4,392
Views
19
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Teaching for clinical reasoning – helping students make the conceptual links

Pages e436-e442 | Published online: 20 Sep 2010

Abstract

Background: Dental educators complain that students struggle to apply what they have learnt theoretically in the clinical context. This paper is premised on the assumption that there is a relationship between conceptual thinking and clinical reasoning.

Aims: The paper provides a theoretical framework for understanding the relationship between conceptual learning and clinical reasoning.

Method: A review of current literature is used to explain the way in which conceptual understanding influences clinical reasoning and the transfer of theoretical understandings to the clinical context.

Results: The paper argues that the connections made between concepts are what is significant about conceptual understanding. From this point of departure the paper describes teaching strategies that facilitate the kinds of learning opportunities that students need in order to develop conceptual understanding and to be able to transfer knowledge from theoretical to clinical contexts. Along with a variety of teaching strategies, the value of concept maps is discussed. The paper provides a framework for understanding the difficulties that students have in developing conceptual networks appropriate for later clinical reasoning.

Conclusion: In explaining how students learn for clinical application, the paper provides a theoretical framework that can inform how dental educators facilitate the conceptual learning, and later clinical reasoning, of their students.

Introduction

A common complaint in dental education is that many students struggle to apply what they have learnt theoretically in the clinical context (Schmidt Citation1983; Masters Citation1989; Lowry Citation1992; Graham Citation1996; DePaola & Slavkin Citation2004; Kalkwarf et al. Citation2005). There is a growing body of literature that argues for a relationship between conceptual thinking and clinical reasoning in trying to understand this difficulty (Rikers et al. Citation2004; De Bruin et al. Citation2005; Woods et al. Citation2005; Novak et al. Citation2006; Woods et al. Citation2006; Auclair Citation2007; Woods et al. Citation2007). The literature signals as significant for later application the way in which concepts are initially developed (Woods et al. Citation2007), the way in which concepts are linked (De Bruin et al. Citation2005; Novak et al. Citation2006; Auclair Citation2007; Woods et al. Citation2007), and the way in which concepts are stored for retrieval (Novak et al. Citation2006; Auclair Citation2007; Woods et al. Citation2007). The argument presented in this article draws on the literature regarding clinical reasoning and conceptual learning to highlight the kinds of learning that dental students need to do in order to transfer knowledge from the theoretical to the clinical context more easily.

This article provides a theoretical framework for understanding how conceptual learning is achieved, and the way in which conceptual understanding influences clinical reasoning and the ability to transfer theoretical understandings to the clinical context. From this point of departure, this article describes teaching strategies that facilitate the kinds of learning that students need in order to develop conceptual understanding and to be able to transfer knowledge from the theoretical to the clinical context. The value of concept mapping as a strategy for assisting the development of conceptual understanding, and for facilitating later transfer, is highlighted. This article makes a specific contribution to dental education in that it extrapolates understandings about conceptual learning, concept mapping and clinical reasoning from the medical and higher education contexts and facilitates application of these concepts in dental education. However, insights from the article will have general applicability to other fields of health sciences education since this article highlights the role that conceptual understanding plays in the transfer of preclinical knowledge to the clinical application context.

Understanding conceptual development

A concept is an encapsulation of ideas that are attributable to a single class or grouping – what Simon et al. (Citation2004) describe as “a mental object” or “a cognitive entity”. Conceptual understanding implies knowledge of an idea, and how it relates to already acquired ideas (Suaalii & Bhattacharya Citation2007). It requires an understanding of the contexts within which the idea is applicable, as well as its limitations (Suaalii & Bhattacharya Citation2007). Conceptual development is hierarchical, with new concepts or concept meanings subsumed under broader, more inclusive concepts (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). Novak and Gowin (Citation1984) highlight the significance of “propositional relationship(s)” between concepts. Propositional relationships are the explicit connections that are generated between concepts (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). At the most simple, two concepts might be connected with a linking word to form a proposition. Thus, “sky is blue” (Novak & Gowin Citation1984) indicates a valid proposition between the concepts “sky” and “blue”.

Conceptual development and clinical reasoning

At a more complex level, propositions signal causal relationships between concepts (Novak & Gowin Citation1984) – for example, the causal relationships between biomedical concepts (i.e. the basic mechanisms and functioning of the human body) and clinical concepts (i.e. the signs, symptoms and associated findings of disease) (Woods et al. Citation2007). Rehder and Hastie (Citation2001) argue that the power of causal theories lies in their ability to provide connections between features. In the medical context, Woods et al. (Citation2007) suggest that causal connections between basic science concepts and categories of disease contribute to the construction of “illness scripts” (Schmidt et al. Citation1990). Illness scripts are the individual theoretical frameworks of disease that clinicians construct. They are the causal theories that provide the relationships between biomedical concepts and clinical concepts. Illness scripts contain “enabling conditions” (i.e., features associated with acquisition of the illness), a description of what is not functioning (i.e., the “fault”), the signs and symptoms that arise due to the fault (i.e., the “consequences”), and are built up through extensive contact with multiple patients with similar symptoms (Schmidt et al. Citation1990). Bransford et al. (Citation1999) argue that when students are provided with the conceptual building blocks for such frameworks and when they are encouraged to explicitly construct and articulate connections between these concepts, knowledge transfer is enhanced.

Auclair (Citation2007) provides an explanation of why this is so. Most problems, including those of clinical diagnosis, are ill-structured (Auclair Citation2007). Information required to solve a problem of clinical diagnosis needs to be organized into patterns so as to produce sense (Auclair Citation2007). Auclair (Citation2007) suggests that this ordering (in other words, the problem formulation) is simply the structuring of concepts linked by understandings of their relationships. In this way, a ‘model’ of a case is generated. Case generation involves the establishment of causal propositions between incoming clinical information and existing clinical and biomedical concepts. Using endocarditis as a case example, Auclair (Citation2007) explains how expert clinicians link conceptual knowledge about bacteraemia, valvular disease and embolic phenomena in causal relationships that have diagnostic utility value. The way in which the clinical problem is formulated – in other words, the way in which information regarding the patient problem is organized and linked to both existing and incoming information – allows the clinician to ‘recognise’ the case as belonging to a ‘theory’ of (in this case) endocarditis. Auclair (Citation2007) argues that it is the similarity between the concepts and their relationships in both the case and the theory that allows clinicians to recognize that they are indeed similar. Choosing which are the most pertinent concepts to pay attention to may thus be the most important aspect in effective diagnosis.

The development of appropriate (and correct) concepts and the establishment of appropriate causal relationships between them are central to effective clinical reasoning. It is these concepts, and their subsequent conceptual networking in the context of extensive and repeated exposure to multiple patients with similar symptoms, that results in competent diagnosis (Rikers et al. Citation2004; Woods et al. Citation2005; Novak et al. Citation2006; Woods et al. Citation2006; Auclair Citation2007; Woods et al. Citation2007). Teaching and curriculum planning in dental education can make a contribution to all of these three aspects – the development of basic concepts, opportunities to network concepts, and extensive and repeated exposure to multiple patients with similar symptoms.

Implications for teaching and learning

Learning is the inborn mental ability and tendency in humans to create records of experience, to sort and compare records, to identify patterns, and thus to establish concepts (Von Glasersfeld Citation1995). In order to reason, plan and make good decisions, an individual must be able to generalize what has been learnt in the past to new learning, and be able to apply and extend this learning to a range of situations (Haskell Citation2001). Transfer – the application of learning acquired within a certain task, situation, or context to another, different task, situation, or context (Murphy & Tyler Citation2005) – is at the heart of being able to generalize from one situation to another. Transfer is achieved through the process of “mindful abstraction” (Salomon & Perkins Citation1989) – extracting what is “central, essential or generic” (MacLellan Citation2005) from a context and creating a mental representation of these attributes (MacLellan Citation2005). These “units of representation” (Carey Citation1992) encapsulate concepts. Conceptual thinking involves creating and using these “units of representation” (Carey Citation1992). In the context of clinical reasoning, these “units of representation” (Carey Citation1992), from the apparently unrelated sources of basic sciences, laboratory contexts, and previous patient encounters, are recalled when a new patient is encountered – for example, when a dentist recognizes the potential relationship between a reported sore throat and HIV infection ().

The process of “mindful abstraction” (Salomon & Perkins Citation1989) is active, rather than automatic (Salomon & Perkins Citation1989). It is achieved through the conscious decisions that people make to use particular strategies to solve new problems (Salomon & Perkins Citation1989), either because the strategies appear to present themselves on relevant occasions (MacLellan Citation2005) or because the individual deliberately searches for and retrieves previously learned strategies for potential application in new situations. It is only the learner who can achieve this abstraction and application (MacLellan Citation2005).

However, generating, recalling and using “units of representation” (Carey Citation1992) requires deliberate practice. While experts recognize the constellation of signs and symptoms and almost automatically infer associated encapsulated concepts and so form a diagnosis (Rikers et al. Citation2004), students need opportunities first to develop the concepts and conceptual frameworks (see, e.g. ) and then opportunities to apply these conceptual frameworks in the clinical context (). Opportunities need to be created for extensive and repeated exposure to multiple patients with similar symptoms (Rikers et al. Citation2004; Woods et al. Citation2005; Novak et al. Citation2006; Woods et al. Citation2006) – as for example, in , students need exposure to patients with sore throats with a variety of associated causes (e.g., HIV infection, malignancies, immune compromised elderly) in order for them to understand the diagnostic contexts within which their particular conceptual frameworks are applicable, and where these frameworks have potential limitations (Suaalii & Bhattacharya Citation2007). Practice is understood to be deliberate because it involves the deliberate, conscious effort of a learner in concept development, in concept application (i.e. in clinical diagnosis), and in monitoring one's own understanding of the actual process of applying prior knowledge to the ‘new’ clinical context (Bereiter & Scardamalia Citation1989). In the clinical context, the teacher facilitates this development by making overt the clinical reasoning process and by helping novice clinicians to understand their thinking processes in action (Mandin et al. Citation1997; Eva Citation2004; Kuiper & Pesuit Citation2004; Bowen Citation2006; Novak et al. Citation2006).

Figure 1. A student's concept map of the relationship between leukoplakia and HIV infection.

Figure 1. A student's concept map of the relationship between leukoplakia and HIV infection.

Figure 2. Concept map of specific diagnostic process.

Figure 2. Concept map of specific diagnostic process.

Because of the interplay between theoretical learning and clinical application in the development and networking of concepts, the teacher's role is fulfilled both in the classroom and in the clinical context (Ramani Citation2003). It is in the classroom where basic concepts – whether bio-psycho-social or medical – are developed and where initial connections between them are forged. Conceptual development is facilitated when learners are actively involved in their own learning process – through opportunities to generate hypotheses, interpret data, and challenge and affirm constructs which are viewed, within particular disciplines, as appropriate ways of interpreting the world (Perkins & Salomon Citation1994; MacLellan Citation2005; Billing Citation2007; Suaalii & Bhattacharya Citation2007). Even given an appropriate learning environment, there are, however, students who still struggle to extract the essence from the detail and tend to miss the core concepts and their essential relationships in the welter of background information to which they are exposed – whether this is through formal input from teachers or when engaged in problem-based learning or evidenced-based medical literature searches (Reilly Citation2007).

Teachers can help these students learn by making “an explicit effort to help students understand knowledge structure and production” (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). One way in which this can be achieved is through the strategy of “concept mapping” (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). Concept maps are intended to represent meaningful relationships between the concepts in the form of propositions (Novak & Gowin Citation1984; All & Havens Citation1997; Gaines Citation1999; All et al. Citation2003; Clayton Citation2006). Both the concepts and the “linking words” signifying the propositional relationships between concepts are recorded on the concept map (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). This strategy allows students to work out what is core and what is not, what is centrally significant to new learning, and how new learning relates to existing knowledge (All & Havens Citation1997; Gaines Citation1999; All et al. Citation2003; Clayton Citation2006). illustrates a concept map of the relationship between leukoplakia and HIV infection that might be generated by a student. In this example, the boxes represent the concepts and the ellipses the connections that the student perceives between the concepts.

As is evident from the example, concept maps differ from traditional mind-mapping in the explicitness with which the connections between concepts are recorded (Mahler et al. Citation1991; Jonassen et al. Citation1993; Plotnick Citation1997; Novak Citation1998; Chularut & DeBacker Citation2004; Clayton Citation2006; Hay et al. Citation2008; Novak & Canas Citation2008). Indeed, Novak and Gowin (Citation1984) argue that the power of concept mapping as a learning and teaching tool lies in this recording since it allows for the development of an understanding of the relationships among different bits of information (All & Havens Citation1997; Baugh & Mellott Citation1998; Mueller et al. Citation2002; All et al. Citation2003), and provides opportunities to build on previous knowledge and skills (Baugh & Mellott Citation1998; Dobbin Citation2001). A study of the example illustrated in demonstrates this claim. This student was required not only to identify core concepts related to both leukoplakia and HIV infection, but also to ‘explain’, through the relationship ellipses, their connections. It is in making these connections that students actually come to understand the complex relationships between the various things that they have already learnt, as well as the relationship between this knowledge and new information. Without the connections, it is easy for a student to make a list of terms without really understanding their significance or their inter-relatedness. Only in making the explicit linkages is the student forced to think about what information is needed, how it needs to be organized and sequenced, and how it fits together.

Concept maps act, thus, as important diagnostic tools for teachers. Novak and Gowin (Citation1984) highlight that concept maps are powerful tools for observing the nuances of meaning a student holds for the concept embedded in his or her map. When concept maps are diligently constructed, they are powerful revealers of students’ cognitive organization (Novak & Gowin Citation1984). Faulty conceptions and linkages are easily identified and can then be addressed by the teacher. To illustrate this point, imagine that a student submitted without any of the connecting ellipses. The experienced clinician-teacher would furnish those connections unconsciously and assume that the student understood the relevant concepts and their relationships. However, what if the concept map was submitted and all the ellipses had been replaced with “is”/”are” – a connecting term that was deemed perfectly appropriate for the proposition “the sky is blue” discussed earlier in this article. While many of these propositions may read as ‘true’, their relevance to the larger clinical problem may be irrelevant, or even incorrect. Clearly, such a student does not understand the significance of the concepts with specific regard to leukoplakia and HIV infection.

The strategy of concept mapping can valuably be applied at multiple points in the student's learning (Salvucci & Andersen 2001; Torree et al. Citation2007; Svinivasan et al. 2008). The teacher might generate the concept map, illustrating the nature of the relationships, when new concepts and embedded knowledge are being introduced. Concept mapping can be used for tutorial activities where students are explicitly asked to track the connections between new concepts and embedded knowledge, and for clinical ‘paper cases’ where students establish causal propositions between incoming clinical information and existing clinical and biomedical concepts. Students can use the strategy both during initial learning and when revising. Given the diagnostic characteristic of concept maps, they also make excellent assessment activities (Norman Citation2006). Students might be asked to illustrate relationships between biomedical concepts and clinical concepts.

Finally, concept maps have potential application in the clinical context. shows a concept map that might be generated by a clinical teacher to illustrate a point of diagnosis to a student in the clinical context. The concept map deals with a particular patient, and contextualizes diagnostic steps and strategies.

Students seldom get a chance in the clinical context of seeing and hearing the thinking steps that experienced clinicians go through when engaged in the diagnostic process. Illustrating this process for students through the use of a concept map makes overt the clinical reasoning process and helps novice clinicians understand their thinking processes in action (Mandin et al. Citation1997; Parsell & Bligh Citation2001; Ramani Citation2003; Eva Citation2004; Irby & Bowen Citation2004; Kuiper & Pesuit Citation2004; Novak et al. Citation2006; Bowen Citation2006; Norman Citation2006; Reilly Citation2007). In this way, clinical teachers help students establish connections between previous learning and a particular clinical presentation.

Three strategies for helping students establish such connections are suggested in the literature. First, clinical teachers can demonstrate the clinical reasoning process, articulating their own problem representations to model the type of abstract summary they expect from students (Irby & Bowen Citation2004; Bowen Citation2006; Reilly Citation2007). This strategy provides an opportunity for the teacher to reason aloud, linking the abstractions to existing illness scripts (Bowen Citation2006). Through reasoning aloud activities, clinical teachers highlight the way in which clinicians seek appropriate diagnostic possibilities in the patient history and physical examination (Bowen Citation2006). By exposing the ambiguity and ambivalence inherent in clinical reasoning (Reilly Citation2007) and admitting their own lack of knowledge (Ramani Citation2003), clinical teachers set the tone for students to admit their limitations and to ask questions.

Second, clinical teachers can elicit clinical reasoning from learners (Parsell & Bligh Citation2001; Ramani Citation2003). The clinical teacher might help students to identify what information is required for care of the patient (Irby & Bowen Citation2004). Learners can be prompted to generate a single sentence summary of the case with the use of appropriate medical terms (Bowen Citation2006; Norman Citation2006), and to prioritize a diagnosis through comparing and contrasting potential diagnoses with the actual clinical data (Bowen Citation2006). In providing opportunities for learners to think about the relationship between clinical findings, biomedical knowledge and relevant diagnoses (Bowen Citation2006), these activities promote the development of illness scripts.

Finally, clinical teachers can listen – without interrupting – while students reason spontaneously (Ramani Citation2003; Irby & Bowen Citation2004). This activity allows the teacher to understand how the student has organized the case in his or her own mind, to identify the learner's understanding of the case, and to distinguish gaps in knowledge or errors in reasoning that can become the focus of teaching (Irby & Bowen Citation2004; Reilly Citation2007). Any of these strategies might be facilitated through the drawing of concept maps, since the map provides a reference point for discussion. The teacher, whether in the theoretical or clinical context, thus has a significant role to play in providing an appropriate learning environment.

Conclusion

Drawing on the assumption that a relationship exists between conceptual thinking and clinical reasoning, this article has outlined a theoretical framework for understanding how conceptual learning is achieved and the way in which conceptual understanding influences clinical reasoning and the ability to transfer theoretical understandings to the clinical context. This theoretical framework signaled ways to assist students to develop the conceptual understanding pre-requisite for application in the clinical context. Effective clinical reasoning depends on the amount of knowledge and its coherence, the linkages networking the knowledge, and frequent opportunities to use the knowledge so as to refine it (Grant & Marsden Citation1987). The argument presented in this article suggests that an emphasis on conceptual learning has the potential to facilitate the development of clinical reasoning. Helping students to map their understandings in the basic sciences and pre-clinical context contributes to the development of conceptual frameworks that can be transferred to the clinical context. Assisting students to retrieve these conceptual frameworks and apply them appropriately is the challenge of clinical teaching. The emphasis in the clinical context is on making overt the clinical reasoning process and helping novice clinicians understand their thinking processes in action. Concept mapping has the potential to assist the development of clinical reasoning as the mapping of relationships between concepts facilitates transfer of insights between basic science and clinical contexts.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Professor Vincent Phillips for his assistance with the content of the concept maps shown in and .

Declaration of interest: The author reports no conflicts of interest. The author alone is responsible for the content and writing of the article.

References

  • All AC, Havens RL. Cognitive/concept mapping: A teaching strategy for nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing 1997; 25: 1210–1219
  • All AC, Huycke LI, Fisher MJ. Instructional tools for nursing education: Concept maps. Nurs Educ Perspect 2003; 24: 311–317
  • Auclair F. Problem formulation by medical students: An observation study. BMC Med Educ 2007; 7: 16
  • Baugh NG, Mellott KG. Clinical concept mapping as preparation for student nurses’ clinical experiences. J Nurs Educ 1998; 37: 253–256
  • Bereiter C, Scardamalia M. Intentional learning as a goal on instruction. Knowing, learning and instruction, L Resnick. Lawrence Erlbaum, New Jersey 1989
  • Billing D. Teaching for transfer of core/key skills in higher education: Cognitive skills. High Educ 2007; 53: 483–516
  • Bowen JL. Educational strategies to promote clinical diagnostic reasoning. New Engl J Med 2006; 355: 2217–2225
  • Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking R. How people learn: Brain, mind, experience and school. National Academy Press, Washington, DC 1999
  • Carey S. The origin and evolution of everyday concepts. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science: Cognitive models of science, RN Giere. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis 1992
  • Chularut P, Debacker TK. The influence of concept mapping on achievement, self-regulation, and self-efficacy in students of English as a second language. Contemp Educ Psychol 2004; 29: 248–263
  • Clayton L. Concept mapping: An effective, active teaching-learning method. Nurs Educ Perspect 2006; 27: 197–203
  • De Bruin ABH, Schmidt HG, Rikers RMJP. The role of basic science knowledge and clinical knowledge in diagnostic reasoning: A structural equation modeling approach. Acad Med 2005; 80: 765–773
  • De Paola DP, Slavkin HC. Reforming dental health professions education: A white paper. J Dent Educ 2004; 68: 1139–1150
  • Dobbin KR. Applying learning theories to develop teaching strategies for the critical care nurse. Crit Nurs Clin North Am 2001; 13: 1–11
  • Eva KW. What every teacher needs to know about clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2004; 39: 98–106
  • Gaines C. Clearing up the “concept fog”. ABNF J 1999; 10: 52–53
  • Graham CL. Conceptual learning processes in physical therapy students. Phys Ther 1996; 76: 856–866
  • Grant J, Marsden P. The structure of memorized knowledge in students and clinicians: An explanation for diagnostic expertise. Med Educ 1987; 21: 92–98
  • Haskell R. Transfer of learning. Academic Press, London 2001
  • Hay D, Kinchin I, Lygo-Baker S. Making learning visible: The role of concept mapping in higher education. Stud High Educ 2008; 33: 295–311
  • Irby DM, Bowen JL. Time-efficient strategies for learning and performance. Clin Teach 2004; 1: 23–28
  • Jonassen D, Beissner K, Yacci M. Structural knowledge: Techniques for representing, conveying and acquiring structural knowledge. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ 1993
  • Kalkwarf KL, Haden NK, Valachovic RW. ADEA commission on change and innovation in dental education. J Dent Educ 2005; 69: 1085–1087
  • Kuiper RA, Pesuit DJ. Promoting cognitive and metacognitive reflective reasoning skills in nursing practice: Self-regulated learning theory. Issues Innovations Nurs Educ 2004; 45: 381–391
  • Lowry S. What's wrong with medical education in Britain?. BMJ 1992; 305: 1277–1280
  • MacLellan E. Conceptual learning: The priority for higher education. Br J Educ Stud 2005; 53: 129–147
  • Mahler S, Hoz R, Fischl D, Tov-Ly E, Lernau OZ. Didactic use of concept mapping in higher education: Application in medical education. Instruct Sci 1991; 20: 25–47
  • Mandin H, Jones A, Woloschuk W, Harasym P. Helping students learn to think like experts when solving clinical problems. Acad Med 1997; 72: 173–179
  • Masters G. Improving the assessment of clinical reasoning. Learning in medical school: A model for the clinical professions, JI Balla, M Gibson, AM Chang. Hong Kong University Press, Hong Kong 1989
  • Mueller A, Johnston M, Bligh D. Joining mind mapping and care planning to enhance student critical thinking and achieve holistic nursing care. Nurs Diagn 2002; 13: 24–27
  • Murphy SM, Tyler S. The relationship between learning approaches to part-time study of management courses and transfer of learning to the workplace. Educ Psychol 2005; 25: 455–469
  • Norman G. Building on experience – The development of clinical reasoning. New Engl J Med 2006; 355: 2251–2252
  • Novak JD. Learning, creating and using knowledge: Concept maps as facilitative tools in schools and corporations. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ 1998
  • Novak JD, Canas AJ. The theory underlying concept maps and how to construct and use them. 2008, Available from: http://cmap.ihmc.us/Publications/ResearchPapers/TheoryUnderlyingConceptMaps/pdf (accessed 17 October 2009)
  • Novak JD, Gowin DB. Learning how to learn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge 1984
  • Novak K, Mandin H, Wilcox E, Mclaughlin K. Using a conceptual framework during learning attenuates the loss of expert-type knowledge structure. BMC Med Educ 2006; 6: 37–44
  • Parsell G, Bligh J. Recent perspectives on clinical teaching. Med Educ 2001; 35: 409–414
  • Perkins DN, Salomon G. Transfer of learning. The international encyclopedia of education, T Husen, T Postlethwaite. Elsevier/Pergamon, Oxford 1994
  • Plotnick E, 1997. Concept mapping: A graphical system for understanding the relationship between concepts. ERIC digest (ERIC Clearinghouse on Information and Technology, Syracuse, NY. Eric Identifier, ED407938)
  • Ramani S. Twelve tips to improve bedside teaching. Med Educ 2003; 25: 112–115
  • Rehder B, Hastie R. Causal knowledge and categories: The effects of causal beliefs on categorization, induction, and similarity. J Exp Psychol Gen 2001; 130: 323–360
  • Reilly BM. Inconvenient truths about effective clinical teaching. The Lancet 2007; 370: 705–711
  • Rikers RMJP, Lovens SMM, Schmidt HG. The role of encapsulated knowledge in clinical case representations of medical students and family doctors. Med Educ 2004; 38: 1035–1043
  • Salomon G, Perkins D. Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educ Psychol 1989; 24: 113–142
  • Salvucci DD, Anderson JR. Integrating analogical mapping and general problem solving: The path-mapping theory. Cog Sci 2001; 25: 67–110
  • Schmidt HG. Problem-based learning: Rationale and description. Med Educ 1983; 17: 11–16
  • Schmidt HG, Norman GR, Boshuizen HPA. A cognitive perspective on medical expertise: Theory and implications. Acad Med 1990; 65: 611–621
  • Simon MA, Tzur R, Heinz K, Kinzel M. Explicating a mechanism for conceptual learning: Elaborating the construct of reflective abstraction. J Res Math Educ 2004; 35: 305–329
  • Srinivasan M, Mcelvany M, Shay JM, Shavelson RJ, West DC. Measuring knowledge structure: Reliability of concept mapping assessment in medical education. Acad Med 2008; 83: 1196–1203
  • Suaalii F, Bhattacharya M. Conceptual model of learning to improve understanding of high school chemistry. J Interact Learn Res 2007; 18: 101–110
  • Torree DM, Daley B, Stark-Schweitzer T, Siddattha S, Petkova J, Ziebert M. A qualitative evaluation of medical student learning with concept maps. Med Teach 2007; 29: 949–955
  • Von Glasersfeld E. Radical constructivism: A way of knowing and learning. Falmer, Washington, DC 1995
  • Woods NN, Brooks LR, Norman GR. The value of basic science in clinical diagnosis: Creating coherence among signs and symptoms. Med Educ 2005; 39: 107–112
  • Woods NN, Brooks LR, Norman GR. It all makes sense: Biomedical knowledge, causal connections and memory in the novice diagnostician. Adv Health Sci Educ 2007; 12: 405–415
  • Woods NN, Howey EHA, Brooks LR, Norman GR. Speed kills? Speed, accuracy, encapsulations and causal understanding. Med Educ 2006; 40: 973–979

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.