classical conditioning was naloxone-insensitive if the previously administered drug was non-opioid.Citation35 This suggests that verbally induced placebo responses are mediated by the endogenous opioid system, whereas conditioned placebo responses are mediated by other mechanisms. Thus, the stronger verbally induced placebo responses in males may be due to a more effective endogenous opioid system in males, compared to females.

An alternative hypothesis for the sex differences in nocebo responses is that adverse drug reactions (nocebo responses) may be a form of compensatory conditioned response,Citation36 but there is no evidence that these are stronger in females.Citation37 Developing a method for measuring information processing in the context of placebo effects may be valuable for future investigations of sex differences. Knowledge of how previous experiences with medications or treatments influence the placebo effect in males and females could contribute to the understanding of the observed sex differences in placebo responding. It is suggested that future studies report previous experience with medications or treatments.

A recent meta-analysis that included randomized controlled trials on psychiatric treatment found no sex differences in the placebo arm of these trials.Citation38 However, the included studies lacked a natural history group. Thus, as several processes other than expectations could be acting in those control groups, that meta-analysis is not relevant for the present review. Although performing a meta-analysis had been preferable in the present study, the required data were not reported in several of the included studies.

There are several limitations to this study. Five of the included studies came from the same laboratory. Two of the included studies were investigating the placebo effect in patients, whereas the remaining 16 investigated placebo or nocebo effects in healthy volunteers. Additionally, the included studies investigated placebo or nocebo effects in several different symptoms (pain, nausea, cognitive performance, mental distress, dopaminergic functioning, IBS symptoms, and side effects). Furthermore, although the placebo and nocebo effect often are defined as similar but opposing effects, producing, for example, opposite effects on pain report and brain activation,Citation39 it cannot be precluded that the results from the present review are not confounded. Another limitation is the small number of included studies, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. Moreover, most studies on the placebo and nocebo effect do not report on sex differences, possibly due to a lack of examining sex differences, or absence of sex differences. Consequently, there is a potential for negative or unreported data on sex differences in placebo and nocebo effects. Thus, the findings from the present study might be biased or underpowered. It should also be noted that studies have reported that sex differences in pain reporting are due to the sex of the experimenter. However, although some journals require authors to report experimenter sex, none of the included studies in the present review investigated the contribution of the sex of the experimenter. To increase the understanding of the repeatedly observed sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect, it is suggested that future studies report on sex differences in the placebo and nocebo effect as well as the sex of the experimenter.

Conclusion

This study suggests that placebo responses are more often observed in males than in females, and that nocebo responses are more often observed in females than in males. Additionally, verbally induced placebo responses are observed more often in males compared to females, whereas conditioned nocebo responses are observed more often in females compared to males. The observed sex differences in placebo responding are probably due to larger stress reduction in males compared to females. The sex differences in nocebo responding may be explained by larger increases in stress and anxiety in females than in males. Furthermore, endogenous opioid transmission has been reported to be more effective in males compared to females and may, therefore, explain the observed sex differences in placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by a grant from the Bial Foundation (grant no. 186/10) and the UiT, University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway.

Supplementary materials

Box S1

Reference list of the included studies

  • AbramsKKushnerMG2004The moderating effects of tension-reduction alcohol outcome expectancies on placebo responding in individuals with social phobiaAddictive Behaviors291221122415236826
  • AslaksenPMLybyPS2015Fear of pain potentiates nocebo hyperalgesiaJournal of Pain Research870371026491370
  • AslaksenPMBystadMVambheimSMFlatenMA2011Gender differences in placebo analgesia: event-related potentials and emotional modulationPsychosomatic Medicine7319319921217098
  • AslaksenPMFlatenMA2008The roles of physiological and subjective stress in the effectiveness of a placebo on experimentally induced painPsychosomatic Medicine7081181818725424
  • BjørkedalEFlatenMA2011Interaction between expectancies and drug effects: an experimental investigation of placebo analgesia with caffeine as an active placeboPsychopharmacology21522334
  • ButcherBECarmodyJJ2012Sex differences in analgesic response to ibuprophen are influenced by expectancy: a randomized, crossover, balanced placebo-designed studyEuropean Journal of Pain16710051322337582
  • CollocaLPineDSErnstMMillerFGGrillonC2016Vasopressin Bossts Placebo Analgesic Effects in Women: A Randomized TrialBiological Psychiatry791079480226321018
  • FaasseKGreyAJordanRGarlandSPetrieKJ2015Seeing is believing: Impact of social modeling on placebo and nocebo respondingHealth Psychology348880525545042
  • FlatenMAAslaksenPMFinsetASimonsenTJohansenO2006Cognitive and emotional factors in placebo analgesiaJournal of Psychosomatic Research61818916813849
  • HaltiaLTRinneJOHelinSParkkolaRNågrenKKaasinenV2008Effects of intravenous placebo with glucose expectation on human basal ganglia dopaminergic functionSynapse629682818566972
  • KelleyJMLemboAJAblonJSVillanuevaJJConboyLALevyRMarciCDKerrCEKirschIJacobsonEERiessHKaptchukTJ2009Patient and practitioner influences on the placebo effect in irritable bowel syndromePsychosomatic Medicine7177899719661195
  • KlosterhalfenSKellermannSBraunSKowalskiASchrauthMZipfelSEnckP2009Gender and the nocebo response following conditioning and expectancyJournal of Psychosomatic Research664323819302890
  • KrummenacherPKossowskyJSchwarzCBruggerPKelleyJMMeyerAGaabJ2014Expectancy-induced placebo analgesia in children and the role of magical thinkingJournal of Pain151212829325261340
  • LorberWMazzoniGKirschI2007Illness by suggestion: expectancy, modeling, and gender in the production of psychosomatic symptomsAnn Behav Med331112617291177
  • OkenBSFlegalKZajdelDKishiyamaSHaasMPetersD2008Expectancy effect: Impact of pill administration on cognitive performance in healthy seniorsJournal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology3071718165936
  • SwiderKBabelP2013The effect of the sex of a model on nocebo hyperalgesia induced by social observational learningPain15481312723725779
  • TheysohnNSchmidJIcenhourAMewesCForstingMGizewskiERSchedlowskiMEisenbruchSBensonS2014Are there sex differences in placebo analgesia during visceral pain processing? A fMRI study in healthy subjectsNeurogastroenterol Motil261217435325346054

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • FlatenMAal’AbsiMPlacebo and placebo effectGellmanMDRick TurnerJEncyclopedia of Behavioral MedicineNew YorkSpringer201314971499
  • BenedettiFThe opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesiaPain19966435355438783319
  • AslaksenPMLybyPSFear of pain potentiates nocebo hyperalgesiaJ Pain Res2015870371026491370
  • AslaksenPMBystadMVambheimSMFlatenMAGender differences in placebo analgesia: event-related potentials and emotional modulationPsychosom Med201173219319921217098
  • ButcherBECarmodyJJSex differences in analgesic response to ibuprofen are influenced by expectancy: a randomized, crossover, balanced placebo-designed studyEur J Pain20121671005101322337582
  • SwiderKBabelPThe effect of the sex of a model on nocebo hyperalgesia induced by social observational learningPain201315481312131723725779
  • RileyJL3rdRobinsonMEWiseEAMyersCDFillingimRBSex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysisPain1998742–31811879520232
  • GarciaEGodoy-IzquierdoDGodoyJFPerezMLopez-ChicheriIGender differences in pressure pain threshold in a repeated measures assessmentPsychol Health Med200712556757917828677
  • WoodrowKMFriedmanGDSiegelaubABCollenMFPain tolerance: differences according to age, sex and racePsychosom Med19723465485564644663
  • FillingimRBKingCDRibeiro-DasilvaMCRahim-WilliamsBRileyJL3rdSex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findingsJ Pain200910544748519411059
  • ForgaysDGRzewnickiROberAJForgaysDKHeadache in college students: a comparison of four populationsHeadache19933341821908496056
  • RollmanGBLautenbacherSSex differences in musculoskeletal painClin J Pain2001171202411289085
  • YunusMBA comprehensive medical evaluation of patients with fibromyalgia syndromeRheum Dis Clin North Am200228220121712122914
  • EippertFBingelUSchoellEDActivation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesiaNeuron200963453354319709634
  • BenedettiFAmanzioMCasadioCOliaroAMaggiGBlockade of nocebo hyperalgesia by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumidePain19977121351409211474
  • BenedettiFAmanzioMMechanisms of the placebo responsePulm Pharmacol Ther201326552052323370213
  • BenedettiFAmanzioMVighettiSAsteggianoGThe biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effectJ Neurosci20062646120141202217108175
  • PopescuALeRescheLTrueloveELDrangsholtMTGender differences in pain modulation by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls: a systematic reviewPain2010150230931820557999
  • ButlerRKFinnDPStress-induced analgesiaProg Neurobiol200988318420219393288
  • SternbergWFLiebeskindJCThe analgesic response to stress: genetic and gender considerationsEur J Anaesthesiol Suppl19951014177641637
  • MogilJSSternbergWFKestBMarekPLiebeskindJCSex differences in the antagonism of swim stress-induced analgesia: effects of gonadectomy and estrogen replacementPain199353117258316385
  • OkenBSFlegalKZajdelDKishiyamaSHaasMPetersDExpectancy effect: impact of pill administration on cognitive performance in healthy seniorsJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol200830171718165936
  • HaltiaLTRinneJOHelinSParkkolaRNågrenKKaasinenVEffects of intravenous placebo with glucose expectation on human basal ganglia dopaminergic functionSynapse200862968268818566972
  • KlosterhalfenSKellermannSBraunSGender and the nocebo response following conditioning and expectancyJ Psychosom Res200966432332819302890
  • AbramsKKushnerMGThe moderating effects of tension-reduction alcohol outcome expectancies on placebo responding in individuals with social phobiaAddict Behav20042961221122415236826
  • LybyPSAslaksenPMFlatenMAIs fear of pain related to placebo analgesia?J Psychosom Res201068436937720307704
  • McCubbinJAWilsonJFBruehlSRelaxation training and opioid inhibition of blood pressure response to stressJ Consult Clin Psychol19966435936018698954
  • KoeppMJHammersALawrenceADAsselinMCGrasbyPMBenchCJEvidence for endogenous opioid release in the amygdala during positive emotionNeuroimage200944125225618809501
  • CollocaLPineDSErnstMMillerFGGrillonCVasopressin boosts placebo analgesic effects in women: a randomized trialBiol Psychiatry2016791079480226321018
  • KessnerSSprengerCWrobelNWiechKBingelUEffect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia: a randomized studyJAMA2013310161733173524150470
  • GoffauxPMichaudKGaudreauJChalayePRainvillePMarchandSSex differences in perceived pain are affected by an anxious brainPain201115292065207321665365
  • KeoghEBirkbyJThe effect of anxiety sensitivity and gender on the experience of painCogn Emot1999136813829
  • LiccardiGSennaGRussoMEvaluation of the nocebo effect during oral challenge in patients with adverse drug reactionsJ Investig Allergol Clin Immunol2004142104107
  • StröhleAIncreased response to a putative panicogenic nocebo administration in female patients with panic disorderJ Psychiatr Res200034643944211165311
  • AmanzioMBenedettiFNeuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystemsJ Neurosci19991914844949870976
  • SiegelSDrug tolerance, drug addiction, and drug anticipationCurr Dir Psychol Sci2005146296300
  • FlatenMASimonsenTWaterlooKOlsenHPharmacological classical conditioning in humansHum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp1997124369377
  • WeimerKCollocaLEnckPPlacebo effects in psychiatry: mediators and moderatorsLancet Psychiatry20152324625725815249
  • BenedettiFLanotteMLopianoLCollocaLWhen words are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effectNeuroscience2007147226027117379417