1,846
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Reports

Inter-individual differences in sleep response to shift work in novice police officers – A prospective study

, , &

ABSTRACT

The aim of this longitudinal study on novice police officers was to investigate inter-individual differences in sleep response to shift work, and to identify potential baseline predictors thereof. A total of 42 subjects were assessed at baseline, prior to commencing shift work. They were re-assessed during three follow-up sessions within the first 2 years of shift work exposure after approximately 4, 12, and 20 months of rotating shift work. Wrist actigraphy and sleep logs were used to investigate nocturnal sleep at baseline and daytime sleep after night shifts during the follow-up sessions. Actigraphically estimated total sleep time and subjective sleep quality were analyzed as outcome variables, using mixed-effects analysis of variance. Systematic inter-individual differences were observed in the overall response of these outcome variables to shift work. In this sample, flexibility of sleeping habits and gender were found to be predictors of daytime total sleep time in the first 2 years of shift work exposure. Flexibility of sleeping habits and subjective quality of nighttime sleep prior to shift work were found to be predictors of subjective quality of daytime sleep. These results suggest that it may be possible to detect and even predict sleep deficiencies in response to shift work early on, which could be a basis for the development of individualized interventions to improve shift work tolerance.

Introduction

Sleep–wake disturbances are among the most pertinent and challenging problems of shift work (Åkerstedt, Citation2003; Åkerstedt & Wright, Citation2009; Vila, Citation2006). Both the duration and the structure of sleep are affected by shifted timing of sleep periods (Åkerstedt et al., Citation1991; Kerkhof & Lancel, Citation1991). In the short term, shift workers suffer from insomnia and/or excessive sleepiness, performance decrements, and mood swings (Boudreau et al., Citation2013; Drake et al., Citation2004; Rajaratnam et al., Citation2013). In the long term, workers may develop shift work disorder and a variety of other physical and mental health-related problems (Roth, Citation2012; Wright et al., Citation2013). Higher incidence rates of metabolic syndrome, cardiovascular disease, mood disorders, and cancer among shift workers have been well documented (Puttonen et al., Citation2010; Vogel et al., Citation2012; Wang et al., Citation2011). Sleep problems resulting from circadian misalignment and associated deficits in performance and health are key determinants of long-term shift work intolerance (Costa, Citation2003; Morris et al., Citation2012; Puttonen et al., Citation2010; Rajaratnam et al., Citation2013; Wang et al., Citation2011). Even in retirement, shift workers report more sleep problems than day workers (Monk et al., Citation2013).

There is substantial inter-individual variability in the development and severity of shift work intolerance (Härmä, Citation1993; Saksvik et al., Citation2011). It would be beneficial in terms of worker productivity, health and well-being if it were possible to recognize symptoms of shift work intolerance early on in those who are most vulnerable. Several studies have been conducted to identify factors predicting shift work (in)tolerance; for a recent review see Saksvik et al. (Citation2011). Age and gender have been suggested as candidate predictors of inter-individual differences in shift work tolerance (Hakola et al., Citation1996; Härmä & Kandolin, Citation2001), although conclusive evidence has yet to be presented. A variety of other potential predictors, such as demographics, personality, and social environments, have also been investigated (Saksvik et al., Citation2011; Storemark et al., Citation2013). Results have been mixed and inconclusive.

Given the central role of sleep deficiencies in shift work, it has been proposed that person-specific characteristics of circadian rhythms and sleep/wake regulation may be involved in inter-individual differences in shift work tolerance. Vulnerability to sleep loss, circadian phase position (chronotype), and flexibility of sleep timing (natural ability to sleep and work at unusual times of day) have been suggested as predictors (Iskra-Golec, Citation1993; Kerkhof, Citation1985; Lammers-van der Holst et al., Citation2006; Van Dongen, Citation2006; Van Dongen & Belenky, Citation2009).

A complication in attempts to identify predictors of shift work tolerance is that it is often impossible to distinguish cause from consequence. Most studies are retrospective, cross-sectional, and/or based on a selection of experienced shift workers, which causes a confounding effect known as the “healthy worker” selection effect (Knutsson & Åkerstedt, Citation1992). To avoid this confounding effect, longitudinal field studies with baseline measures taken prior to commencing shift work are needed.

This investigation involved a longitudinal study of novice police officers. Baseline measures were taken before they commenced shift work, and three follow-up measures were taken within the first 2 years of shift work exposure. We examined inter-individual differences in sleep responses at this early stage of shift work, and sought to identify unbiased baseline predictors.

Methods

Subjects

In this longitudinal study, 42 novice police officers (30 males, 12 females; ages 20–46 years, mean age 27 years) from two cohorts (classes) of the Dutch Police Force Academy participated. Initially, 46 police officers volunteered for the study, but four of them did not provide any data, leaving a sample size of N = 42 for analyses.

At the start of the study, the subjects were near the end of their 4-year education and training period. They were fit for duty by the criteria of the Dutch Police Force, and reported to be healthy with no psychiatric records. They denied prior shift work experience, and their exposure to shift work during the training period was sporadic and not sustained. All subjects gave written informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Amsterdam, and the experimental protocol was conform to international ethical standards (Portaluppi et al., Citation2010).

Experimental design

Subjects were tracked over a period of two years, during which they were tested four times. In the initial assessment (T0), baseline measurements were taken while subjects were still at the police academy, working daytime schedules. During three follow-up assessments (T1–T3), one cohort (N = 18) worked full time in rotating shift work throughout, and the other cohort (N = 24) worked full time in rotating shift work with an 8-week period of daytime duty between T1 and T2. The first, second and third follow-up assessments took place at 16 ± 4 weeks, 54 ± 5 weeks, and 82 ± 6 weeks (mean ± SD) after commencing shift work, respectively.

The nominal shift work schedule involved three 8-hour shifts, i.e., morning shifts (07:00–15:00), evening shifts (15:00–23:00), and night shifts (23:00–07:00), rotating in forward direction. Extended shifts (≥9 h) occurred on 41% of all recorded workdays. Schedules were also characterized by a high degree of ad hoc scheduling, characteristic of the field work of police officers.

The study was conducted in the years 2004–2007. Preliminary results of T1 and T2 measurements in a subgroup of 26 subjects were published previously (Lammers-van der Holst et al., Citation2006). Here we included the full sample of the study, added the T3 assessment, updated the analytical approach, and revisited the identification of potential predictor variables.

Sleep measurements

Sleep characteristics were assessed by means of sleep logs and wrist actigraphs. The a priori intent was to assess sleep periods for 4–5 consecutive work days. At baseline, this was achieved by the majority of subjects (mean and SD consecutive days of measurement: 4.5 ± 0.8). During the follow-up assessments, ad hoc scheduling caused the number of consecutive days recorded to be more variable. The follow-up assessments involved an average of 2.5 (T1: 2.4 ± 0.8, T2: 2.4 ± 0.8, T3: 2.6 ± 0.8) consecutive night shifts and subsequent daytime sleep periods.

Objective and subjective (self-report) characteristics of nighttime sleep (baseline assessment) and daytime sleep (follow-up assessments) were measured. Subjects filled out a sleep log upon awakening. They recorded bedtimes and any sleep medications used. On 5-point scales, they indicated whether their sleep was restful/restless and whether or not they felt well-rested. The average score on the two scales was used as a subjective sleep quality (SSQ).

Subjects also recorded use of alcohol. Sleep periods preceded by consumption of 4 or more alcoholic drinks were excluded from analyses.

During the baseline period (T0), subjects wore a wrist actigraph of Gaehwiler Electronics (Hombrechtikon, Switzerland). During the follow-up periods (T1–T3), they wore the smaller and more convenient Actiwatch of Cambridge Neurotechnology Ltd. (Cambridge, UK). Actigraphs were worn on the non-dominant arm, 24 h per day, except when subjects were in contact with water or doing vigorous physical exercise.

Total sleep time (TST) was extracted from the data provided by both actigraph types using the Actiwatch Sleep Analysis software (version 1.06, Cambridge Neurotechnology Ltd.), guided by the information recorded in the sleep logs.

TST and SSQ served as the primary outcome variables in this study.

Predictor variables

At baseline, background information about personal, demographic, and circadian characteristics was assessed by means of a computerized questionnaire.

For each primary outcome variable, six candidate predictors were assessed: baseline value of the primary outcome variable (TST/SSQ), age, gender, morningness, flexibility of sleeping habits, and vigorousness (i.e., the ability to overcome drowsiness). Morningness was tested with a validated 7-item morningness-eveningness questionnaire, VOA (range 7–31) (Kerkhof, Citation1984). Sleep flexibility (range 6–30) and vigorousness (range 4–20) were derived from subscales of a Dutch translation of the Circadian Type Questionnaire (CTQ) (Folkard et al., Citation1979), with a 5-point rating scale (10 items).

Statistical analyses

Analyses were performed using SAS version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Out of the 42 subjects, seventeen subjects had some missing data, due to equipment failure, ad hoc scheduling, or withdrawal from the study. One assessment was missing for twelve subjects, two assessments were missing for two subjects, and three assessments were missing for three subjects. Our analysis approach was not predicated on having complete data, so these subjects’ data were included and properly accounted for in the analyses.

The primary outcome variables (TST and SSQ) and the candidate predictors were first examined by means of descriptive statistics. The change in the primary outcome variables from T0 to T1–T3 was investigated using mixed-effects analysis of variance (ANOVA) with fixed effects for time point (T0–T3), cohort, and their interaction, and a random effect over subjects on the intercept (Van Dongen et al., Citation2004). A planned contrast was implemented in order to compare T0 to T1–T3.

The TST and SSQ data of the follow-up assessments were then analyzed with mixed-effects ANOVA with fixed effects for time point (T1–T3) and a random effect over subjects on the intercept. Inter-individual differences in overall responses to shift work sustained over T1–T3 were assessed using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), defined as the between-subjects variance divided by the sum of the between- and within-subjects variances (Van Dongen et al., Citation2004).

Next, the TST and SSQ data of the follow-up assessments were converted to difference scores (T2 minus T1, T3 minus T2). The difference scores were analyzed with mixed-effects ANOVA with fixed effects for time point (T2, T3), cohort, and their interaction, and a random effect over subjects on the intercept. Systematic inter-individual differences in the rate of change over time points were assessed using the ICC.

If inter-individual differences in overall responses to shift work or in the rate of change over time points were found to be statistically significant, then the analyses were repeated with baseline TST or baseline SSQ included as covariate, both separately and in interaction with time point. These last analyses were repeated with each of the five remaining candidate predictors (age, gender, morningness, flexibility of sleeping habits, and vigorousness), one by one, included as additional covariate, both separately and in interaction with time point. The statistical significance of each candidate predictor was assessed with a variance ratio test for nested models.

Results

shows the means and standard errors of TST and SSQ across the four measurement periods. For TST, there was a significant main effect of time point (F3,95 = 8.54, p < 0.001). The planned contrast comparing T0 to T1–T3 was significant (F1,95 = 22.48, p < 0.001), indicating that daytime TST after commencing shift work was an average of 48 min shorter than baseline nighttime TST. For SSQ, there was no significant main effect of time point, but the planned contrast comparing T0 to T1–T3 was significant (F1,87 = 4.80, p = 0.031) reflecting an overall increase (improvement) of 0.26 points in the SSQ rating from before to after commencing shift work. Note the discrepancy between the change in sleep as measured objectively (reduced TST) versus subjectively (improved SSQ).

Figure 1. Time courses (means and standard errors) for total sleep time (TST; top) and subjective sleep quality (SSQ; bottom) over the baseline assessment (T0) and the follow-up assessments after commencing shift work (T1–T3). SSQ ratings range from 1 to 5, with higher values indicating better subjective sleep quality. Values TST: 401 (8), 365 (11), 346 (11), 343 (11); SSQ: 3.2 (0.1), 3.5, (0.1), 3.5 (0.1), 3.3 (0.2).

Figure 1. Time courses (means and standard errors) for total sleep time (TST; top) and subjective sleep quality (SSQ; bottom) over the baseline assessment (T0) and the follow-up assessments after commencing shift work (T1–T3). SSQ ratings range from 1 to 5, with higher values indicating better subjective sleep quality. Values TST: 401 (8), 365 (11), 346 (11), 343 (11); SSQ: 3.2 (0.1), 3.5, (0.1), 3.5 (0.1), 3.3 (0.2).

Systematic inter-individual differences were observed for TST (ICC = 0.44, p = 0.002) and SSQ (ICC = 0.41, p = 0.007) during the follow-up assessments after commencing shift work (T1–T3). For TST, 44% of the variance was explained by systematic inter-individual variability. Thus, up to 44% of the variance in TST during shift work exposure could be predictable by the police officers’ baseline characteristics. For SSQ, 41% of the variance was explained by systematic inter-individual variability, and thus potentially predictable. There were no significant inter-individual differences in the rate of change from T1 to T3 in either TST or SSQ.

Candidate predictor analyses were based on a sample of N = 39, as baseline TST and SSQ values were missing in 3 police officers. Baseline TST (at T0) was not a significant predictor of inter-individual differences in daytime TST after commencing shift work (at T1–T3). However, baseline SSQ was a significant predictor of inter-individual differences in daytime SSQ after commencing shift work (F6,49 = 3.48, p = 0.006), with higher baseline SSQ predicting higher SSQ during the follow-up sessions.

Descriptive statistics and pairwise correlations of the remaining candidate predictors as measured at baseline are shown in . Age was correlated with gender – as a group, females (23 ± 3 y) were younger than males (28 ± 8 y). Vigorousness was related to both age and gender, where older age and being male were related to higher scores of vigorousness in our sample.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) and correlations of the person-specific characteristics.

Gender (F2,54 = 4.85, p = 0.012) and flexibility of sleeping habits (F2,54 = 4.21, p = 0.020) were found to be significant predictors of daytime TST during shift work. Females exhibited greater TST during the follow-up sessions than males, and flexible types showed greater TST during the follow-up sessions than more rigid types. Flexibility of sleeping habits was a significant predictor of daytime SSQ during shift work as well (F1,48 = 4.86, p = 0.032), but flexible types showed slightly lower SSQ over the three follow-up sessions than more rigid types.

Discussion

In this prospective study, we investigated inter-individual variability in the objective and subjective sleep response to shift work and sought to identify potential baseline predictors. We studied a relatively small sample of a population uniquely suited for our purpose, i.e., novice police officers. In their occupational setting, police officers are in a high-stress environment and they frequently experience irregular and extended shifts (Vila, Citation2006; Zimmerman, Citation2012). In this study, 41% of recorded work days were 9 h or longer. Our sample consisted of relatively young police officers who were fit for duty by the criteria of the Dutch Police Force and scored low on morningness (i.e., high on eveningness). These characteristics might have worked to their advantage in terms of shift work tolerance (Kecklund et al., Citation2008).

We observed systematic inter-individual differences in TST after commencing shift work. Entry into shift work was associated with daytime sleep, which was restricted (by 48 min on average) compared to nocturnal sleep prior to commencing shift work (Figure 1). Systematically reduced TST is important both operationally and clinically, as chronic sleep restriction is associated with degraded performance and safety and adverse health effects and decreased life expectancy (Cappuccio et al., Citation2011; Raslear et al., Citation2011).

We also observed systematic differences between subjects in the subjective quality of daytime sleep after commencing shift work. Counterintuitively, the SSQ of daytime sleep in shift work was slightly higher than the SSQ of nighttime sleep at baseline (Figure 1). The dissociation between changes in objective sleep duration and in SSQ may involve a relative change of subjective reference point rather than an absolute improvement in SSQ.

The presence of stable inter-individual differences in the overall sleep response to shift work (TST and SSQ) is consistent with our preliminary results reported previously (Lammers-van der Holst et al., Citation2006). A large amount (>40%) of the variance of TST and SSQ was explained by systematic inter-individual variability. This suggests that the sleep response to shift work may be predictable, and person-specific characteristics may be examined for their potential as predictors. We found that the SSQ of nighttime sleep prior to shift work was a significant predictor of the SSQ of daytime sleep after commencing shift work, which is consistent with results of a previous study (Radoševic-Vidaček et al., Citation1995).

Our finding that flexibility of sleeping habits predicted both the TST and the SSQ response to shift work is congruent, to some extent, with results of a prospective survey of novice nurses (West et al., Citation2007). This study reported a negative correlation between flexibility of sleeping habits and subjective sleep disturbance associated with night shift after 6 months of shift work; however, this relationship disappeared after 12 months of shift work (West et al., Citation2007). Our finding that females (which constituted only 28.6% of the sample) showed greater TST values after night shifts than males would seem to contradict the typical finding that female gender predisposes to shift work intolerance (Saksvik et al., Citation2011). In our sample, however, females as a group were younger (23 ± 3 y) than males (28 ± 8 y). The female shift workers in our sample may not have had domestic duties and/or children at home yet, which is believed to be a significant contributor to shift work intolerance in shift workers in general (Monk, Citation1988).

Enduring sleep deficiencies is considered to be a major component of shift work intolerance (Reinberg & Ashkenazi, Citation2008). Early detection of sleep deficiencies is an essential first step in the deployment of targeted treatments and countermeasures in order to decrease the risks of sleepiness-related accidents and help prevent long-term adverse health outcomes. The inter-individual variability in the sleep response to commencing shift work documented in this paper underlines the importance of an individualized approach to this issue (Van Dongen, Citation2006). Participation of the individual shift worker in scheduling working times, for instance through self-rostering, offers one potential strategy for mitigating sleep problems and improving shift work tolerance (Albertsen et al., Citation2014).

Declaration of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Dutch Police Force for their support of this study.

Funding

This study was supported by ZonMw (“Zorg Onderzoek Nederland”) project number 40100010, awarded to G.A. Kerkhof. H.P.A. Van Dongen was supported by Office of Naval Research grant N00014-13-1-0302.

Additional information

Funding

This study was supported by ZonMw (“Zorg Onderzoek Nederland”) project number 40100010, awarded to G.A. Kerkhof. H.P.A. Van Dongen was supported by Office of Naval Research grant N00014-13-1-0302.

References

  • Åkerstedt T. (2003). Shift work and disturbed sleep/wakefulness. Occup Med. 53:89–94.
  • Åkerstedt T, Kecklund G, Knutsson A. (1991). Manifest sleepiness and the spectral content of the EEG during shift work. Sleep. 14:221–5.
  • Åkerstedt T, Wright KP. (2009). Sleep loss and fatigue in shift work and shift work disorder. Sleep Med Clin. 4:257–71.
  • Albertsen K, Garde AH, Nabe-Nielsen K, et al. (2014). Work-life balance among shift workers: Results from an intervention study about self-rostering. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 87:265–74.
  • Boudreau P, Dumont GA, Boivin DB. (2013). Circadian adaptation to night shift work influences sleep, performance, mood and the autonomic modulation of the heart. PLoS One. 8:e70813.
  • Cappuccio FP, Cooper D, D’Elia L, et al. (2011). Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Eur Heart J. 32:1484–92.
  • Costa G. (2003). Factors influencing health of workers and tolerance to shift work. Theor Issues Ergon Sci. 4:263–88.
  • Drake CL, Roehrs T, Richardson G, et al. (2004). Shift work sleep disorder: Prevalence and consequences beyond that of symptomatic day workers. Sleep. 27:1453–62.
  • Folkard S, Monk TH, Lobuan M. (1979). Towards a predictive test of adjustment to shift work. Ergonomics. 22:79–91.
  • Hakola T, Härmä MI, Laitinen JT. (1996). Circadian adjustment of men and women to night work. Scand J Work Environ Health. 22:133–8.
  • Härmä M. (1993). Individual differences in tolerance to shiftwork: A review. Ergonomics. 36:101–9.
  • Härmä M, Kandolin I. (2001). Shiftwork, age and well-being: Recent developments and future perspectives. J Hum Ergol. 30:568–73.
  • Iskra-Golec I. (1993). The relationship between circadian, personality, and temperament characteristics and attitude towards shiftwork. Ergonomics. 36:149–53.
  • Kecklund G, Eriksen CA, Åkerstedt T. (2008). Police officers attitude to different shift systems: Association with age, present shift schedule, health and sleep/wake complaints. Appl Ergon. 39:565–71.
  • Kerkhof GA. (1984). A Dutch-language questionnaire for the selection of morning and evening type individuals. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie. 39:281–94.
  • Kerkhof GA. (1985). Inter-individual differences in the human circadian system: A review. Biol Psychol. 20:83–112.
  • Kerkhof GA, Lancel M. (1991). EEG slow wave activity, REM sleep, and rectal temperature during night and day sleep in morning‐type and evening‐type subjects. Psychophysiology. 28:678–88.
  • Knutsson A, Åkerstedt T. (1992). The healthy-worker effect: Self-selection among Swedish shift workers. Work Stress. 6:163–7.
  • Lammers-van der Holst HM, Van Dongen HPA, Kerkhof GA. (2006). Are individuals’ nighttime sleep characteristics prior to shift-work exposure predictive for parameters of daytime sleep after commencing shift work? Chronobiol Int. 23:1217–27.
  • Monk TH. (1988). Shiftwork: Determinants of coping ability and areas of application. In Hekkens W, Kerkhof G, Rietveld W, eds. Trends in chronobiology. Leiden: Pergamon Press, p. 195–207.
  • Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Billy BD, et al. (2013). Shiftworkers report worse sleep than day workers, even in retirement. J Sleep Res. 22:201–8.
  • Morris CJ, Yang JN, Scheer FA. (2012). The impact of the circadian timing system on cardiovascular and metabolic function. Prog Brain Res. 199:337–58.
  • Portaluppi F, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y. (2010). Ethics and methods for biological rhythm research on animals and human beings. Chronobiol Int. 27:1911–29.
  • Puttonen S, Härmä M, Hublin C. (2010). Shift work and cardiovascular disease—Pathways from circadian stress to morbidity. Scand J Work Environ Health. 36:96–108.
  • Radoševic-Vidaček B, Vidaček S, Kaliterna L, Prizmic Z. (1995). Sleep and napping in young shiftworkers: A 5-year follow-up. Work Stress. 9:272–80.
  • Rajaratnam SMW, Howard ME, Grunstein RR. (2013). Sleep loss and circadian disruption in shift work: Health burden and management. Med J Aust. 199:11–15.
  • Raslear T, Hursh S, Van Dongen HPA. (2011). Predicting cognitive impairment and accident risk. Prog Brain Res. 190:155–67.
  • Reinberg A, Ashkenazi I. (2008). Internal desynchronization of circadian rhythms and tolerance to shift work. Chronobiol Int. 25:625–43.
  • Roth T. (2012). Shift work disorder: Overview and diagnosis. J Clin Psychiatry. 73:e09.
  • Saksvik IB, Bjorvatn B, Hetland H, et al. (2011). Individual differences in tolerance to shift work – A systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 15:221–35.
  • Storemark SS, Fossum IN, Bjorvatn B, et al. (2013). Personality factors predict sleep-related shift work tolerance in different shifts at 2-year follow-up: A prospective study. BMJ Open. 3:e003696.
  • Van Dongen HPA. (2006). Shift work and inter-individual differences in sleep and sleepiness. Chronobiol Int. 23:1139–47.
  • Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G. (2009). Individual differences in vulnerability to sleep loss in the work environment. Ind Health. 47:518–26.
  • Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Dinges DF. (2004). Dealing with inter-individual differences in the temporal dynamics of fatigue and performance: Importance and techniques. Aviat Space Environ Med. 75:A147–54.
  • Vila B. (2006). Impact of long work hours on police officers and the communities they serve. Am J Ind Med. 49:972–80.
  • Vogel M, Braungardt T, Meyer W, Schneider W. (2012). The effects of shift work on physical and mental health. J Neural Transm. 119:1121–32.
  • Wang XS, Armstrong MEG, Cairns BJ, et al. (2011). Shift work and chronic disease: The epidemiological evidence. Occup Med. 61:78–89.
  • West SH, Ahern M, Byrnes M, Kwanten L. (2007). New graduate nurses adaptation to shift work: Can we help? Collegian. 14:23–30.
  • Wright KP, Bogan RK, Wyatt JK. (2013). Shift work and the assessment and management of shift work disorder (SWD). Sleep Med Rev. 17:41–54.
  • Zimmerman FH. (2012). Cardiovascular disease and risk factors in law enforcement personnel: A comprehensive review. Cardiol Rev. 20:159–66.