Publication Cover
Stress
The International Journal on the Biology of Stress
Volume 16, 2013 - Issue 5
415
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Experience-dependent effects of context and restraint stress on corticolimbic c-Fos expression

, , , , &
Pages 587-591 | Received 07 Jan 2013, Accepted 23 Apr 2013, Published online: 20 Jun 2013

Abstract

Stressors are typically multidimensional, comprised of multiple physical and sensory components that rarely occur as single isolated events. This study used a 2-day stress exposure paradigm to assess functional activation patterns (by Fos expression) in key corticolimbic structures following repeated context, repeated restraint, context followed by restraint or restraint followed by context. On day 1, rats were transported to a novel context and either restrained for 6 h or left undisturbed. On day 2, these two groups were either restrained or not in the same context, then processed for Fos immunohistochemistry. Regardless of prior stress experience, rats exposed to context only on day 2 expressed more Fos-like immunoreactive (IR) labeling in CA1 and CA3 of dorsal hippocampus, basolateral amygdala and central amygdala than those that were not. This pattern was reversed in the dentate gyrus infrapyramidal blade. In contrast, in the infralimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the experience of a single restraint on either day 1 or day 2 rats elevated Fos-like IR relative to rats that had been exposed to context alone. These data show that exposure to context produces robust Fos induction in the hippocampus and amygdala, regardless of prior experience with restraint and compared to the immediate experience of restraint, with prior experience modulating Fos expression within the mPFC.

Introduction

It is well established that a real or perceived threat results in engagement of the neuroendocrine stress response via the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis. Upon detection of a stressor, the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) participates in a cascade of events that reliably results in the adrenal cortex releasing glucocorticoids (GCs), which have widespread effects throughout the body and brain. An early misconception about the neuroendocrine stress response is that it is nonspecific, given that the output (i.e. GCs) appears similar across stressor types (Selye, Citation1998). We now know that the stress response is specific and not all stressors engage the HPA axis in the same way with each exposure (Herman et al., Citation2005; Pacak et al., Citation1998).

Immediate early genes (IEG) have provided a powerful resource to investigate functional activation in the brain following a stressor (Kovacs, Citation1998; Stamp & Herbert, Citation1999). In rodents, Fos is rapidly induced in response to novel contexts and exposure to restraint, two robust stressors commonly used in many behavioral paradigms (Buynitsky & Mostofsky, Citation2009; Pfister, Citation1979). However, the extent to which restraint and context exposure in combination influence the functional activation of corticolimbic brain regions is not well understood. This is especially important because stressors are typically not unidimensional events, but are rather comprised of multiple physical and sensory components. For instance, a rodent’s encounter with a novel context might coincide with additional stressors, such as the visual or olfactory cues of a predator. In our paradigm, we implemented a widely used restraint manipulation to investigate the functional activation patterns of key corticolimbic structures in response to context exposure alone, its combination with restraint and how prior experience with either of these modulates subsequent activation. In this study, we re-exposed rats to the context in which a previous experience of restraint occurred once before and measured Fos-like immunoreactive (IR) labeling in forebrain and limbic structures that are sensitive to stress, including the hippocampus, amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). We predicted that context exposure and its combination with restraint will result in differential functional activation within limbic structures, with prior experience in either condition modulating subsequent Fos-like IR labeling.

Methods

Subjects

Thirty-two male Sprague–Dawley rats weighing ∼225–250 g upon arrival (Charles River Laboratories) were pair-housed (21–22 °C) on a 12:12 reverse light cycle (lights off at 6 a.m.). Food and water were available ad libitum. All procedures occurred between 9 a.m. to 4 p.m. The procedures followed the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources on Life Science, National Research Council, 1996) and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Arizona State University.

Experimental design

A 2 × 2 experimental design (n = 8/group) for condition on days 1 and 2 was implemented. On day 1, rat pairs were transported in their home cages to a novel context (four different contexts that were unoccupied rooms with various behavioral testing and laboratory equipment, counterbalanced across conditions) and were either left undisturbed or restrained in wire mesh restrainers (16 cm circumference, 24 cm length) for 6 h, then removed from restraint within that context and returned back to the home colony. On day 2, rats were again transported in their home cages back to the same context, restrained or left undisturbed, depending on group assignment. Rats in the restraint condition on either day were given ∼3 min to form a representation of the context with lights on. Lights were then turned off to stay consistent with the reverse light cycle. Group assignments were: days 1 and 2 context only (CC), day 1 context and day 2 restraint (CR), day 1 restraint and day 2 context (RC), and days 1 and 2 restraint (RR; see ).

Figure 1. Experimental timeline. Pair-housed rats were transported in their home cage to a novel context in which they were restrained (R) or left undisturbed in the context (C) for 6 h, then returned to the colony. The next day, rats again were transported to the previous day’s context and either restrained or left undisturbed, yielding four groups (first and second letter represent conditions on days 1 and 2, respectively; CC, CR, RC, RR). All groups were euthanized 90 min after placement in condition on day 2 for tissue processing.

Figure 1. Experimental timeline. Pair-housed rats were transported in their home cage to a novel context in which they were restrained (R) or left undisturbed in the context (C) for 6 h, then returned to the colony. The next day, rats again were transported to the previous day’s context and either restrained or left undisturbed, yielding four groups (first and second letter represent conditions on days 1 and 2, respectively; CC, CR, RC, RR). All groups were euthanized 90 min after placement in condition on day 2 for tissue processing.

Procedures on day 2 lasted 90 min, to capture peak context- or restraint-induced Fos expression (Nikulina et al., Citation2004). Rats were overdosed (Euthasol, 100 mg/kg, i.p.) 90 min after placement in the respective day 2 conditions, and transcardially perfused with heparinized phosphate buffered saline (pH 7.4) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (pH 7.4). Brains were removed and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight (4 °C). Brains were then cryoprotected in 15 and 30% sucrose over 2 days and stored (4°C) until sectioning.

Tissue preparation and Fos immunohistochemistry

Brains were sectioned (40 µm), mounted on subbed slides and stored (−80 °C) until tissue processing. Target sections were washed (3×, 0.05 M potassium-phosphate buffered saline, KPBS, pH 7.4) and incubated in 5% normal goat serum/0.05 M KPBS/0.4% Triton-X (60 min, room temperature). Sections of dorsal hippocampus, amygdala and mPFC were incubated with rabbit polyclonal antisera for Fos (Ab-5, Calbiochem, 1:15,000 in 5% normal goat serum/0.05 M KPBS/0.4% Triton-X). Following incubation (48 h, 4 °C), sections were incubated with avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (Vectastain ABC kit) for 45 min, then washed again in KPBS and processed using DAB with nickel-intensification (DAB peroxidase substrate kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). This procedure was adapted from Nikulina et al. (2004).

Fos protein analysis

Tissue sections were examined for the presence of a dark grey reaction product indicating immobilized antigen. For each group, data were obtained from 4 to 6 sections/brain through each subregion on both hemispheres, and averaged across each region for each rat. Selected areas (30,000 µm2 for the hippocampal regions and 150,000 µm2 for the mPFC and amygdala regions) were captured and digitized using a camera (CX9000, MBF Biosciences, Burlington, VT) interfaced with a microscope (Olympus BX51), using a 20× objective. A cell profile was considered labeled if its pixel intensity was >2 SDs darker than the background, as calculated by Stereo Investigator software (MBF Biosciences). For hippocampal analyses, targeted subregions included CA1, CA3 and the infrapyramidal (or ventral, lower) and suprapyramidal (or dorsal, upper) blades of the dentate gyrus (DGInf and DGSup, respectively). Sampling areas within each subregion were consistent among each hippocampal slice. Once each subregion was identified at 20×, the subregion was outlined and Stereo Investigator calculated the area (mm2). All positively labeled profiles were quantified and that value was divided by the area value to determine a density value. For the mPFC and amygdala analyses, adjacent cresyl violet stained sections were used to localize regions or nuclei with high confidence because the borders of these regions are less distinct; for mPFC, analyzed regions included anterior cingulate cortex (ACG), prelimbic cortex (PL) and infralimbic cortex (IL); for the amygdala, analyzed nuclei included basolateral amygdala (BLA), central amygdala (CEA) and medial amygdala (MEA). Neuronal labeling was quantified using a systematic random approach to achieve unbiased counts. Stereo Investigator software partitioned each image into 20 equal counting frames (100 μm × 75 μm each), half of which were randomly selected and analyzed. Labeling density was calculated by dividing the estimated total number of labeled profiles by the total area analyzed (adapted from Fanous et al. (Citation2011)). Intra-rater reliability measures were performed with 92.1% reliability. Inter-rater reliability measures were performed with 93.9% reliability.

Data analysis

For immunohistochemistry data, specific inclusion criteria were implemented to ensure that sufficient data per animal were used for analyses and defined as having at least four quantifiable subregions per animal. Those excluded were due to technical complications from tissue processing. Group sizes ranged from n = 3 to 8, depending on the group and specific brain region. Data were analyzed by two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). When significant interactions were detected at p ≤ 0.05, post hoc analyses were performed. Data are represented as mean ± SEM.

Results

Hippocampus

Separate omnibus ANOVAs performed for each hippocampal subregion (CA1, CA3, DGInf and DGSup) revealed that Fos expression differed depending upon restraint condition (CC, CR, RC, RR) in each area examined. Two-way ANOVAs were performed using day 1 condition (restraint or context) and day 2 condition (restraint or context) as factors. Fos-like IR labeling data (Fos-positive cells/mm2) was assessed in each subregion. ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of day 2 for CA1, CA3 and DGInf (CA1, F1,23 = 58.548, p < 0.001; CA3, F1,23 = 33.66, p < 0.001; DGInf, F1,22 = 9.19, p < 0.01, ), but not DGSup (main effect for day 2, F1,22 = 3.783, p = 0.06). These effects were consistent with a significant increase of Fos-like IR expression within CA1 and CA3 for groups that were placed in the context on day 2 and not restrained (CC, RC) compared to those restrained on day 2 (regardless of day 1 experience, CR, RR). In contrast, within the DGInf, there was a significant increase of Fos-like IR expression in groups that were restrained on day 2 (CR, RR) compared to those that were placed in the context and not restrained on day 2 (CC, RC). Therefore, conditions on day 2 predominately influenced Fos-IR labeling in the CA1, CA3 and DGInf, regardless of the experience on day 1. No other effects were significant.

Figure 2. Fos-IR labeling in hippocampus. (A) Regardless of day 1 experience, groups placed in context and not restrained on day 2 (CC, RC) showed elevated Fos-IR labeling in CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus compared to those restrained on day 2 (***p < 0.001 versus CR, RR in CA1; ***p < 0.001 versus CR, RR in CA3). By contrast, groups that were restrained on day 2 (CR, RR) exhibited greater Fos-IR labeling in the infrapyramidal blade of the dentate gyrus (DGInf) compared to those that were placed in the context but not restrained (§p < 0.05 versus CC, RC; n = 6–8/group). (B) Representative photomicrographs of Fos immunolabeling across groups in the selected subregions of the hippocampus. Scale bar: 100 μm.

Figure 2. Fos-IR labeling in hippocampus. (A) Regardless of day 1 experience, groups placed in context and not restrained on day 2 (CC, RC) showed elevated Fos-IR labeling in CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus compared to those restrained on day 2 (***p < 0.001 versus CR, RR in CA1; ***p < 0.001 versus CR, RR in CA3). By contrast, groups that were restrained on day 2 (CR, RR) exhibited greater Fos-IR labeling in the infrapyramidal blade of the dentate gyrus (DGInf) compared to those that were placed in the context but not restrained (§p < 0.05 versus CC, RC; n = 6–8/group). (B) Representative photomicrographs of Fos immunolabeling across groups in the selected subregions of the hippocampus. Scale bar: 100 μm.

Amygdala

Within the amygdala BLA and CEA, significant main effects of day 2 manipulation (restraint or context) on Fos expression were observed (BLA, F1,24 = 8.546, p < 0.01; CEA, F1,24 = 14.655, p = 0.001, ). These analyses indicated significantly greater BLA and CEA Fos-IR labeling in the groups that were placed in context only on day 2 (CC, RC) compared to the group that were restrained, regardless of their experience on day 1 (CR, RR). No other effects were significant.

Figure 3. Fos-IR labeling in amygdala and mPFC. (A) Groups that were placed in a novel context but not restrained on day 2 exhibited elevated Fos expression in BLA and CEA compared to those that were restrained (**p < 0.01 versus CR, RR in BLA; ***p = 0.001 versus CR, RR in CEA; n = 5–8/group). (B) Restraint and context conditions altered Fos labeling within the IL without significantly affecting the PL or the ACG. Specifically, rats placed in the previous day’s restraint context (RC) but not restrained showed elevated Fos-IR labeling within the IL compared to those that were never restrained (#p < 0.05 versus CC). Rats that were acutely restrained (CR) also exhibited greater Fos-IR labeling compared to those that were never restrained (##p < 0.01 versus CC; n = 3–8/group).

Figure 3. Fos-IR labeling in amygdala and mPFC. (A) Groups that were placed in a novel context but not restrained on day 2 exhibited elevated Fos expression in BLA and CEA compared to those that were restrained (**p < 0.01 versus CR, RR in BLA; ***p = 0.001 versus CR, RR in CEA; n = 5–8/group). (B) Restraint and context conditions altered Fos labeling within the IL without significantly affecting the PL or the ACG. Specifically, rats placed in the previous day’s restraint context (RC) but not restrained showed elevated Fos-IR labeling within the IL compared to those that were never restrained (#p < 0.05 versus CC). Rats that were acutely restrained (CR) also exhibited greater Fos-IR labeling compared to those that were never restrained (##p < 0.01 versus CC; n = 3–8/group).

Medial prefrontal cortex

In contrast to the hippocampus and amygdala subregions, conditions on day 1 impacted Fos expression within the mPFC depending on day 2 conditions. A two-way ANOVA for conditions on days 1 and 2 revealed a significant day 1 × day 2 interaction in the IL (F1,15 = 10.827, p < 0.01) but not the PL (F1,20 = 3.894, p = 0.06; ). Post hoc analyses revealed that rats previously placed in the restraint context (RC) exhibited greater IL Fos-like IR labeling compared to those that were never restrained (CC; p < 0.05). Additionally, rats that were acutely restrained (CR) had significantly greater Fos-like IR labeling compared to those that were never restrained (CC; p < 0.01). Similar patterns were observed within the PL, albeit not statistically significant. No other effects were significant.

Discussion

In the current study, we used a 2-day exposure paradigm to understand the experience-dependent relationship between context and restraint on functional activation patterns, assessed by cellular labeling of Fos-like IR protein in subregions of the hippocampus, amygdala and mPFC. Overall, context exposure on day 2 increased Fos expression in the hippocampus (CA1 and CA3) and amygdala (BLA and CEA), regardless of the experience on the previous day. The DGInf exhibited contrasting patterns, with groups restrained on day 2 showing elevated expression of Fos. Interestingly, we found interactions between conditions on days 1 and 2 for Fos expression in the mPFC. In the IL, restraint on day 1 followed by context exposure only on day 2 (RC) and context on day 1 followed by restraint on day 2 (CR) expressed significantly more Fos compared to animals with no experience with restraint (CC). While many studies have documented that the immediate experience of restraint or context engages the corticolimbic system (Melia et al., Citation1994; Pace et al., Citation2005; VanElzakker et al., Citation2008), the present data suggest that context might engage the limbic system to a greater extent than does restraint or the combination of these two. Furthermore, the mPFC has a role in the modulation of experience-dependent corticolimbic engagement to context or its combination with restraint.

In the present study, groups that were exposed to context on day 2 but not restrained (CC, RC), exhibited consistent patterns of elevated Fos expression in the hippocampus (CA1 and CA3) and amygdala (BLA and CEA), compared to groups that were restrained on day 2 (CR, RR). Specifically, prior experience did not alter the immediate experience in response to context. One interpretation is that context exposure alone engaged the neurocircuitry of the CA1, CA3, BLA and CEA regions, as the dorsal hippocampus is important in context integration (Fanselow, Citation2000) and sends projections to the amygdala (Pitkanen et al., Citation2000). Novel contexts are also stressors in rodents (Pfister, Citation1979; van den Buuse et al., Citation2001) and the combined functional activation of the hippocampus and amygdala suggests that a second exposure to context still maintained novelty. Our data corroborate findings that environmental novelty induces robust Fos expression in the hippocampus (VanElzakker et al., Citation2008), and that exposure to experimental testing cages alone results in Fos induction in the hippocampus and amygdala (Campeau et al., Citation1997). It is also possible that restraint suppressed context-induced Fos induction in these regions. Some evidence reveals that acute stress increases GABAergic inhibitory transmission within the hippocampus (Bowers et al., Citation1998), which might suppress neighboring pyramidal cell Fos induction in those restrained on day 2. Future double-labeling studies could address this possibility.

Within the DG, in the DGinf restraint enhanced Fos expression relative to context on day 2 (compare CR, RR with CC, RC), whereas expression was unaltered in the DGsup. The literature reveals that a variety of novel experiences reduce DGInf activation (Fevurly & Spencer, Citation2004; Pace et al., Citation2005; VanElzakker et al., Citation2008). Moreover, some IEG products are differentially expressed in the two blades of the DG after various stressor challenges (Fevurly & Spencer, Citation2004; Pace et al., Citation2005; VanElzakker et al., Citation2008), corroborating our findings.

The mPFC (IL) was the only area where experience-dependent modulation of Fos induction was observed. Previous research on dendritic arborization found the mPFC to be exquisitely sensitive to subtle environmental challenges, as dendritic retraction occurred with vehicle injections (Wellman, Citation2001) or after 1 week of restraint manipulations (Brown et al., Citation2005). This contrasts with the several weeks required for hippocampal dendritic changes (McLaughlin et al., Citation2007). It is noteworthy to speculate that the mPFC is the first to undergo modifications based on experiences that contribute to adjustments in neuroplasticity. However, more studies are needed on the time course of experience-dependent effects of context and restraint on IEG induction.

Our Fos data reveal differential effects of context and its combination with restraint within the hippocampus, amygdala and mPFC, suggesting that these stress-sensitive structures integrate environmental experiences differently. We show that exposure to context drives functional activation in comparison to its combination with restraint in the majority of the hippocampus and amygdala, with restraint engaging these structures to a lesser degree. Only the IL mPFC and perhaps the PL (which has similar, though not significant expression changes) give evidence for experience-dependent influences of restraint exposure on subsequent functional activation to context. One caveat is that Fos is not constitutively expressed: it is rapidly induced by many stimuli, including acute restraint (Melia et al., Citation1994), but then quickly returns to baseline, undetectable levels of expression. Consequently, any effects that suppress activity or produce lasting effects would not be detected and the main reason for omitting a quiet control for comparison. Future directions would include using an IEG that can be up or down regulated. In summary, the experience-dependent relationship between context and restraint on functional activation patterns corroborates the expectation that experiences do not occur in isolation, but are complex and may build upon each other, with perhaps the mPFC being one of the first regions revealing experience-dependent outcomes.

Declaration of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

This research was supported by funding from the Arizona Biomedical Research Commission (C.D.C.), NIH grant DA0226451 (E.M.N.), the ASU School of Life Sciences and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute through the Undergraduate Science Education Program and Barrett Honors College (D.P.A.).

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Miles Orchinik and Heather A. Bimonte-Nelson.

References

  • Bowers G, Cullinan WE, Herman JP. (1998). Region-specific regulation of glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) mRNA expression in central stress circuits. J Neurosci 18:5938–47
  • Brown SM, Henning S, Wellman CL. (2005). Mild, short-term stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 15:1714–22
  • Buynitsky T, Mostofsky DI. (2009). Restraint stress in biobehavioral research: recent developments. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:1089–98
  • Campeau S, Falls WA, Cullinan WE, Helmreich DL, Davis M, Watson SJ. (1997). Elicitation and reduction of fear: behavioural and neuroendocrine indices and brain induction of the immediate-early gene c-fos. Neuroscience 78:1087–104
  • Fanous S, Lacagnina MJ, Nikulina EM, Hammer RP Jr. (2011). Sensitized activation of Fos and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the medial prefrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area accompanies behavioral sensitization to amphetamine. Neuropharmacology 61:558–64
  • Fanselow MS. (2000). Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res 110:73–81
  • Fevurly RD, Spencer RL. (2004). Fos expression is selectively and differentially regulated by endogenous glucocorticoids in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus and the dentate gyrus. J Neuroendocrinol 16:970–9
  • Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H. (2005). Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 29:1201–13
  • Kovacs KJ. (1998). c-Fos as a transcription factor: a stressful (re)view from a functional map. Neurochem Int 33:287–97
  • McLaughlin KJ, Gomez JL, Baran SE, Conrad CD. (2007). The effects of chronic stress on hippocampal morphology and function: an evaluation of chronic restraint paradigms. Brain Res 1161:56–64
  • Melia KR, Ryabinin AE, Schroeder R, Bloom FE, Wilson MC. (1994). Induction and habituation of immediate early gene expression in rat brain by acute and repeated restraint stress. J Neurosci 14:5929–38
  • Nikulina EM, Covington HE III, Ganschow L, Hammer RP Jr, Miczek KA. (2004). Long-term behavioral and neuronal cross-sensitization to amphetamine induced by repeated brief social defeat stress: Fos in the ventral tegmental area and amygdala. Neuroscience 123:857–65
  • Pacak K, Palkovits M, Yadid G, Kvetnansky R, Kopin IJ, Goldstein DS. (1998). Heterogeneous neurochemical responses to different stressors: a test of Selye’s doctrine of nonspecificity. Am J Physiol 275:R1247–55
  • Pace TW, Gaylord R, Topczewski F, Girotti M, Rubin B, Spencer RL. (2005). Immediate-early gene induction in hippocampus and cortex as a result of novel experience is not directly related to the stressfulness of that experience. Eur J Neurosci 22:1679–90
  • Pfister HP. (1979). The glucocorticosterone response to novelty as a psychological stressor. Physiol Behav 23:649–52
  • Pitkanen A, Pikkarainen M, Nurminen N, Ylinen A. (2000). Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the hippocampal formation, perirhinal cortex, and postrhinal cortex in rat. A review. Ann N Y Acad Sci 911:369–91
  • Selye H. (1998). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. 1936. J Neuropsychiat Clin Neurosci 10:230–1
  • Stamp JA, Herbert J. (1999). Multiple immediate-early gene expression during physiological and endocrine adaptation to repeated stress. Neuroscience 94:1313–22
  • van den Buuse M, Van Acker SA, Fluttert M, De Kloet ER. (2001). Blood pressure, heart rate, and behavioral responses to psychological “novelty” stress in freely moving rats. Psychophysiology 38:490–9
  • VanElzakker M, Fevurly RD, Breindel T, Spencer RL. (2008). Environmental novelty is associated with a selective increase in Fos expression in the output elements of the hippocampal formation and the perirhinal cortex. Learn Mem 15:899–908
  • Wellman CL. (2001). Dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons in medial prefrontal cortex after chronic corticosterone administration. J Neurobiol 49:245–53

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.