77
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Uncovering the influence of social skills and psychosociological factors on pain sensitivity using structural equation modeling

, , , &
Pages 2223-2231 | Published online: 11 Sep 2017

Abstract

Pain is a subjective emotional experience that is influenced by psychosociological factors such as social skills, which are defined as problem-solving abilities in social interactions. This study aimed to reveal the relationships among pain, social skills, and other psychosociological factors by using structural equation modeling. A total of 101 healthy volunteers (41 men and 60 women; mean age: 36.6±12.7 years) participated in this study. To evoke participants’ sense of inner pain, we showed them images of painful scenes on a PC screen and asked them to evaluate the pain intensity by using the visual analog scale (VAS). We examined the correlation between social skills and VAS, constructed a hypothetical model based on results from previous studies and the current correlational analysis results, and verified the model’s fit using structural equation modeling. We found significant positive correlations between VAS and total social skills values, as well as between VAS and the “start of relationships” subscales. Structural equation modeling revealed that the values for “start of relationships” had a direct effect on VAS values (path coefficient =0.32, p<0.01). In addition, the “start of relationships” had both a direct and an indirect effect on psychological factors via social support. The results indicated that extroverted people are more sensitive to inner pain and tend to get more social support and maintain a better psychological condition.

Introduction

Pain is a subjective experience and is caused not only by physical damage but also emotionalCitation1 or cognitive aspects (e.g., attention, and expectation).Citation2,Citation3 Therefore, the sensitivity of pain varies among individuals even for the same intensity of stimulation; previous studies have suggested that individual evaluation and intervention are necessary in clinical practice. Meanwhile, pain can be induced simply by observing painful scenes in photographs.Citation4Citation6 Observations of painful scenes activated the brain area that was related to the pain matrix, and the observation was thought to be useful for creating experimental pain emotion without the influence of peripheral organ.Citation7,Citation8

The biopsychosocial model is proposed as the framework for understanding the concept of complex treatment for pain.Citation9 This model stresses the importance of the psychological and social aspects, which reflect emotions, feelings, and the role of the self in the social environment and society surrounding the person of object, in addition to a biological factor due to the physical damage.

The relationship between pain and social aspects has attracted attention in recent years. For example, Macdonald et alCitation10 reported that social pain is induced by exclusion or low evaluation in a close relationship (e.g., a friend, lover, or family member). In social pain, the frustration of basic desires, which includes the so-called position desire and good relationships with others, induces social pain as an alarm reaction.Citation11Citation13 Eisenberger has reported that social pain and physical pain have a common neural mechanism.Citation14 Furthermore, the sensitivity of the social pain and the sensitivity of the physical pain were shown to be positively correlated,Citation14,Citation15 and it is thought that the person who is sensitive to social pain is also sensitive to physical pain. In this way, it has been revealed that social pain has similar neural process as physical pain, and it is thought that the social environment surrounding an individual modulates the pain sensitivity.Citation16,Citation17 It is known that such social environments have a direct influence on physical pain, and the quality of the relationship with a close friend has been reported to modulate the physical pain sensitivity.Citation18 In addition, it has been revealed that physical pain is reduced by social support (e.g., gentle words, existence of a partner, clenching the hand of others).Citation19Citation22 In this way, it has been revealed that sensitivity of the pain is low in the person who builds good social relations and has good support from those around him/her. However, loneliness in cancer patients exacerbated psychological conditions such as depression and perception of pain;Citation23 depression and anxiety can also increase physical pain sensitivity.Citation24,Citation25 Thus, interactions with one’s social environment is critical for managing pain; social skills are among the personal characteristics required for adequate social interaction.

Social skills are defined as the ability to effectively interact with other people.Citation26 This skill comprises aspects of verbal and nonverbal communication and includes styles of social problem solving (e.g., rational, impulsive, and avoidant), confidence, assertiveness, goal direction, and self-monitoring.Citation27 In previous studies, it has been reported that depression and loneliness are high in people with low social skills.Citation28Citation30 In addition, people who have high social skills can readily receive social support, and they tend not to find themselves in extremely difficult states.Citation31 Additionally, in previous studies using structural equation modeling, people whose social skills are high have been found to have high social support as well as quality of life, and they did not fall into depression.Citation32 In this way, it has been revealed that social skills influence the formation and construction of personal psychosociology (social support, depression, loneliness).Citation28Citation31 In addition, it was found that social support reduces pain sensitivityCitation19Citation22 and that negative psychological condition increases pain sensitivity.Citation23Citation25 However, there are no studies that have examined relations between social skills and pain sensitivity and that have examined mutual relationships between psychosociological factors and pain sensitivity. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine 1) the relations between social skills and pain sensitivity and 2) the influence of social skills and psychosociological factors on pain sensitivity, using structural equation modeling.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

The experimental protocol was approved by the Kio University Ethics Committee (approval number: H26-31), and the study protocol conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided written consent to participate in the experiment after receiving an explanation of the procedures involved.

Participants

This study was carried out from October 2014 to June 2015. The participants were recruited from three organizations to ensure variations in age and type of occupation (medical institution, general company, university). Exclusion criteria were chronic or acute pain, current psychiatric diagnosis, or use of anxiolytics or antidepressants. The participants were all volunteers, and no entrance fee was paid. They were 101 healthy volunteers (41 men and 60 women; mean age: 36.6±12.7 years); 41 were in health care occupations, 38 were in other types of work, 21 were university students, and 1 was unemployed.

Sample size for this exploratory study was calculated using the computer program G*Power prior to data collection. We calculated the sample size necessary for a correlation analysis with the effect size used in a previous study (effect size = 0.3 power = 0.8).Citation33 Using these parameters, the recommended sample size was 82; we aimed to recruit a few more participants than this sample size. Since some of the previous studies used more than 100 samples in the structural equation modeling,Citation34Citation36 we decided the final sample size in reference to these works.

Procedure

In this study, we used an inner experience of pain as the pain stimulation. This decision was made because it is relatively easier to collect samples without the risk associated with violence or physical aggression. After administering an inner experience of pain to each subject, we performed the autocriticism task for each question.

Pain stimulation

The inner experience of pain was proposed by Ogino et al,Citation7 and the subjective feeling of pain is said to occur because of such an inner experience.

The task was performed in a block design, and a total of three sets were carried out. One set consisted of 10 trials, and 1 trial consisted of 1 second of fixation on an image of a cross, followed by a 2-second fixation on an image showing a painful event, and then a 2-second fixation on a blank white page (). The images showing painful events were those used in a previous study by the authors.Citation5,Citation7 Examples of images of painful events are shown in . This was the frequently used approach in the previous studies.Citation37,Citation38 We used eight kinds of pain images (e.g., “I cut a finger with a kitchen knife,” “A needle on the finger stings”) involving both the left and the right hands. The images were angled so that they allowed for the participant to feel as though the painful events were happening to him/her. To evoke the participants’ sense of inner pain, we showed them images of painful events on a PC screen and asked them to evaluate the intensity of pain using the VAS. We then calculated the mean VAS of the three sets of images.

Figure 1 Pain stimulation procedure.

Notes: The experiment consisted of three sets. The subjects evaluated the pain intensity at the end of each set. One set consisted of 10 trials, and 1 trial consists of 1 second of fixation on an image of a cross, followed by a 2-second fixation on an image showing a painful event, and then a 2-second fixation on a blank white page.
Figure 1 Pain stimulation procedure.

Figure 2 Sample painful images.

Notes: We used eight kinds of pain images involving both the left and the right hands. The images were angled so that they allowed for the participant to feel as though the painful events were happening to him/her.
Figure 2 Sample painful images.

Measures

Visual analog scale

A 10 cm VAS was provided for participants to rate their current level of pain, which was assessed after each set of inner experience tasks. The VAS is a psychometric scale along a continuous line that measures subjective pain levels. The scale ranges from “0” or “none,” indicating no pain, to ‘’10” or “agonizing,” indicating severe pain. The advantage of the VAS is its simplicity, which allows quick and intuitive answers from patients.Citation39,Citation40

Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults

Because social skills are affected by culture and nationality, we used the Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults designed to evaluate social skills in Japan.Citation41 This self-rating scale is a standard measure that can simultaneously assess concrete and communication skills in an interpersonal interaction.Citation41 It comprises six subscales: “start of relationships,” “decoding,” “claim,” “control of feelings,” “maintenance of relationships,” and “encoding.” The scale consists of 35 items, including four reversed items. An example of an item in the 8-item “start of relationships” subscale is “You’re friendly with anyone.” An example of an item in the 8-item “decoding” subscale is “You read a person’s feelings from one’s look.” An example of an item in the 7-item “claim” subscale is “When your friend hurts your feelings, you tell your feelings clearly.” An example of an item in the 4-item “control of feelings” subscale is “It’s difficult to control your feelings.” An example of an item in the “maintenance of relationships” subscale is “You behave depending on the situation.” An example of an item in the 4-item “encoding” subscale is “You express your feelings openly.” The items were rated on a scale from 1 to 4 (1= “very strongly disagree” to 4= “very strongly agree”), with higher scores indicating higher social skills. The Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults has demonstrated good reliability and validity.Citation41

Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

Perceived social support was assessed with the 12-item Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS), which measures global perceived support (e.g., “There is a special person around when I am in need”).Citation42 The items were rated on a scale from 1 to 7 (1= “very strongly disagree” to 7= “very strongly agree”). Internal consistencies of the subscales and total scale were excellent (Cronbach’s α =0.85–0.91), and the scales had demonstrated strong test–retest stability over a 2- to 3-month interval (r=0.72–0.85). The construct validity of the total MSPSS scale has been demonstrated through its significant negative correlation with depression.Citation42

Beck Depression Inventory

Depression was measured with the Japanese version of the second edition of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II).Citation43 The BDI-II is a validated self-administered questionnaire evaluating how a patient feels and experiences things.Citation44 It consists of 21 questions, each of which is scored on a scale ranging from 0 to 3. All scores are added to provide a total score, with a maximum score of 36. The total score gives an estimation of the severity of existing depression (0–13= no depression, 14–19= mild depression, 20–28= moderately severe depression, 29–63= severe depression). The Japanese version of the BDI-II was developed by Kojima et alCitation43 using a translation-backtranslation procedure to establish cross-language equivalence, and has been well validated with the general population as well as with a clinical sample.

The Japanese Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale

The revised University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale-Japanese version was employed to measure loneliness. Russell created the UCLA Loneliness Scale based on his understanding that loneliness occurs from conditional perspectives and that loneliness is unidimensional.Citation45,Citation46

This scale was translated and revised by Moroi,Citation47 tested with participants in their teenage years to those aged over 65 years, and then named the Japanese revised UCLA Loneliness Scale. An example of items is “You stand alone from other people,” “There is none of the people knowing you well.” The scale consists of 20 items including 10 reversed items. The items are rated on a scale from 1 to 4 (1= “absolutely disagree” to 4= “very strongly agree”).

Data analyses

Analyses were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and SEM was performed using SPSS Amos 19.0 (SPSS Inc.). Normality of distribution of data was determined by the Shapiro–Wilk test. Statistical analysis was conducted according to the following three-stage approach. First, Student’s t-tests were conducted to examine differences between men and women on each measure. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) test was conducted to compare between three types of occupations (health care occupations, other types of work, and university study) on measures of social skills, social support, loneliness, depression, or VAS. After the ANOVA test, results with significant variation were subjected to post hoc tests to compare the significant differences between the three types of occupations. The post hoc analyses were performed using the Bonferroni test. Second, the correlation between social skills (include the subscales) and VAS was examined using Pearson correlation analysis. As we paid attention to a relationship between social skills and VAS, we focused on those relationships and performed a correlation analysis. Finally, we constructed a hypothetical model based on the results from previous studies and the current correlation analysis results (), and then we used structural equation modeling to assess the model’s fit with the data. We assessed data model fitness with the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSA). The goodness-of-fit was evaluated by the following criteria: GFI >0.90, CFI >0.90, and RMSEA <0.05.Citation48 Results of comparisons with a p-value of 0.05 or lower were considered to represent statistically significant differences. The method of Holm was used to adjust the p-values in multiple testing.

Figure 3 Hypothesized structural relationships between pain intensity and psychosociological factors (hypothesis model).

Abbreviation: VAS, visual analog scale.
Figure 3 Hypothesized structural relationships between pain intensity and psychosociological factors (hypothesis model).

Results

Descriptive statistics

The participants were fairly evenly distributed across the age groups, except for the 55–65 age bracket: “15–24 years old” (21), “25–34 years old” (26), “35–44 years old” (23), “45–54 years old” (22), and “55–65 years old” (9). There were no statistically significant differences between men and women on the measures of “social skills” (p=0.23), social support (p=0.91), loneliness (p=0.06), depression (p=0.21), or VAS (p=0.36). The results of ANOVA revealed a statistically significant difference in loneliness according to occupation as assessed by Japanese Revised UCLA Loneliness score (F=3.775, p=0.02). The results of post hoc testing and loneliness scores in health care occupations were significantly higher than those found for the university students (p=0.03).

Correlation analysis

Statistically significant positive correlations were observed between VAS and total social skills values, as well as between VAS and “start of relationships” subscales (). The VAS was most strongly correlated with the “start of relationships” subscale (r=0.36, p<0.01).

Table 1 Correlations between social skills (including subscales) and pain intensity

Covariance structure analysis of the hypothesized model

The hypothesized model was constructed on the basis of a previous studyCitation25Citation29 and the result of the current correlational analysis involving total social skills values, VAS, social support, and psychological factors (depression, loneliness). The hypothesis model, with standardized path coefficients, is presented in . The statistical goodness-of-fit of the hypothesis model, denoted by the χ2 value, was 0.448 (df=2, p=0.799). The GFI was 0.998, the CFI was 1.000, and the RMSEA was 0.000. The social skills had a meaningful negative influence on the psychological factors (path coefficient =−0.58, p<0.001) and positive influence on social support (path coefficient =0.30, p<0.01), and these findings affirmed those of a previous study.Citation25Citation29 In addition, social skills had both a direct and an indirect effect on psychological factors via social support. However, the relation between social skills and VAS became apparent due to the direct effect of the social skills and not the indirect effects via psychological factors or via social support.

Figure 4 Hypothesized model (nonsignificant paths are indicated by dashed lines).

Notes: Model fit indices: χ2 = 0.448, df =2, p=0.799, goodness-of-fit index (GFI) =0.998, comparative fit index (CFI) =1.000, root mean squared error of the approximation (RMSEA) =0.000; **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
Abbreviations: e, error; VAS, visual analog scale.
Figure 4 Hypothesized model (nonsignificant paths are indicated by dashed lines).

Model adjusted by the covariance structure analysis

We performed a model correction based on the results of the hypothesized model. The modified model, with standardized path coefficients, is presented in . The statistical goodness-of-fit of the hypothesis model, denoted by the χ2 value, was 0.446 (df=2, p=0.800). The GFI was 0.998, the CFI was 1.000, and the RMSEA was 0.000.

Figure 5 Modified model (nonsignificant paths are indicated by dotted lines).

Notes: Model fit indices: χ2 =0.446, df =2, p=0.800, goodness-of-fit index (GFI) =0.998, comparative fit index (CFI) =1.000, root mean squared error of the approximation (RMSEA) =0.000; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
Abbreviations: e, error; VAS, visual analog scale.
Figure 5 Modified model (nonsignificant paths are indicated by dotted lines).

Since the influence of social skills on VAS did not become clear in the hypothesized model, we substituted “start of relationships” for social skills, as it had the highest correlation coefficient with VAS. The relationship between “start of relationships,” social support, and psychological factors turned out to be similar to that found in the hypothesized model, and the direct positive influence of “start of relationships” on VAS became clear (path coefficient =0.32, p<0.01).

Discussion

The correlational analysis in this study showed that pain intensity of inner experience was slightly related to high social skills. In addition, pain intensity was positively correlated with values of the subscales (“start of relationships”) as well as with the total social skills scores. The structural equation model results indicated that in comparison to their peers with low scores on “start of relationships” (included in social skills subscale), participants whose score on “start of relationships” was high tended to receive more social support, had healthier psychological factors in the modified model, and more readily felt inner pain by observing painful scenes.

Relationship between social skill and pain sensitivity

To identify factor(s) that could predict pain sensitivity, we investigated the relationship between social skills and pain sensitivity. Results showed that pain sensitivity was positively correlated with social skills. In the previous study, social skills were found to be related to the ability to be empathetic.Citation28 In addition, one’s ability to be empathetic influenced his/her inner experience of pain from observations of painful scenes.Citation49 Therefore, high social skills might be indirectly related to inner experience of pain due to high empathetic ability.

Result of covariance structure analysis (hypothesized model)

In addition to analyzing the relationship between social skills and pain sensitivity, we used structural equation modeling to reveal inter-relationships among the psychosociological factors such as psychological factors (loneliness and depression) and social support. In the hypothesized model, we designed a structural model that included each factor in addition to the relationships revealed by the correlational analysis and the results of a previous study which indicated that psychological factors (e.g., loneliness and depression) and social support were related to social skills.Citation28Citation32

The hypothesized model specified that social skills had a direct effect on social support and psychological factors. Additionally, the psychological factors were directly affected by social skills and indirectly via social support. Thus, social skills were related to psychosociological factors in the present study as well as in the previous study,Citation28Citation32 and it was revealed that individuals who have high social skills more readily received support from their neighbors and had more positive psychological condition. The previous study revealed a relationship between negative psychological condition and poor social support,Citation49Citation52 and our hypothesized model corroborated the previous findings. However, there were no significant links between social skills and pain sensitivity, so the first hypothesized model did not clarify the relationship between social skill and pain sensitivity. Social skills encompass a wide variety of concepts and are multidimensional; therefore, many variabilities exist among elements in certain individuals. The variabilities among elements may have interfered with some significant relationships between social skills and pain sensitivity in the first hypothesized model.

In the correlational analysis, pain sensitivity was significantly correlated with partial subscales of the Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults. Consequently, analyzing the correlations with the total value of social skill most likely attenuated the sensitivity of the scale to detect the relationship between social skills and pain sensitivity. Therefore, we modified the model, replacing social skills with the subscale “start of relationships” and refitted the structural equation model.

Result of covariance structure analysis (modified model)

We examined the modified model using the subscale “start of relationships,” which showed the highest correlation coefficient with pain sensitivity. The result revealed that the relationships among “start of relationships,” “social support,” and “psychological factors” were the same as in the hypothesized model. A direct effect from “start of relationships” to pain sensitivity was observed. In other words, the value of “start of relationships” had a positive effect on pain sensitivity of inner experience.

The “start of relationships” subscale is comprised of eight items, such as “You’re open with anyone,” “You’re friendly with anyone,” “You start to have a conversation with a person who you’ve never met before,” “I am good at talking with a person,” “You readily join a conversation,” “You readily greet anyone,” “It’s difficult to make a chance to start a conversation when you want to get to know a person,” “You introduce yourself well to a person who you’ve never met before.” The question picks out an extroverted personality trait, namely being sociable, whose association overflows into laughter and humor.Citation53 A previous study also suggested that there is a strong relationship between social skills and extroversion; it clarified that extroversion becomes more prominent with higher level of social skills.Citation54

In addition, previous studies revealed that high extroversion or high ability for empathy had significant relationships with low loneliness.Citation28,Citation55 In other words, high extroversion (i.e., high ability of start of relationships) was correlated with low levels of loneliness and high empathetic ability. In the modified model of this study, the “start of relationships” had negative effects on psychological factors and positive effects on pain sensitivity of inner experience. Therefore, “start of relationships,” one of the subscales of the Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults, might reflect extroversion, and high extroversion generally prevents fall into negative psychological conditions, and high empathetic ability appears to evoke high pain sensitivity from observations of painful scenes.

Conclusion and limitations

First, this study aimed to clarify the relationships between social skills and pain sensitivity. Second, we researched mutual intervals between social skills and pain sensitivity in addition to the psychosocial factors related to social skills. The result of correlation analysis showed significant positive correlations between VAS and total social skills values, as well as with the “start of relationships” subscales. Additionally, structural equation modeling revealed that “start of relationships” values had a direct effect on VAS values. The “start of relationships” had both a direct and an indirect effect via social support on the psychological factors. The social support had positive relationships with psychological factors but showed no significant links with VAS. Considering previous studies,Citation19Citation23 similar tendency in the pain intensity of inner experience had been expected; however, social skills had positive effects on pain sensitivity of inner experience. Previous studies suggested that empathetic ability is strongly related to inner pain experiences that were induced by observing painful scenes. Therefore, the inner experience of pain might include an element of “the empathy of pain.”

The ability to empathize with others’ experiences of negative feelings was thought to be important in constructing a good societal environment.Citation56Citation58 These factors help us understand the relationship between social skills and the inner experience of pain. This is to say that the person who is easy to empathize with for the inner experience of pain has high social skills and is less likely to fall into depression; the person’s interaction with the society is also likely to advance. We think that it is necessary to consider the influence of individual social skills for the clinical practice of pain in this study.

There are several limitations to this study. First, it showed no statistically significant differences between men and women in its measures. However, several studies have indicated that depression is more prevalent in women.Citation59,Citation60 It is thought that representativeness of the sample collected in this study is related to these discrepancies. The sample used healthy volunteers, restricting generalizability to general and patient samples. Second, the Social Skills Self-Rating Scale for Adults, which we used as an evaluation of social skills, is mainly used in Japan, and only one reference exists. Finally, we estimated sensitivity to pain through an experimental procedure and self-reporting of the measured variables. We considered that the expectations of the experimenters had no influence, but we cannot completely deny this.

In the future, we will repeat this examination by including a measurement of objective social skills; it is necessary to compare the results of this study with those of a study on pain sensitivity in response to physical pain stimulation. Also, the psychological measures (depression, loneliness) that we used in this study have to observe the tendency in the clinical sample.

Acknowledgments

The abstract of this paper was presented at the 16th World Conference on Pain (2016) as a poster presentation with interim findings. The poster’s abstract was published in “Poster Abstracts” in 16th World Congress on Pain website (https://event.crowdcompass.com/wcp2016).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • SchweinhardtPKalkNWartolowskaKInvestigation into the neural correlates of emotional augmentation of clinical painNeuroimage20084075976618221890
  • KongJGollubRLRosmanISBrain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imagingJ Neurosci20062638138816407533
  • ValetMSprengerTBoeckerHDistraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain–an fMRI analysisPain200410939940815157701
  • SingerTSeymourBO’dohertyJKaubeHDolanRJFrithCDEmpathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of painScience20043031157116214976305
  • JacksonPLMeltzoffANDecetyJHow do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathyNeuroimage20052477177915652312
  • LammCDecetyJSingerTMeta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage2011542492250220946964
  • OginoYNemotoHInuiKSaitoSKakigiRGotoFInner experience of pain: imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brainCereb Cortex2007171139114616855007
  • MaeokaHMatsuoAHiyamizuMMoriokaSAndoHInfluence of transcranial direct current stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on pain related emotions: a study using electroencephalographic power spectrum analysisNeurosci Lett20125121121622326385
  • WaddellGBurtonAKConcepts of rehabilitation for the management of low back painBest Pract Res Clin Rheumatol200519465567015949782
  • MacdonaldGLearyMRWhy does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical painPsychol Bull200513120222315740417
  • JacksonJCSantoroMJElyTMImproving patient care through the prism of psychology: application of Maslow’s hierarchy to sedation, delirium and early mobility in the ICUJ Crit Care201429343844424636724
  • EisenbergerNILiebermanMDWilliamsKDDoes rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusionScience2003302564329029214551436
  • HartgerinkCHvan BeestIWichertsJMWilliamsKDThe ordinal effects of ostracism: a meta-analysis of 120 Cyberball studiesPLoS One2015105e012700226023925
  • EisenbergerNIThe neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosom Med201274212613522286852
  • KrossEBermanMGMischelWSmithEEWagerTDSocial rejection shares somatosensory representations with physical painProc Natl Acad Sci USA2011108156270627521444827
  • GatchelRJComorbidity of chronic pain and mental health disorders: the biopsychosocial perspectiveAm Psychol200459879580515554853
  • GatchelRJPengYBPetersMLFuchsPNTurkDCThe biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: scientific advances and future directionsPsychol Bull2007133458162417592957
  • VigilJMRowellLNChouteauSSex differences in how social networks and relationship quality influence experimental pain sensitivityPLoS One2013811e7866324223836
  • EisenbergerNIMasterSLInagakiTKAttachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experienceProc Natl Acad Sci USA201110828117211172621709271
  • BrownJLSheffieldDLearyMRRobinsonMESocial support and experimental painPsychosom Med200365227628312651995
  • OnodaKOkamotoYNakashimaKNittonoHUraMYamawakiSDecreased ventral anterior cingulate cortex activity is associated with reduced social pain during emotional supportSoc Neurosci20094544345419562631
  • YoungerJAronAParkeSChatterjeeNMackeySViewing pictures of a romantic partner reduces experimental pain: involvement of neural reward systemsPLoS One2010510e1330920967200
  • JaremkaLMFagundesCPGlaserRBennettJMMalarkeyWBKiecolt-GlaserJKLoneliness predicts pain, depression, and fatigue: understanding the role of immune dysregulationPsychoneuroendocrinology20133881310131723273678
  • TamrinSBYokoyamaKJalaludinJThe association between risk factors and low back pain among commercial vehicle drivers in peninsular Malaysia: a preliminary resultInd Health200745226827817485871
  • ShuchangHMingweiHHongxiaoJEmotional and neurobehavioural status in chronic pain patientsPain Res Manage20111614143
  • SegrinCSpecifying the nature of social skill deficits associated with depressionHum Commun Res19921989123
  • LibermanRPAssessment of social skillsSchizophr Bull1982862837071537
  • BeadleJNBrownVKeadyBTranelDParadisoSTrait empathy as a predictor of individual differences in perceived lonelinessPsychol Rep2012110131522489373
  • WierzbickiMMcCabeMSocial skills and subsequent depressive mood symptomatology in childrenJ Clin Child Psychol198817203208
  • ColeDAMilsteadMBehavioral correlates of depression: antecedents or consequences?J Couns Psychol198936408416
  • CohenSSherrodDRClarkMSSocial skills and the stress-protective role of social supportJ Pers Soc Psychol19865059639733486973
  • MüllerRPeterCCiezaASocial skills: a resource for more social support, lower depression levels, higher quality of life, and participation in individuals with spinal cord injury?Arch Phys Med Rehabil201596344745525264110
  • CohenJStatistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences2nd edHillsdale (NJ)Lawrence Earlbaum Associates1988
  • PelentsovLJFielderALLawsTAEstermanAJDevelopment of the parental needs scale for rare diseases: a tool for measuring the supportive care needs of parents caring for a child with a rare diseaseJ Multidiscip Healthc201694253327672327
  • KassabSEAl-ShafeiAISalemAHOtoomSRelationships between the quality of blended learning experience, self-regulated learning, and academic achievement of medical students: a path analysisAdv Med Educ Pract20156273425610011
  • Lewis-BeckCAbouzaidSXieLBaserOKimEAnalysis of the relationship between psoriasis symptom severity and quality of life, work productivity, and activity impairment among patients with moderate-to-severe psoriasis using structural equation modelingPatient Prefer. Adherence2013719920523508007
  • JacksonPLBrunetEMeltzoffANDecetyJEmpathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel painNeuropsychologia20064475276116140345
  • BuruckGWendscheJMelzerMStrobelADörfelDAcute psychosocial stress and emotion regulation skills modulate empathic reactions to pain in othersFront Psychol2014305517
  • KatzJMelzackRMeasurement of painSurg Clin North Am199979223125210352653
  • HawkerGAMianSKendzerskaTFrenchMMeasures of adult pain: visual analog scale for pain (VAS pain), numeric rating scale for pain (NRS pain), McGill pain questionnaire (MPQ), short-form McGill pain questionnaire (SF-MPQ), chronic pain grade scale (CPGS), short form-36 bodily pain scale (SF-36 BPS), and measure of intermittent and constant osteoarthritis pain (ICOAP)Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken)20116311S240S25222588748
  • AikawaAFujitaMAn attempt to construct a social skills self-rating scale for adultsBull Tokyo Gakugei Univ (Series I, Science of Education)2005568793 Japanese
  • ZimetGDahlemNZimetSFarleyGThe multidimensional scale of perceived social supportJ Pers Assess1988523041
  • KojimaMFurukawaTATakahashiHKawaiMNagayaTTokudomeSCross-cultural validation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in JapanPsychiatry Res200211029129912127479
  • ArnauRCMeagherMWNorrisMPBramsonRPsychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II with primary care medical patientsHealth Psychol20012011211911315728
  • RussellDPeplauLAFergusonMLDeveloping a measure of lonelinessJ Pers Assess197842290294660402
  • RussellDPeplauLACutronaCEThe revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidenceJ Pers Soc Psychol1980394724807431205
  • MoroiHDimensions of the revised UCLA Loneliness ScaleJinbun Ronsyu1991422351 Japanese
  • BymeBMStructural Equation Modeling with AMOS-Basic Concepts, Applications, and ProgrammingMahwah (NJ)Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc2001
  • MoriguchiYDecetyJOhnishiTEmpathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymiaCereb Cortex20071792223223417150987
  • PollardCKennedyPA longitudinal analysis of emotional impact, coping strategies and post-traumatic psychological growth following spinal cord injury: a 10-year reviewBr J Health Psychol200712Pt 334736217640451
  • StroudMWTurnerJAJensenMPCardenasDDPartner responses to pain behaviors are associated with depression and activity interference among persons with chronic pain and spinal cord injuryJ Pain200672919916459274
  • ElfströmMRydénAKreuterMTaftCSullivanMRelations between coping strategies and health-related quality of life in patients with spinal cord lesionJ Rehabil Med200537191615788327
  • EysenckHJEysenckSBGManual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (junior and adult)LondonHodder and Stoughton1975
  • ArgyleMLuLHappiness and social skillsPers Individ Dif1990111212551261
  • KongXWeiDLiWNeuroticism and extraversion mediate the association between loneliness and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortexExp Brain Res2015233115716425234401
  • BatsonCDBatsonJGSlingsbyJKHarrellKLPeeknaHMToddRMEmpathic joy and the empathy-altruism hypothesisJ Pers Soc Psychol19916134134261941512
  • EisenbergNMillerPAThe relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviorsPsychol Bull19871011911193562705
  • SifneosPEThe prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patientsPsychother Psychosom19732222552624770536
  • AlbertPRWhy is depression more prevalent in women?J Psychiatry Neurosci201540421922126107348
  • KawakamiNEpidemiology of depressive disorders in Japan and the worldNihon Rinsho200765915781584 Japanese17876978