Publication Cover
Stress
The International Journal on the Biology of Stress
Volume 18, 2015 - Issue 4
6,871
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

Stress exposure, food intake and emotional state

, , , &
Pages 381-399 | Received 20 Feb 2015, Accepted 13 Jun 2015, Published online: 13 Aug 2015

Abstract

This manuscript summarizes the proceedings of the symposium entitled, “Stress, Palatable Food and Reward”, that was chaired by Drs. Linda Rinaman and Yvonne Ulrich-Lai at the 2014 Neurobiology of Stress Workshop held in Cincinnati, OH. This symposium comprised research presentations by four neuroscientists whose work focuses on the biological bases for complex interactions among stress, food intake and emotion. First, Dr Ulrich-Lai describes her rodent research exploring mechanisms by which the rewarding properties of sweet palatable foods confer stress relief. Second, Dr Stephanie Fulton discusses her work in which excessive, long-term intake of dietary lipids, as well as their subsequent withdrawal, promotes stress-related outcomes in mice. Third, Dr Mark Wilson describes his group’s research examining the effects of social hierarchy-related stress on food intake and diet choice in group-housed female rhesus macaques, and compared the data from monkeys to results obtained in analogous work using rodents. Finally, Dr Gorica Petrovich discusses her research program that is aimed at defining cortical–amygdalar–hypothalamic circuitry responsible for curbing food intake during emotional threat (i.e. fear anticipation) in rats. Their collective results reveal the complexity of physiological and behavioral interactions that link stress, food intake and emotional state, and suggest new avenues of research to probe the impact of genetic, metabolic, social, experiential and environmental factors on these interactions.

Introduction

Acute or chronic exposure to stress (defined as an ongoing or anticipated threat to homeostasis or well-being) evokes a constellation of physiological and behavioral responses that markedly alter metabolic and behavioral state in humans and experimental animals (Dallman et al., Citation2003). Stress-induced activation of the neuroendocrine hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis increases glucocorticoid synthesis and glucose availability to fuel the metabolic demands of other physiological and behavioral stress responses. Glucocorticoids also regulate the accumulation and storage of body fat, and can increase appetite, food intake and body weight gain. Acute and chronic exposure to stress can alter both the quantity and quality of calories consumed, and stress-induced alterations in food intake and energy balance can interact with emotional state (Epel et al., Citation2001). However, although functional associations among stress, food intake/energy balance and emotion are readily apparent, mechanisms linking these outcomes are poorly understood. Compelling experimental evidence supports the view that stress can either increase or decrease caloric intake, and numerous reports indicate that chronic stress exposure can promote either hyperphagia and obesity, or anorexia and loss of body weight within certain dietary environments.

Stressors impact energy balance and affective state in a manner that depends on a multitude of biological and environmental factors, including temporal, genetic, social, contextual, species-specific, sex-dependent, nutritional, developmental, metabolic and experience-dependent elements. This profound complexity offers unique opportunities for researchers to selectively manipulate biological and environmental conditions in order to probe the functional underpinnings of causal relationships of interest. In this Symposium, four researchers present their experimental work investigating a variety of linkages among stress, food choice and intake, body weight gain and emotional behavior. The collective work of these investigators spans a broad array of biologically distinct models and approaches, and their findings highlight key similarities and differences in the complex relationship between stress and food intake that depend on both biological and environmental contexts.

In the second section of this Symposium report (Stress relief by palatable food reward), Dr Yvonne Ulrich-Lai presents findings by her research group exploring the ability of “comfort foods” to reduce physiological and behavioral stress responses in rats. In her experimental paradigm, rats that receive twice-daily limited access to 30% sucrose solution display no significant changes in body weight or adiposity, but display markedly attenuated behavioral and physiological stress responsiveness. The stress-reducing effect of sweet “comfort food” access not only depends on the nutritional properties of sucrose, but also appears to require reward signaling through central neural circuits that promote synaptic remodeling within the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala.

In the third section of this report (Emotional consequences of prolonged high-fat feeding), Dr Stephanie Fulton presents her laboratory's findings using an experimental model in which mice consuming a high-fat diet (HFD) become obese and display related metabolic derangements that serve to generate a state of stress. These diet-induced obese mice are characterized by a depressive-like behavioral phenotype and a hyper-responsive HPA stress axis. Further, when their HFD diet is replaced with standard chow (in an experimental “dieting” condition), the mice display behavioral and endocrine anxiety-like responses accompanied by increased motivation (operant responding) for palatable foods. Dr Fulton also presents evidence that saturated fats in the diet produce a larger effect than unsaturated fats to alter neural processes of emotion and reward.

In the fourth part of this Symposium report (Stress-induced eating), Dr Mark Wilson summarizes research using socially housed, non-human primates (female rhesus macaques) to explore the biological bases of stress-induced “emotional” over-eating. His research program also considers the role of social status and history of adverse experience on stress-induced eating. Dr Wilson compares and contrasts data from rodent and monkey studies to make the important point that changes in food intake after stress exposure depend critically on history of stress, the type of food available for consumption, and the sex of the animal, and he further suggests that stress-induced reductions in central dopamine signaling, possibly mediated through activation of central corticotropin releasing factor type 1 receptors (CRF-R1), may provide a common thread for increased eating and other goal-directed behaviors as a consequence of stress exposure.

While the initial sections of this Symposium report focus on the neurobiological substrates of increased food intake after stress, the final section (Stress-induced anorexia), prepared by Dr Gorica Petrovich, considers the opposite side of the coin: what are the neural substrates of hypophagic responses to stress? To probe the underlying circuits, Dr Petrovich's research group uses a rat model of conditioned fear in order to suppress food intake in hungry rats during exposure to danger cues that also increase anxiety-like behavior. Dr Petrovich reports striking sex differences in experimental outcomes, such that female rats take much longer to extinguish their conditioned “fear anorexia” compared to males. Brain regions that are critical for feeding suppression by conditioned fear cues include the prefrontal cortex, central amygdala and lateral hypothalamus, and she points out that selective neural signaling through the basolateral versus central amygdala may comprise a biological “fear-or-feeding” switch.

Stress relief by palatable food reward – Yvonne Ulrich-Lai

“Comfort food”

For most people, stress influences both the amount and types of food consumed. For example, approximately 35–60% of people report eating more total calories when they are experiencing stress, whereas approximately 25–40% of people report eating less (Epel et al., Citation2004; Oliver & Wardle, Citation1999; Weinstein et al., Citation1997). Furthermore, for many people, stress alters food selection towards eating a greater proportion of calories from highly-palatable foods (i.e. tasty, calorically-dense foods containing high amounts of sugars, other carbohydrates and/or fats) (Groesz et al., Citation2012; Kim et al., Citation2013; Laugero et al., Citation2011; McCann et al., Citation1990; Oliver & Wardle, Citation1999; Tryon et al., Citation2013), and this shift can even occur in individuals who reduce their total caloric intake during stress (Oliver & Wardle, Citation1999). This altered food selection is often referred to as eating “comfort food” – a term that reflects the idea that palatable food intake reduces stress responses, thereby providing a potential means for people to “self-medicate” for stress relief. Consistent with this idea, the intake of palatable and/or high carbohydrate food is associated with improved mood, decreased perceived stress and reduced plasma cortisol concentration in people (Anderson et al., Citation1987; Barr et al., Citation1999; de Castro, Citation1987; Deuster et al., Citation1992; Dube et al., Citation2005; Fernandez et al., Citation2003; Gibson, Citation2006; Lieberman et al., Citation1986; Utter et al., Citation1999), particularly those with high stress-proneness (Markus et al., Citation2000).

Similar changes in palatable food intake are observed in rodents during stress. When rats are offered both a highly palatable food (e.g. lard, sucrose) and a less palatable food (e.g. chow), they choose to eat a large proportion of their daily calories as the highly-palatable food (la Fleur et al., Citation2014; Packard et al., Citation2014; Pecoraro et al., Citation2004; Zeeni et al., Citation2013). When these rodents are then exposed to chronic stress, palatable food intake is generally preserved whereas chow intake is reduced, resulting in a greater proportion of their daily food intake being derived from the palatable food source (Packard et al., Citation2014; Pecoraro et al., Citation2004). Moreover, elevated palatable food intake in rodents is associated with reduced stress indices, including anxiety-related behaviors, stress-induced learned helplessness, post-stress plasma corticosterone concentration and reactions to pain and distress (Bell et al., Citation2002; Francolin-Silva et al., Citation2006; la Fleur et al., Citation2005; Minor & Saade, Citation1997; Prasad & Prasad, Citation1996; Shide & Blass, Citation1989; Strack et al., Citation1997; Suchecki et al., Citation2003). Based on evidence reviewed by Dr Fulton (see next section), such relief may be transitory/temporary in the face of continued stress exposure, which generally leads to increased HPA activation that, in turn, may sustain palatable food intake and promote obesity. Taken together, however, results from human and rodent studies strongly suggest that palatable food intake blunts acute stress responses, although the mechanisms by which such “comfort” foods engender stress relief are largely unknown. In order to explore these mechanisms, we first developed and characterized a paradigm of limited exposure to a sucrose drink, as described below.

Limited sucrose intake paradigm reduces stress responses

To explore the mechanisms by which moderate palatable food intake dampens stress, we have developed a limited sucrose intake (LSI) “snacking” model, in which adult male rats with ad libitum chow and water available are given additional brief (30 min) twice-daily access to a small amount (4 ml) of 30% sucrose solution (or water as a control) for 2–4 weeks (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007, Citation2010). The rats rapidly begin to drink the sucrose solution, but not the water, with intakes approximating the maximum permitted (8 ml per day, ∼9 kcal/d) (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007). Rats with this LSI modestly decrease chow intake (by ∼9 kcal/d), resulting in no effect on body weight or adiposity (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007). As a result, this sucrose paradigm allows us to explore the effects of a modest dietary change on stress responses without the additional, and perhaps confounding, effects of increased body weight and adiposity that could alone influence stress systems (see later section by Dr Fulton).

A history of LSI blunts several types of behavioral and physiological responses to stress. For example, LSI reduces indices of anxiety-related behaviors () in social interaction, elevated-plus maze and open field tests of behavioral anxiety (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). In addition, both CRF mRNA expression in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007) and HPA axis hormonal responses to a subsequent acute stressor (e.g. restraint) are decreased after 14 days of LSI () (Christiansen et al., Citation2011, Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007, Citation2010, Citation2011). The tachycardic response to restraint is also reduced following LSI (), suggesting that the sympathetic response to acute stress is similarly blunted by sucrose ingestion (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). These data support the view that LSI provides a general stress relief that impacts multiple components of the stress response.

Figure 1. A history of limited sucrose intake reduces behavioral and physiological responses to stress. (A) In a social interaction test of behavioral anxiety, rats with a history of limited sucrose (S) drink spent a greater proportion of time interacting socially with a novel conspecific relative to water (W) controls, regardless of whether they received an acute restraint stress (Restraint) or not (No Restraint) immediately prior to the social interaction test (left). *p < 0.05 versus water controls. This increased social interaction was similar to that which occurs after treatment with the known anxiolytic diazepam (D) versus vehicle (V) (right). *p < 0.05 versus vehicle. (B) The tachycardic response to acute restraint was reduced by limited sucrose drink relative to water controls. *p < 0.05 versus water controls. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

Figure 1. A history of limited sucrose intake reduces behavioral and physiological responses to stress. (A) In a social interaction test of behavioral anxiety, rats with a history of limited sucrose (S) drink spent a greater proportion of time interacting socially with a novel conspecific relative to water (W) controls, regardless of whether they received an acute restraint stress (Restraint) or not (No Restraint) immediately prior to the social interaction test (left). *p < 0.05 versus water controls. This increased social interaction was similar to that which occurs after treatment with the known anxiolytic diazepam (D) versus vehicle (V) (right). *p < 0.05 versus vehicle. (B) The tachycardic response to acute restraint was reduced by limited sucrose drink relative to water controls. *p < 0.05 versus water controls. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

Figure 2. A history of limited sucrose intake reduces hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responses to stress and is largely mediated by the pleasurable and rewarding properties of the sucrose. (A) The plasma adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) response to a 20-min restraint stress is blunted by either limited sucrose (Suc; 30%) or saccharin (Sac; 0.1%; a non-caloric, artificial sweetener) drink intake, suggesting that the pleasurable or rewarding properties of these sweet drinks are sufficient for stress relief. *p < 0.05 versus Water (Wat). (B) Rats given twice-daily sucrose (S) drink have a reduced integrated (area-under-the-curve) plasma corticosterone response to restraint relative to water-only controls (left). In contrast, those receiving twice-daily orogastric gavage of 4 ml of sucrose versus water (to replicate many of the post-ingestive effects of the drink while minimizing taste and hedonic properties), do not have a reduced plasma corticosterone response (right), suggesting that sucrose’s hedonic/rewarding properties are necessary. *p < 0.05 versus Water. (C) Male rats with a history of daily, limited (30 min) access to a sexually responsive female engaged in sexual activity (Sex) and had a reduced integrated plasma corticosterone response to restraint compared to both undisturbed controls (C) and sham-sex rats (Sh; this group received a female rat in their cages, but the female was enclosed in a wire mesh box that prevented physical interactions), suggesting that other types of natural reward provide similar HPA-dampening. *p < 0.05 versus control and sham. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

Figure 2. A history of limited sucrose intake reduces hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responses to stress and is largely mediated by the pleasurable and rewarding properties of the sucrose. (A) The plasma adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) response to a 20-min restraint stress is blunted by either limited sucrose (Suc; 30%) or saccharin (Sac; 0.1%; a non-caloric, artificial sweetener) drink intake, suggesting that the pleasurable or rewarding properties of these sweet drinks are sufficient for stress relief. *p < 0.05 versus Water (Wat). (B) Rats given twice-daily sucrose (S) drink have a reduced integrated (area-under-the-curve) plasma corticosterone response to restraint relative to water-only controls (left). In contrast, those receiving twice-daily orogastric gavage of 4 ml of sucrose versus water (to replicate many of the post-ingestive effects of the drink while minimizing taste and hedonic properties), do not have a reduced plasma corticosterone response (right), suggesting that sucrose’s hedonic/rewarding properties are necessary. *p < 0.05 versus Water. (C) Male rats with a history of daily, limited (30 min) access to a sexually responsive female engaged in sexual activity (Sex) and had a reduced integrated plasma corticosterone response to restraint compared to both undisturbed controls (C) and sham-sex rats (Sh; this group received a female rat in their cages, but the female was enclosed in a wire mesh box that prevented physical interactions), suggesting that other types of natural reward provide similar HPA-dampening. *p < 0.05 versus control and sham. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

Role of reward in LSI-induced stress relief

Brain reward pathways are largely responsible for processing information related to the motivation, expectation and pursuit of pleasurable experiences. Palatable food intake is a naturally rewarding behavior, and it is possible that sucrose ingestion may constrain stress system activation, at least in part, via its actions on brain reward circuitry. We addressed this possibility by determining the extent to which the taste, hedonic and/or rewarding properties of sucrose contributes to its stress-relieving effects. The non-caloric sweetener saccharin is highly palatable to rats, and was used to replicate many of the hedonic/rewarding properties of sucrose while minimizing the post-ingestive consequences (e.g. macronutrients, calories, osmolarity) (Collier & Novell, Citation1967; Smith & Sclafani, Citation2002). Rats given limited saccharin (0.1%) drink intake showed HPA axis-dampening similar to the effects of sucrose ingestion (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010), suggesting that the hedonic/rewarding properties of sucrose may be sufficient for stress relief (). In contrast, twice-daily orogastric gavage of 4 ml of sucrose was used to replicate many of the post-ingestive effects of sucrose while minimizing its taste, hedonic and/or reward properties [though likely not completely preventing reward system activation (Sclafani, Citation2004)]. Rats given twice-daily limited sucrose gavage did not exhibit HPA axis-dampening (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010), suggesting that the post-ingestive effects of sucrose are not sufficient, and furthermore indicating that hedonic/rewarding properties are likely necessary (). Finally, if the naturally pleasurable and rewarding properties of sucrose are mediating stress relief, then another type of naturally rewarding behavior may replicate these effects. In order to test this possibility, male rats were given limited (up to 30 min) intermittent (once daily) access to a sexually receptive female for two weeks, followed by assessment of restraint-induced increases in plasma corticosterone (). Rats with a history of sexual activity had a reduced HPA response to restraint compared to both undisturbed home cage controls and also to sham-sex males (which were exposed to a female in their cage that was contained within a wire mesh box so the male could not physically access the female) (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). Collectively, this work indicates that in this limited access paradigm, the pleasurable and rewarding properties of palatable food, as well as other types of natural rewards, appear to play a major role in attenuating the response to a stressor.

A pivotal role for the basolateral amygdala in reward and stress processing

The basolateral amygdala (BLA) contributes to brain reward circuitry and is activated by palatable food intake, which increases neuronal firing and cFos expression (Fontanini et al., Citation2009; Muramoto et al., Citation1993; Verwey et al., Citation2007; Yamamoto et al., Citation1997). Moreover, the BLA is implicated in the control of behavioral and physiological stress responses (Bhatnagar et al., Citation2004; Coover et al., Citation1973; Feldman et al., Citation1983; Goldstein et al., Citation1996), and stress-induced c-fos mRNA responses in the BLA are reduced by a prior history of LSI (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2007). Taken together, this suggests that the BLA is well-positioned to mediate stress relief by LSI. In order to test this possibility, we assessed whether axon-sparing, ibotenate lesions of the BLA prevent HPA-dampening by sucrose ingestion () (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). As a positive control, rats given vehicle injection into the BLA, and hence with an intact BLA, showed typical HPA-dampening by LSI. In contrast, LSI did not result in HPA-dampening in rats with confirmed bilateral BLA lesions (using NeuN-immunolabeling to characterize the lesion extent), despite equivalent sucrose intake. Finally, rats with ibotenate lesions that missed the BLA showed typical HPA-dampening by LSI. As a whole, this work suggests that neuronal activity in the BLA is necessary for LSI-mediated stress relief.

Figure 3. Neuronal activity in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) is necessary for hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis-dampening by limited sucrose intake. The plasma corticosterone response to restraint stress was assessed after sucrose (S) versus water (W) intake in rats with bilateral vehicle-injected, intact BLA (Intact) or bilateral ibotenate infusion that produced lesions that either missed (Miss) or hit (Lesion) the BLA. (A) Representative images of neuron-specific nuclear protein (NeuN)-immunolabeling in the BLA of Intact and Lesion rats. CeA = central amygdala, MeA = medial amygdala, opt = optic tract. Scale bar = 500 um. (B) Sucrose reduced the integrated (area-under-the-curve) plasma corticosterone response to restraint stress (relative to water control rats) in Intact and Miss rats, while this HPA-dampening did not occur in rats with bilateral BLA lesion (Lesion). *p < 0.05 versus water controls, #p < 0.05 versus Intact-Sucrose and Miss-Sucrose. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

Figure 3. Neuronal activity in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) is necessary for hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis-dampening by limited sucrose intake. The plasma corticosterone response to restraint stress was assessed after sucrose (S) versus water (W) intake in rats with bilateral vehicle-injected, intact BLA (Intact) or bilateral ibotenate infusion that produced lesions that either missed (Miss) or hit (Lesion) the BLA. (A) Representative images of neuron-specific nuclear protein (NeuN)-immunolabeling in the BLA of Intact and Lesion rats. CeA = central amygdala, MeA = medial amygdala, opt = optic tract. Scale bar = 500 um. (B) Sucrose reduced the integrated (area-under-the-curve) plasma corticosterone response to restraint stress (relative to water control rats) in Intact and Miss rats, while this HPA-dampening did not occur in rats with bilateral BLA lesion (Lesion). *p < 0.05 versus water controls, #p < 0.05 versus Intact-Sucrose and Miss-Sucrose. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. Reproduced from (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010).

In order to identify potential intra-BLA mechanisms for stress relief by sucrose, we collected BLA tissue after LSI (versus water controls) and performed a microarray analysis of mRNA expression (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). This procedure identified 145 genes whose expression was altered by LSI. Functional clustering analyses indicated that these genes were significantly enriched in members of the intracellular calcium signaling and long-term potentiation (LTP) pathways, suggestive of synaptic plasticity (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). Subsequent neuroanatomical studies showed that LSI increased BLA immunolabeling for synaptophysin (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010), a marker of pre-synaptic terminals (Tominaga-Yoshino et al., Citation2008; Wiedenmann & Franke, Citation1985), indicating an increased synaptic density in the BLA. Furthermore, LSI increased the number of phospho-cAMP response element-binding protein (pCREB)-, phospho-calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II alpha (pCamKII)- and FosB/deltaFosB-positive cells in the BLA (Christiansen et al., Citation2011; Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010), all of which are post-synaptic indices that are linked with synaptic strengthening and plasticity (Huang et al., Citation2000; Lisman et al., Citation2002; Rodrigues et al., Citation2004; Silva et al., Citation1998). Taken together, these data indicate that LSI induces synaptic remodeling in the BLA. Consistent with this inference, both LSI-induced HPA-dampening and elevated BLA FosB/delta FosB levels persist for at least 21 days following cessation of LSI (Christiansen et al., Citation2011) – these long-lasting effects of sucrose are suggestive of enduring changes in synaptic remodeling. Collectively, this work indicates that LSI-mediated stress relief may result, at least in part, from sucrose ingestion-induced plasticity and remodeling in the BLA. Future work will be directed towards understanding the phenotype of this plasticity and the defining role that it plays in this process.

Summary

Evidence reviewed in this section indicates that a modest amount of sucrose intake can reduce collective (behavioral, HPA axis and cardiovascular/sympathetic) stress responses, in the absence of clear changes in body weight and adiposity. These stress effects are primarily mediated by the pleasurable and rewarding properties of the sucrose drink, and the BLA, a key reward- and stress-regulatory brain region, is necessary for sucrose-induced stress relief. Finally, the BLA undergoes synaptic remodeling following sucrose intake, which may enable stress relief to persist beyond the cessation of sucrose intake. Importantly, this work suggests that individuals may not need to consume large amounts of palatable food to achieve relief from acute stress. Moreover, the ability of other types of natural rewards to provide similar effects may be a useful strategy to diminish the contribution of life stress to obesity and other stress-related disorders.

Emotional consequences of prolonged high-fat feeding – Stephanie Fulton

Strong ties exist between the ingestion of foods rich in fat and/or sugar and the modulation of emotional states. As previously described, palatable food intake can relieve negative emotions by dampening signs of stress and anxiety following exposure to stressors or conditions of chronic unpredictable stress (Finger et al., Citation2011, Citation2012; la Fleur et al., Citation2005; Maniam & Morris, Citation2010; Pecoraro et al., Citation2004; Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010). Moreover, individuals experiencing depressed mood show increased preference and consumption of “comfort foods” and report the use of these foods as a means by which to alleviate adverse feelings (Macht, Citation2008). Such positive emotional reactions to tasty, energy-dense food, including their rewarding and hedonic actions, are considered to play a significant role in overeating and the development of obesity (Fulton, Citation2010). While intake of palatable foods can attenuate negative emotions and mood states in the short term, their excessive consumption leading to increased adipose tissue mass and obesity can heighten vulnerability to depression and anxiety (Hryhorczuk et al., Citation2013). Mood disorders are now well recognized as a significant risk factor for obesity and associated metabolic pathologies such as type 2 diabetes mellitus. Obesity elevates the odds of depression by about 55% (Luppino et al., Citation2010), whereas it is estimated that as many as 1 in 3 individuals with diabetes experience depressed mood at a level that diminishes overall functioning, disrupts glycemic control, and increases the risk of diabetic complications (Anderson et al., Citation2001). Indeed, apart from impairing quality of life and everyday functioning, depressed mood presents additional threats by counteracting adherence to treatment and lifestyle changes and increasing the risk of complications associated with metabolic diseases.

Diet-induced obesity elicits depressive-like behavior

Emerging findings are beginning to uncover the mechanisms that promote depressed mood in obesity, including the contribution of diet and abdominal adiposity. Using the diet-induced obesity (DIO) model, we demonstrated that adult C57Bl6 mice consuming a diet high in trans-fat (as 58% of kcal in diet) for 12 weeks develop depressive-like features characterized by greater immobility in the forced swim task and reduced time spent in open areas of the elevated-plus maze and open field tests (Sharma & Fulton, Citation2013) (). These behavioral indices of despair and anxiety were accompanied by elevated basal HPA activity and increased corticosterone secretion in response to stress. Analysis of the neuroadaptive changes in brain nuclei controlling motivation and emotional regulation in response to excess dietary fat is essential to understand the pathogenesis of diet-induced depression. The nucleus accumbens (NAc) is a limbic region that is strongly implicated in motivation, food-seeking and the hedonic deficits associated with depression. The NAc is highly interconnected with other brain areas important for mood regulation, including the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and amygdala (Russo & Nestler, Citation2013). The significance of the NAc in the etiology of depression is underscored by the success of deep brain stimulation targeting the NAc to improve mood ratings in depressed individuals (Bewernick et al., Citation2012). The well-characterized reduction in brain monoamine levels in depression is posited to be linked to neuroplastic adaptations that include transcriptional and translational changes generated by signals such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and CREB (Russo & Nestler, Citation2013). Depressive-like responses can be triggered by stress-induced increases in pCREB activation and BDNF expression in the NAc, whereas antidepressant-like actions are linked to their downregulation in this region (Martinowich et al., Citation2007; Nestler & Carlezon, Citation2006). Diet-induced depressive behavior was associated with decreased NAc protein expression of the rate-limiting enzyme for dopamine biosynthesis, tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), and by increases in ΔFosB, pCREB and BDNF (Sharma et al., Citation2013a). Increases in BDNF and pCREB protein levels in the NAc correlated positively with the magnitude of behavioral despair in the forced swim test, suggesting that prolonged high-fat feeding may promote depressive symptomatology via the neuroplastic changes elicited by these signaling molecules.

Figure 4. Diet-induced depressive-like behavior. (A) Percentage of time spent in open arms of the elevated plus maze (EPM) was significantly reduced in high fat diet (HFD)-fed mice as compared to control low fat diet (LFD)-fed mice. (B) Significant increase in time spent in center of open field in HFD mice compared to control LFD mice. (C) Effect of chronic consumption of HFD on depressive behavior expressed as immobility time in the forced swim test (FST) conducted in a separate cohort of mice. (D) Spontaneous locomotor activity expressed as total distance traveled during 15 min in a metabolism cage in C57Bl6 LFD and HFD mice (n = 6 each group). Mean ± SEM *p < 0.05. Figure from Sharma & Fulton (Citation2013).

Figure 4. Diet-induced depressive-like behavior. (A) Percentage of time spent in open arms of the elevated plus maze (EPM) was significantly reduced in high fat diet (HFD)-fed mice as compared to control low fat diet (LFD)-fed mice. (B) Significant increase in time spent in center of open field in HFD mice compared to control LFD mice. (C) Effect of chronic consumption of HFD on depressive behavior expressed as immobility time in the forced swim test (FST) conducted in a separate cohort of mice. (D) Spontaneous locomotor activity expressed as total distance traveled during 15 min in a metabolism cage in C57Bl6 LFD and HFD mice (n = 6 each group). Mean ± SEM *p < 0.05. Figure from Sharma & Fulton (Citation2013).

Diets high in saturated fat and relatively low in monounsaturated and polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA) may contribute to the pathogenesis of mood disorders during obesity. The consumption of foods rich in saturated and/or trans-fat are associated with an increased risk of depression, while a diet consisting of mostly unsaturated fats, such as the Mediterranean diet, is largely protective. Interestingly, elevated dietary consumption of trans-fatty acids, such as that implemented in studies described above, has been shown to reduce brain docosahexaenoic acid, an omega-3 PUFA with described antidepressant actions (Phivilay et al., Citation2009). Indeed, insufficient dietary PUFA augments depression risk (Peet et al., Citation1998), whereas increasing dietary omega-3 PUFA has been shown to be protective (Lin & Su, Citation2007; Moranis et al., Citation2012; Oddy et al., Citation2011) and to prevent inflammation-induced depression (Su et al., Citation2014). Diets rich in saturated fatty acids are associated with increases in overall adiposity and bias visceral fat accumulation (Rosqvist et al., Citation2014). Adipose accumulation in abdominal stores is more effective than total body fat in predicting the risk of several complications of obesity, including insulin resistance and the metabolic syndrome (Tchernof & Despres, Citation2013). In a similar manner, central adipose depot mass is a better predictor of developing depression than body weight or body mass index (BMI) (van Reedt Dortland et al., Citation2013a). Consistent with these findings, Sharma and colleagues recently demonstrated that male adult mice consuming a saturated high-fat diet (50% kcal palm oil) for eight weeks develop greater visceral obesity and metabolic impairments, including elevated levels of plasma corticosterone and cytokines, along with increased anxiety and depressive-like behaviors, as compared to mice consuming an isocaloric monounsaturated fat diet (Sharma et al., Citation2013a). These effects could be related to the actions of saturated fats to stimulate pro-inflammatory signaling. Obesity is characterized by a state of prolonged low-grade inflammation, and several gene markers of neuroinflammation are upregulated in the brain of rodents by chronic high-fat feeding (De Souza et al., Citation2005; Thaler et al., Citation2012). Correspondingly, several lines of evidence implicate immune responses in the pathophysiology of depression (Miller et al., Citation2009). In agreement with a role for inflammation, a recent study demonstrated that elevated circulating levels of the pro-inflammatory signal C-reactive protein accounts for most of the association between depression severity with lipid and obesity levels (van Reedt Dortland et al., Citation2013b). Collectively, these data suggest that abdominal obesity and associated circulating inflammatory signals contribute to the etiology of depression; however, the neural mechanisms underlying this functional link remain unknown.

Stress, anxiety and craving responses to high-fat diet removal

Is the onset of depressive-like behavior that is induced by prolonged high-fat feeding secondary to metabolic impairments produced by obesity? To begin addressing this question, we examined whether high-fat feeding could provoke behavioral depressive-like symptomatology and neurobiological changes even before the onset of severe obesity and associated metabolic dysfunction (Sharma & Fulton, Citation2013). HFD consumption for 6 weeks resulted in moderate weight gain (∼11% increase versus controls), enhanced stress-induced HPA activation and sucrose anhedonia (reduced ability to experience pleasure/reward from sucrose drink), but did not increase anxiety-like behavior. Similarly, others have shown that high-fat feeding independent of obesity (using a pair-feeding paradigm) can dampen mesolimbic dopamine turnover and reward responses (Davis et al., Citation2008). However, returning high-fat fed mice to a normal chow diet (i.e. akin to “dieting”) increased anxiety-like behavior () and physiological stress responses (), and also increased motivation to access both sucrose and fatty food () (Sharma et al., Citation2013b). Similarly, several groups have shown that removal of a high-fat/sugar diet following prolonged intermittent or regular intake can increase behavioral and physiological signs of depression and anxiety akin to withdrawal symptoms (Avena et al., Citation2008; Cottone et al., Citation2009a,Citationb; Teegarden et al., Citation2008). Thus, not only can chronic high-fat feeding provoke signs of anhedonia and attenuate mesolimbic dopamine function prior to development of severe metabolic dysfunction, it appears to augment sensitivity to stress, including stress triggered by dieting, which may further perpetuate overeating, weight gain and depressed mood.

Figure 5. High-fat diet (HFD) withdrawal (dieting) increases anxiety, stress and palatable food craving. (A) Elevated plus maze (EPM) open arm time was significantly reduced in mice withdrawn from HFD as compared to mice withdrawn from low fat diet (LFD). (B) Elevated basal plasma corticosterone (CORT) concentration in HFD withdrawn mice as compared to LFD withdrawn mice. (C) Body weights decreased post-withdrawal in mice subjected to HFD withdrawal (WD: day of withdrawal). (D) Withdrawal from HFD, but not LFD, significantly increased breakpoint thresholds for sucrose rewards (indicating increased motivation for sucrose reward) as of day 3 of withdrawal (WD D3). Breakpoints remained elevated on day 4 and 5 of withdrawal when sucrose rewards were replaced by high-fat food rewards. Mean ± SEM; *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.001. Figure from Sharma et al. (Citation2013b).

Figure 5. High-fat diet (HFD) withdrawal (dieting) increases anxiety, stress and palatable food craving. (A) Elevated plus maze (EPM) open arm time was significantly reduced in mice withdrawn from HFD as compared to mice withdrawn from low fat diet (LFD). (B) Elevated basal plasma corticosterone (CORT) concentration in HFD withdrawn mice as compared to LFD withdrawn mice. (C) Body weights decreased post-withdrawal in mice subjected to HFD withdrawal (WD: day of withdrawal). (D) Withdrawal from HFD, but not LFD, significantly increased breakpoint thresholds for sucrose rewards (indicating increased motivation for sucrose reward) as of day 3 of withdrawal (WD D3). Breakpoints remained elevated on day 4 and 5 of withdrawal when sucrose rewards were replaced by high-fat food rewards. Mean ± SEM; *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.001. Figure from Sharma et al. (Citation2013b).

In order to isolate potential neurobiological mechanisms contributing to increased anxiety and palatable food craving following withdrawal from high-fat feeding, we analyzed protein expression of several neuroplasticity markers in the NAc and amygdala. The 6-week period of high-fat feeding (not associated with anxiety or HPA stimulation) increased pCREB in the BLA and central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), similar to findings reported for the BLA following the LSI paradigm (Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2010) (see previous section), and elevated ΔFosB in the NAc as previously documented (Sharma & Fulton, Citation2013; Teegarden et al., Citation2008). However, several days after removal of the HFD, heightened anxiety and stress responses and elevated motivation for palatable food were associated with lower pCREB and NAc ΔFosB levels in the amygdala. At the same time, NAc BDNF protein levels were increased, similar to findings in the diet-induced depression model described above. Thus, even prior to the development of severe obesity and metabolic dysfunction, chronic high-fat feeding provokes signs of stress, anxiety and food craving in response to dieting in a manner inversely related to neuroplastic adaptations that are associated with stress relief. These neurobiological and behavioral responses to dieting may reinstate palatable food-seeking and thereby foster overeating, weight gain and the development of depression that accompanies diet-induced obesity.

Summary

It is increasingly evident that the amount and quality of the nutrients consumed over the long run can impact the neural circuits controlling emotions, motivation and mood. Findings on several fronts tie the development of depressed mood in obese individuals to poor dietary lifestyle, central adipose mass and associated metabolic disturbances. Studies in mice indicate that weight gain and to a greater extent obesity produced by excessive intake of dietary fats can generate depressive-like symptomatology and several neuroplastic adaptations in brain reward circuitry. Chronic high-fat feeding may affect neural processes of emotion and reward via actions on energy metabolism, endocrine function and immunity, or perhaps through the direct actions of free fatty acids on the central nervous system. Recent research findings implicate the nature of the dietary fat as an important element, with saturated fats being more deleterious than unsaturated fats. Trans- and saturated fats favor central fat deposition and are linked with cardiometabolic and neurological diseases (Barnard et al., Citation2014; Kanoski & Davidson, Citation2011; Micha & Mozaffarian, Citation2010). Our knowledge of the mechanisms contributing to diet-induced depression can be advanced by building upon our understanding of the metabolic disturbances associated with abdominal obesity, and how they influence central signaling mechanisms that regulate emotion. It also is imperative to understand the neurobehavioral processes by which sensitivity to stressors, including dieting, is enhanced by prolonged high-fat feeding. Improved understanding of these mechanisms may suggest novel ways to avert the self-defeating cycle of depressed mood promoting the intake of comfort foods as a means to achieve stress relief.

Research by Dr Ulrich-Lai and Dr Fulton also indicates that palatable food intake can have opposing effects on physiological and behavioral responses to stress. Stress responses are decreased in chow-fed rats with a history of intermittent intake of small amounts of sweetened drink (LSI), and this effect appears to be mediated primarily by the rewarding (versus macronutrient/energetic) properties of the food (see previous section). In contrast, unlimited and prolonged access to high dietary fat in mice increases stress responses in a manner that is modulated by the type of fat. Several key differences between these two palatable food paradigms likely influence the valence of stress effects. These factors include the amount and type of palatable food offered (small amounts of sugar versus large amounts of fat), the duration of exposure (2 weeks for LSI versus 12 weeks for HFD), whether the paradigm altered either body weight or adiposity (no effects for LSI versus increase in body weight with HFD), whether the food was offered as a choice (choice with LSI versus no choice with HFD), whether food intake was limited (intake limited with LSI versus unlimited with HFD), as well as the species tested (rat for LSI versus mouse for HFD). Indeed, some evidence suggests that stress relief is associated with increased carbohydrate intake and also with the presence of food choice, whereas exacerbated stress is associated with reduced dietary carbohydrate, lack of dietary choice and increased adiposity (Anderson et al., Citation1987; Barr et al., Citation1999; de Castro, Citation1987; Deuster et al., Citation1992; Markus et al., Citation2000; Utter et al., Citation1999). We refer readers to the “Concluding remarks” section for additional discussion of this intriguing topic.

Stress-induced eating – Mark Wilson

Diet-induced obesity (DIO) resulting from the persistent consumption of a calorically dense diet is a well-accepted strategy to study the consequences of obesity in animal models (Hariri & Thibault, Citation2010; Panchal & Brown, Citation2011). However, some animals show resistance to developing this phenotype (Archer & Mercer, Citation2007). While a number of genetic factors could account for this variance in susceptibility to DIO (Levin, Citation2000; Yazbek et al., Citation2010), accumulating data indicate a history of adverse experience resulting from chronic stressor exposure may also be important (Dallman et al., Citation2007; Tamashiro et al., Citation2011). It must be noted, however, that the effects of stressor exposure on food intake are complex and may be bidirectional, with the specific effect dependent on a number of factors such as whether the experience is severe (see later section by Dr Petrovich), and/or whether it is acute or chronic (Maniam & Morris, Citation2012). Furthermore, macronutrient content of the diet is also likely important for the directionality of effect, as a large literature from rodent models suggests that stressor exposure to animals fed a typical low calorie, laboratory chow diet produces a state of inappetence (Harris et al., Citation1998; Tamashiro et al., Citation2006), an effect reversed by the central administration of a CRF receptor antagonist (Smagin et al., Citation1999). Indeed, acute central administration of CRF to adult macaques has immediate and lasting suppressive effects on food intake (Glowa & Gold, Citation1991). However, an extensive literature in rodent models indicates that chronic exposure to stressors is a mitigating factor in the preference for and excess consumption of high-caloric diets containing fats and sugars (Tamashiro et al., Citation2006; Warne, Citation2009). These data underscore the importance of considering diet composition when assessing the consequences of stressor exposure on appetite.

Increased stress-induced intake of calorically dense diets in rodents is consistent with the notion of comfort food ingestion in people (Adam & Epel, Citation2007; Epel et al., Citation2000) (see previous section by Dr Ulrich-Lai). With the exception of a few reports (Solomon et al., Citation2011), the majority of studies in rodent models have used males (Tamashiro et al., Citation2005; Warne, Citation2009), despite the growing evidence highlighting significant bias towards stress-induced eating and obesity in women (Grunberg & Straub, Citation1992; Kitraki et al., Citation2004; Soulis et al., Citation2005, Citation2007). Socially housed female macaques provide an opportunity to further define mechanisms for stress-induced emotional feeding in women. A large body of data shows that social subordination in established macaque groups produces a number of stress-related phenotypes in females (Michopoulos et al., Citation2012a), including a dysregulation of the HPA axis (Kaplan et al., Citation2010; Michopoulos et al., Citation2012a; Shively et al., Citation1997a) and an upregulation of a number of proinflammatory genes (Tung et al., Citation2012). In a dietary environment where a typical low fat, high fiber laboratory chow diet is available (“low caloric diet” or LCD), subordinate female rhesus monkeys consistently have lower body weights and lower serum concentrations of leptin than more dominant group mates (Michopoulos & Wilson, Citation2011). By employing automated feeding stations that detect food intake by free-feeding individuals in these social groups (Wilson et al., Citation2008), the reduced body weight in subordinate females was shown to be associated either with periodic mild inappetence (Michopoulos et al., Citation2009) or with calorie intake that was similar to intake by dominant females (Michopoulos et al., Citation2012a). What is not known is whether there is a social status-related difference in energy expenditure in these socially housed monkeys.

A different pattern emerges when animals are maintained in a rich dietary environment and given the choice between the LCD and a high fat, high sugar diet. This dietary choice environment, where animals have access to both the LCD and high caloric diet (HCD), which better models the human dietary environment, sustains stress-induced consumption of HCDs (Warne, Citation2009) and promotes obesity more effectively than HCD alone (Rolls et al., Citation1983). Under these conditions, all animals prefer the HCD (Arce et al., Citation2010; Michopoulos et al., Citation2012c). However, dominant females consume a similar number of calories in this rich dietary environment as they do when only the LCD is available (Arce et al., Citation2010; Michopoulos et al., Citation2012c). In contrast, subordinates consume significantly more calories in this dietary choice environment compared with dominant females () (Arce et al., Citation2010; Michopoulos et al., Citation2012c). This status difference in caloric intake is the result of increased meal size by subordinates compared with dominant females, and is not due to more frequent meals or calories consumed outside of defined meals (snacks) () (Moore et al., Citation2013). Interestingly, these statistically defined meals were significantly more often comprised of calories from both diets rather than either diet alone (Moore et al., Citation2013). While these short-term studies were focused on changes in appetite rather than the long-term consequences on body composition, the data nonetheless suggest that female rhesus monkeys provide a useful model to better understand how psychosocial stressor exposure may account for variance in DIO in females.

Figure 6. Average (±SEM) daily caloric intake for dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys from both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD). Redrawn from Arce et al. (Citation2010) and Michopoulos et al. (Citation2012c).

Figure 6. Average (±SEM) daily caloric intake for dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys from both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD). Redrawn from Arce et al. (Citation2010) and Michopoulos et al. (Citation2012c).

Figure 7. Comparison of average (± SEM) meal size (kcal), meal number and calories from snacks for dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys maintained in a dietary environment where both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD) are available. Redrawn from Moore et al. (Citation2013).

Figure 7. Comparison of average (± SEM) meal size (kcal), meal number and calories from snacks for dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys maintained in a dietary environment where both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD) are available. Redrawn from Moore et al. (Citation2013).

Possible mechanisms for stress-induced emotional feeding

It is still unclear how chronic stressor exposure increases susceptibility to DIO in female monkeys. One possibility is that subordinate females may be more sensitive to orexigenic signals. Although acute ghrelin administration increases intake of a LCD in subordinate but not dominant females (Michopoulos et al., Citation2010), it has no effect on consumption of a HCD. In contrast, a number of studies in rats have confirmed that glucocorticoids dose-dependently increase the intake of diets high in fat and sugar (Dallman et al., Citation2005; Warne, Citation2009), supporting the well-established notion that these steroids promote food intake (Sominsky & Spencer, Citation2014). Although prospective studies assessing the direct effects of stress-like glucocorticoid levels on diet preference and caloric intake have not been performed in monkeys, serum cortisol concentrations following a dexamethasone suppression test positively predict consumption of a HCD but not a LCD in female rhesus monkeys (Michopoulos et al., Citation2012c). These data suggest that female monkeys with reduced glucocorticoid negative feedback will consume more calories from a palatable, calorically denser diet.

Importantly, the consumption of palatable HCDs in male rats subsequently reduces their response to acute stressors (Foster et al., Citation2009; la Fleur et al., Citation2005; Pecoraro et al., Citation2004; Ulrich-Lai et al., Citation2011). However, a different pattern emerges in female monkeys. Both dominant and subordinate females respond similarly to an acute separation from their social group with a robust increase in serum cortisol concentration. However, this response is significantly greater when monkeys are maintained in a rich dietary environment, with access to both a HCD and LCD, compared to a condition when only the LCD is available (Arce et al., Citation2010; Michopoulos et al., Citation2012b). These data are consistent with results showing that intake of calorically dense foods increases HPA axis activity in rodents (Kamara et al., Citation1998; Tannenbaum et al., Citation1997) and in humans (Pasquali et al., Citation2002). Furthermore, data indicating that glucocorticoids can alter food salience and increase caloric intake (Dallman et al., Citation2005; Warne, Citation2009) are consistent with the idea that emotional feeding is sustained by activation of the HPA axis, and that acute stress superimposed on chronic stress may promote continued intake of HCDs. While these observations in female monkeys are similar to those reported by Dr Fulton in the previous section, i.e. that HFD consumption enhances stress responsivity, they contrast with those summarized above by Dr Ulrich-Lai, which clearly show that comfort food intake diminishes the acute response to a stressor in male rats. It is unclear what accounts for these differences in HPA responsivity in animals maintained on palatable diet, but important factors may include the nature of the stressor and/or the history of comfort food intake. It is tempting to suggest that these differences may reflect sex differences in HPA axis regulation (Handa et al., Citation1994; Toufexis & Wilson, Citation2012; Viau et al., Citation2005), and in how HPA axis responses are modified by dietary environment. However, results from the different experimental paradigms using rats and mice described above by Drs. Ulrich-Lai and Fulton were obtained only from males.

Additional observations from rodents indicate that chronic activation of central CRF-R1 receptors are likely important for stress-induced overconsumption of palatable diets. In these rodent models, chronic diet cycling (intermittent dietary restriction of a palatable diet) functions as a mild stressor, and immunohistochemistry reveals elevated CRF-positive cells in the CeA among diet cycled versus control rats (Cottone et al., Citation2009a; Iemolo et al., Citation2013). Importantly, microinfusion of a CRF-R1 antagonist, Antalarmin, into the CeA of diet cycled male rats fully blocks hyperphagia of a palatable food, with no effect on standard chow intake (Iemolo et al., Citation2013). Our data in female rhesus monkeys are consistent with these findings: during placebo administration, subordinate females consume significantly more calories than non-subordinate females within a dietary environment that includes a choice between a LCD and HCD. However, acute peripheral administration of Antalarmin significantly reduces average daily caloric intake in subordinate female monkeys to amounts comparable to those consumed by dominant group mates () (Moore et al., Citation2015). As Antalarmin penetrates the blood brain barrier and this dose and route of administration does not affect peripheral cortisol secretion (Herod et al., Citation2011a,Citationb), the data suggest that activation of central CRF-R1 may be important for sustaining this emotional feeding phenotype across species.

Figure 8. Average (± SEM) daily caloric intake of both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD) in dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys during two days of placebo and two days of treatment with the corticotropin releasing factore receptor type 1 (CRF-R1) antagonist, Antalarmin. Subordinate but not dominant females showed a significant attenuation in caloric intake during Antalarmin treatment. Reprinted from Moore et al. (Citation2015).

Figure 8. Average (± SEM) daily caloric intake of both a low caloric (LCD) and high caloric diet (HCD) in dominant and subordinate female rhesus monkeys during two days of placebo and two days of treatment with the corticotropin releasing factore receptor type 1 (CRF-R1) antagonist, Antalarmin. Subordinate but not dominant females showed a significant attenuation in caloric intake during Antalarmin treatment. Reprinted from Moore et al. (Citation2015).

An emerging hypothesis is that excess food consumption and resulting obesity is a form of addiction involving the same corticostriatal circuitry engaged by drugs of abuse (Tomasi & Volkow, Citation2013). Assuming this hypothesis is viable, the question remains as to what accounts for differential vulnerability to the addictive properties of food. Because chronic stressor exposure, possibly mediated through central CRF signaling, is a risk factor for psychostimulant abuse in people (Sinha & Jastreboff, Citation2013) and for drug self-administration in experimental animals (Goeders, Citation2002; Koob & Kreek, Citation2007), including monkeys (Morgan et al., Citation2002), it is possible that exposure to chronic stressors increases vulnerability to food addiction (Dallman et al., Citation2005). As highlighted in previous sections by Dr Ulrich-Lai and Dr Fulton, evidence from rodents and humans suggests that changes in dopamine activity are at least part of the mechanism linking stress to comfort food ingestion (Bassareo & Di Chiara, Citation1999; Martel & Fantino, Citation1996a,Citationb; Pelchat, Citation2002; Rada et al., Citation2005). Based on studies in rodents, it is well established that signals from the stress axis, including glucocorticoids and CRF, target dopamine neurons in mesolimbic regions (Ambroggi et al., Citation2009; Burke & Miczek, Citation2013; Koob et al., Citation2014). This apparently produces a dysregulation of dopamine neurotransmission (Izzo et al., Citation2005) that increases the expression of anhedonia and the risk for developing an addictive phenotype (Anisman & Matheson, Citation2005; Koob & Kreek, Citation2007; Koob & Le Moal, Citation2001). This stress-induced dopamine dysfunction has been referred to as a “reward deficiency syndrome”, characterized by reduced dopamine activity, notably including reduced availability of dopamine type 2 receptors (D2R) (Blum et al., Citation1996) that is both predictive of an addictive phenotype (Volkow & Wise, Citation2005; Volkow et al., Citation2003) and observed in human obesity (Wang et al., Citation2001). Whether the imposition of chronic stress in the monkey model of social subordination leads to such a reward deficiency syndrome that predicts stress-induced consumption of calorically dense diets remains to be determined. We do know that social subordination in female macaques produces widespread reduction in D2R binding potential throughout the dopamine-rich reward network (Grant et al., Citation1998; Shively et al., Citation1997a), including the striatum and prefrontal cortex, which is critical for regulating reward- and goal-directed behaviors and responses (Haber & Knutson, Citation2010). Studies of obese humans indicate the presence of disrupted connectivity within cortico-striatal pathways (Tomasi & Volkow, Citation2013). Preliminary data suggest that social subordination in female macaques reduces functional connectivity within the cingulo-opercular/limbic network, which includes the NAc, amygdala and prefrontal cortex, and has been linked to stimulus salience (Godfrey et al., Citation2013). As stressor exposure affects structural and functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and other regions regulating reward (Govindan et al., Citation2010; Kaufman & Charney, Citation2001; Sanchez et al., Citation1998; Teicher et al., Citation2002), it is possible that exposure to chronic stress compromises dopamine modulation of prefrontal cortical regulation of the striatum, thereby increasing vulnerability for food addiction.

Summary

Identifying the mechanisms by which chronic stress increases the preference for and consumption of palatable, calorically dense foods may provide insights into possible treatment strategies for overeating and obesity. While results from studies in female macaques show many similarities to results from studies of male rodents, further investigation is needed to determine if sex differences in stress responsivity lead to differences in vulnerabilities to food addiction, as they do for psychostimulant addiction (Bobzean et al., Citation2014). Given that a history of stressor exposure can have lasting effects on biological signaling and behavior (Zorrilla et al., Citation2014), an important question is whether the alleviation of chronic stress – by experimentally manipulating social rank so that formerly subordinate females are now dominant (Shively et al., Citation1997b) – reverses the preference for, and consumption of, palatable, calorically dense diets. Such a finding would be critically important for people attempting to make life style changes to improve their health. The use of female macaque monkeys provides a translational animal model for women that complements the use of rodent models to understand the causal relationship between chronic stress and risk for obesity.

Stress-induced anorexia – Gorica Petrovich

Stress has profound and multifaceted effects on food intake and body weight, which often are stimulatory (see previous section) (Meye & Adan, Citation2014; Tamashiro et al., Citation2011). Fear of anticipated stress, however, inhibits food intake (Cannon, Citation1915; Schachter et al., Citation1968), and sustained stress, fear and anxiety have been linked to prolonged cessation of eating, including anorexia nervosa (Hotta et al., Citation1999; Kaye, Citation2008; Klein & Walsh, Citation2004; Vallés et al., Citation2000). Anticipation of aversive events also suppresses food approach and consummatory and instrumental actions towards food (Petrovich, Citation2013). This suppression of feeding is adaptive and promotes survival when fear is triggered by real danger (i.e. avoiding imminent danger takes priority over feeding), but could become maladaptive when sustained fear interferes with feeding despite an energy-depleted state, such as in anorexia nervosa. The neural mechanisms controlling the balance between hunger and fear are therefore important in normal function and in disease. Nevertheless, the brain substrates mediating anorexigenic effects of stress anticipation (i.e. fear) on food consumption have been largely unexplored. Here, a behavioral model for short-term fear anorexiaFootnote1 will be described, along with functional and anatomical evidence supporting a critical role for a prefrontal–amygdala–hypothalamic circuitry.

Fear anorexia

The fear anorexia model utilizes conditioned fear cues to inhibit feeding in hungry rats. A fear cue in these preparations is a signal that predicts danger, which is a tone and/or an environment (context) previously associated with mild, electric foot-shocks through Pavlovian conditioning (Fendt & Fanselow, Citation1999; LeDoux, Citation2000; Maren, Citation2001). After conditioning, discrete (tone) or contextual fear cues can potently inhibit feeding in hungry rats (Petrovich & Lougee, Citation2011; Petrovich et al., Citation2009; Reppucci et al., Citation2013).

During the feeding tests, conditioned fear cues also induce freezing behavior, a species-typical defense response for rodents, characterized by complete immobility, except as necessitated for breathing (Blanchard & Blanchard, Citation1969; Fanselow, Citation1984). Importantly, cessation of feeding in the conditioned fear anorexia model is not simply a product of freezing behavior. Rats do not halt eating simply because they are immobile; rather, anorexia and freezing are two separate behavioral outputs driven by the same fear cue (Petrovich and Lougee, Citation2011; Petrovich et al., Citation2009; Reppucci et al., Citation2013). Furthermore, fear anorexia and freezing are supported by dissociable amygdalar substrates. The CeA, but not the BLA, is critical for fear cue anorexia, while both are necessary for fear cue-driven freezing behavior (Petrovich et al., Citation2009).

This animal model may provide insights into aspects of eating dysregulation in human, particularly the potential contribution of fear in the maintenance of low food intake in anorexia nervosa (AN) (Petrovich & Lougee, Citation2011). AN is a complex eating disorder characterized by relentless maintenance of extremely low body weight through restricted eating, sometimes combined with excessive physical activity and purging (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013; Kaye, Citation2008; Klein & Walsh, Citation2004; Treasure et al., Citation2010). Restricted food intake is maintained persistently, despite emaciation and despite serious health consequences that can be fatal. Hypophagia under these circumstances is paradoxical and occurs despite physiological hunger signals, including – high ghrelin and low leptin baseline circulating concentrations (Jimerson & Wolfe, Citation2006; Kaye, Citation2008) and increased central amounts of the potent orexigenic neuropeptide, neuropeptide Y (Kaye, Citation2008).

An intense and persistent fear of weight gain (including fear of food and eating), despite being underweight, is a key symptom and a diagnostic criterion of AN (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). Anxiety, typically associated with sustained fear, is also highly prevalent in AN. Furthermore, individuals with AN have aberrant processing within the key brain fear network regions (the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex) (Giordano et al., Citation2001; Seeger et al., Citation2002; Takano et al., Citation2001; Uher et al., Citation2004). Therefore, increased underlying fear, and associated over-activity within the fear network in AN, could be a critical facilitator of eating inhibition. Nevertheless, the effects of fear on food consumption under conditions of chronic food restriction and low body weight remain to be established.

Sex differences in fear anorexia

A recent study compared learning and feeding behavior of adult male and female rats in the fear anorexia paradigm to determine whether there were sex differences () (Petrovich & Lougee, Citation2011). Males and females acquired fear cues similarly well, as evidenced by similar freezing behavior during tests (). The extent of fear anorexia also was similar between the two sexes during the first of four tests. However, a pronounced sex difference, emerged during extinction: females showed sustained inhibition of feeding compared to males. Male rats extinguished this fear anorexia during the second test, when rats in the conditioned and control groups ate similar, large amounts of food. It took females in the conditioned group two additional tests to reach the consumption levels of the controls () (Petrovich & Lougee, Citation2011). These findings suggest a biological susceptibility of the female sex that is potentially relevant to AN, which afflicts women disproportionately more than men (see discussion on fear and AN, above). This rodent fear anorexia model provides a novel framework for investigating sex differences in the control of feeding and the underlying brain substrates.

Figure 9. (A) Food consumption (mean ± SEM) of male and female rats during tests with fear cue presentations. During training, rats in the conditioned groups received tones paired with footshocks, while rats in the control groups received tones but no shocks. Tests were conducted on separate days, under acute food deprivation (no footshocks given during tests). Significant differences (p < 0.05) between Conditioned and Control groups are indicated by * for females and by the # for males. (B) Conditioned freezing behavior during food consumption tests. Graphs show percentages (mean ± SEM) of the total time spent freezing during the tests. Adapted from Petrovich & Lougee (Citation2011), with permission.

Figure 9. (A) Food consumption (mean ± SEM) of male and female rats during tests with fear cue presentations. During training, rats in the conditioned groups received tones paired with footshocks, while rats in the control groups received tones but no shocks. Tests were conducted on separate days, under acute food deprivation (no footshocks given during tests). Significant differences (p < 0.05) between Conditioned and Control groups are indicated by * for females and by the # for males. (B) Conditioned freezing behavior during food consumption tests. Graphs show percentages (mean ± SEM) of the total time spent freezing during the tests. Adapted from Petrovich & Lougee (Citation2011), with permission.

Prefrontal–amygdala–hypothalamic circuitry

Extensive work over the last two decades has focused on determining the brain substrates underlying fear and its behavioral sequelae (LeDoux, Citation2012; Seymour & Dolan, Citation2008), and similar efforts have been directed towards examining the central substrates of hunger, food intake and energy balance (Berthoud, Citation2007; Morton et al., Citation2006). Knowing when to eat and when to halt feeding and attend to danger is essential for survival, and dysfunction of this system could lead to disease. How fear and hunger compete, however, has not been adequately addressed, and the underlying brain substrates are not well understood.

Recent work has begun to identify components of a fear anorexia neural network. One of the key structures in fear acquisition and behavioral expression, the CeA, also was found to be critical for fear anorexia (Petrovich et al., Citation2009). Neurotoxic, bilateral lesions of the CeA eliminated decreased consumption during tests with fear cues, and Fos induction in the CeA during similar tests corresponded with the amounts of food consumed (Petrovich et al., Citation2009; Reppucci & Petrovich, Citation2014). The circuitry through which the CeA acts to inhibit food intake has not been delineated, but connectional and functional evidence strongly implicates the lateral hypothalamus (LHA). The LHA has historically been associated with the initiation of feeding (Elmquist et al., Citation1999), and recently was conceptualized as a critical integrator of physiological and environmental feeding signals (Berthoud & Münzberg, Citation2011; Petrovich, Citation2013). Indeed, the LHA is crucial for cue-induced feeding, which requires integration between physiological and environmental feeding signals (Petrovich, Citation2013). Cue-induced feeding is driven by food cues and occurs independently of hunger, and the LHA is an integral part of its critical forebrain network, together with the BLA and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Petrovich, Citation2013). The BLA also is necessary for stress relief by palatable food reward, and is a site of plasticity in the LSI paradigm, described by Dr Ulrich-Lai in a previous section. Fear anorexia, similar to cue-induced feeding, requires competition with physiological hunger/satiety signals and the amygdala; however, the amygdalar component for fear anorexia is the CeA, instead of the BLA.

The CeA is anatomically well positioned to influence LHA processing, via direct and indirect pathways (Petrovich et al., Citation2001; Yoshida et al., Citation2006). Its targets within the LHA include the suprafornical region, which is important for ingestive behavior (Hahn & Swanson, Citation2010), and which contains neurons that express an orexigenic peptide, orexin/hypocretin (de Lecea et al., Citation1998; Nakamura et al., Citation2009; Sakurai et al., Citation1998), which is important in arousal, reward processing and anticipatory feeding motivation (Petrovich et al., Citation2012). In addition, the CeA sends direct neural projections to brainstem targets that also receive LHA input and are responsible for feeding suppression, the parabrachial nucleus and the nucleus of the solitary tract (Carter et al., Citation2013; Hahn & Swanson, Citation2010; Maniscalco et al., Citation2013; Swanson & Petrovich, Citation1998).

Another major telencephalic input to the LHA arises from the vmPFC (Petrovich, Citation2013), an area critical for cognitive processing and executive function, including decision making in goal-directed behaviors (O'Doherty, Citation2011). The vmPFC and its connections to the LHA support cue-induced feeding (Petrovich, Citation2013), and are set up to control other forms of feeding. Stimulation of the vmPFC µ-opioid (Mena et al., Citation2011) and dopamine receptor (Land et al., Citation2014) systems elicit robust feeding in sated animals, and these effects depend on the LHA (Mena et al., Citation2013) and amygdala (Land et al., Citation2014), respectively. In turn, the vmPFC has access to LHA and CeA processing via direct, topographically organized connections (Hahn & Swanson, Citation2010; Reppucci & Petrovich, Citation2012; Swanson & Petrovich, Citation1998). In particular, the infralimbic cortical area sends direct projections to the CeA, as well as to its targets within the LHA and brainstem, including the parabrachial nucleus and the nucleus of the solitary tract (Hahn & Swanson, Citation2010). This connectional pattern might allow the vmPFC to simultaneously control the CeA and its outputs, which would be an efficient way to regulate feeding under imminent stress.

Summary

Dissociable amygdala substrates participate in stimulatory and inhibitory control of food intake. The BLA, but not CeA, is critical for food cue-driven feeding (Petrovich, Citation2013) and is recruited during initial cue-food learning (Cole et al., Citation2013), while the CeA, but not BLA, is critical for fear cue inhibition of feeding (Petrovich et al., Citation2009). Both amygdalar subregions are connected with the vmPFC and LHA. The vmPFC-BLA-LHA system mediates food cue stimulatory effects on feeding, while a parallel system formed by the CeA, vmPFC and LHA is well positioned to support fear cue inhibitory effects on feeding. Future work should improve our understanding of how these two systems interact in the control of food intake, and reveal the specific neurochemical mediators recruited to control a “fear-or-feeding” switch.

Concluding remarks

This research symposium, and each of the contributing authors' research programs, shines a spotlight on the complex physiological and behavioral interactions that link stress, food intake and emotion. Each researcher has his or her own distinct perspective, working hypotheses, experimental models and scientific schema that drive their interest in these issues, but their cumulative efforts promote an increased appreciation and understanding of the myriad biological problems to be addressed.

Effects of palatable food intake on stress responses

As summarized in , Dr Ulrich-Lai has applied a rat model incorporating stable energy balance, access to limited amounts of palatable (sucrose) diet and dietary choice to demonstrate improved stress coping via “comfort food” exposure. Conversely, Dr Fulton's research uses mice that have become overweight and obese after unlimited, long-term access to a diet high in fat/sugar, without dietary choice, which promotes increased physiological and behavioral stress responsiveness. Collectively, these and other research findings indicate that intake of highly palatable foods can have opposing effects on stress-regulatory systems, with effect direction dependent upon several factors. First, consistent with the work of Dr Susanne la Fleur’s group (la Fleur et al., Citation2005), dietary choice may promote reduced stress responses, whereas lack of dietary choice may promote increased stress responses. However, while Dr Wilson’s approach provides a dietary choice to female rhesus monkeys, his results suggest a facilitation of the stress response under these dietary conditions. Second, the stress-regulatory effects of palatable foods may vary with macronutrient composition. For instance, Dr Fulton’s work indicates that some dietary fats can foster increased stress responses (see previous section), whereas dietary carbohydrates may preferentially curb stress responses (Anderson et al., Citation1987; Barr et al., Citation1999; de Castro, Citation1987; Deuster et al., Citation1992; Markus et al., Citation2000; Utter et al., Citation1999). Indeed, the high caloric diet used in Dr Wilson's studies of rhesus monkeys derived 45% of its calories from saturated fats. Finally, weight gain and associated metabolic dysfunction secondary to palatable food intake, as occurs following long-term and/or unlimited access to palatable food, may enhance stress responses. Consistent with this inference, increased adiposity is often associated with increased HPA axis tone (Francis et al., Citation2013; Lawlor et al., Citation2011; Lu et al., Citation2014; Ryan et al., Citation2014). Notably, results from Dr Ulrich-Lai’s limited sucrose paradigm suggest that it may be possible to prevent these stress-promoting effects by limiting the amount and/or duration of palatable food intake.

Figure 10. Summary schematic illustrating the complex inter-relationships between stress and ingestive behavior, and highlighting the overall approach and key findings of each symposium speaker.

Figure 10. Summary schematic illustrating the complex inter-relationships between stress and ingestive behavior, and highlighting the overall approach and key findings of each symposium speaker.

Effects of stress on food intake

Dr Wilson has used a non-human primate model (female rhesus monkeys) to examine the role of social stress on dietary choice and intake, and the resulting physiological impact of stress-induced food intake. His group has shown that compared to social dominants, social subordinates eat more when offered unlimited access to a rich dietary environment (choice between standard chow and a high fat-high sugar diet). In contrast, Dr Petrovich has used a rat model to examine the neurobiological mechanism by which a more intense and imminent psychological stress, i.e. conditioned fear, promotes hypophagia and anorexia. She has shown that the CeA, likely working with the vmPFC and LHA, is essential for reducing food intake during fear anticipation in otherwise hungry rats. Importantly, the divergent consummatory effects that are observed by these two research groups provide critical insight into the consequences of stress on food intake. For example, the influence of stress on ingestive behavior depends in large part on the type and/or chronicity of stress experienced, with acute, intense and imminent threats of pain/injury being linked with reduced food intake (Bellisle et al., Citation1990; Popper et al., Citation1989) (see previous section by Dr Petrovich), while ongoing social stress is associated with increased food intake (Bartolomucci et al., Citation2009) (see previous section by Dr Wilson). Stress effects also vary with the type of food offered, such that highly palatable food options tend to produce more overeating in response to stress than low palatability foods (Bertiere et al., Citation1984; Pecoraro et al., Citation2004; Willner et al., Citation1998) (see section by Dr Wilson).

Future research priorities

This research symposium closed with an extended group discussion between the contributing authors and workshop participants that was moderated by Dr Rinaman. This lively discussion focused in large part on highlighting future research priorities for the field. More specifically, the group broached several intriguing questions that are ripe for experimental analysis. A few of these questions are posed below.

  • How, when, and by what central neural mechanism(s) does palatable food intake transition from promoting stress relief to enhancing stress responsiveness? In other words, how does the apparently adaptive or beneficial effect of consuming rewarding foods become maladaptive and deleterious with regards to stress responsiveness? Important considerations include the type and duration of the stressor, the macronutrient quality of the palatable diet, and subject sex.

  • What is the role or importance of timing (with regards to stress exposure) and context (e.g. safe/familiar environment versus novel/challenging environment) in mechanisms that underlie stress-induced overeating versus stress-induced anorexia?

  • What is the role of sex hormones in mechanisms that underlie stress-induced overeating versus stress-induced anorexia? Is the female vulnerability to stress-related overeating and stress-related anorexia mediated by a common mechanism?

  • Do “comfort foods” provide transitory stress relief by blunting activation of the HPA axis and other stress responses, by stimulating dopamine reward or other systems that counteract or compete with stress, or both? Attention needs to be focused on peripheral signals (e.g. glucocorticoids and inflammatory cytokines) and central neuropeptides (e.g. CRF) as mediators for these effects.

  • Is diet important for the bi-directionality of stress on food intake? For example, can prolonged exposure to stress suppress appetite and food intake even when palatable foods are available? Does dietary choice matter and, if so, how does stress-induced disruption of circuits involving prefrontal regions predict diet choice and consumption?

  • Is diet-induced obesity linked to a particularly vulnerable stress-responsive phenotype? Are the deleterious effects of stress amplified in individuals who are prone to dietary obesity? Is obesity resistance associated with stress resilience?

As these and other fascinating questions are addressed by ongoing and future work, including thoughtful consideration and integration of data across species and diverse experimental paradigms, the field will move towards an improved understanding of the functional associations and causal links among stress, food intake and emotion.

Acknowledgements

The collaborative efforts of Dr Zach Johnson, Dr Mar Sanchez, Dr Donna Toufexis and Dr Vasiliki Michopoulos were critical for Dr Wilson’s studies.

Declaration of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. Yvonne M. Ulrich-Lai was supported by NIH grants R01 DK091425 (Y. M. U.), R03 DK089018 (Y. M. U.) and K01 DK078906 (Y. M. U.). Stephanie Fulton was supported by a CIHR grant (MOP-123280) and New Investigator salary award. Mark Wilson was supported by NIH grants MH081816 (D. T.), MH79100 (M. W.), DK096983 (M. W.), and ODP51011132. Gorica Petrovich was supported by NIH grant DK085721, and Linda Rinaman was supported by NIH grants DK100685 and MH059911.

Notes

1In this section, “anorexia” refers to inhibition of food consumption in general, while “anorexia nervosa” refers to an eating disorder.

References

  • Adam TC, Epel ES. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol Behav 91:449–58
  • Ambroggi F, Turiault M, Milet A, Deroche-Gamonet V, Parnaudeau S, Balado E, Barik J, et al. (2009). Stress and addiction: glucocorticoid receptor in dopaminoceptive neurons facilitates cocaine seeking. Nat Neurosci 12:247–9
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press
  • Anderson KE, Rosner W, Khan MS, New MI, Pang SY, Wissel PS, Kappas A. (1987). Diet-hormone interactions: protein/carbohydrate ratio alters reciprocally the plasma levels of testosterone and cortisol and their respective binding globulins in man. Life Sci 40:1761–8
  • Anderson RJ, Freedland KE, Clouse RE, Lustman PJ. (2001). The prevalence of comorbid depression in adults with diabetes: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care 24:1069–78
  • Anisman H, Matheson K. (2005). Stress, depression, and anhedonia: caveats concerning animal models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:525–46
  • Arce M, Michopoulos V, Shepard KN, Ha QC, Wilson ME. (2010). Diet choice, cortisol reactivity, and emotional feeding in socially housed rhesus monkeys. Physiol Behav 101:446–55
  • Archer ZA, Mercer JG. (2007). Brain responses to obesogenic diets and diet-induced obesity. Proc Nutr Soc 66:124–30
  • Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. (2008). Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:20–39
  • Barnard ND, Bunner AE, Agarwal U. (2014). Saturated and trans fats and dementia: a systematic review. Neurobiol Aging 35:S65–73
  • Barr RG, Pantel MS, Young SN, Wright JH, Hendricks LA, Gravel R. (1999). The response of crying newborns to sucrose: is it a “sweetness” effect? Physiol Behav 66:409–17
  • Bartolomucci A, Cabassi A, Govoni P, Ceresini G, Cero C, Berra D, Dadomo H, et al. (2009). Metabolic consequences and vulnerability to diet-induced obesity in male mice under chronic social stress. PLoS One 4:e4331
  • Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. (1999). Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience 89:637–41
  • Bell ME, Bhargava A, Soriano L, Laugero K, Akana SF, Dallman MF. (2002). Sucrose intake and corticosterone interact with cold to modulate ingestive behaviour, energy balance, autonomic outflow and neuroendocrine responses during chronic stress. J Neuroendocrinol 14:330–42
  • Bellisle F, Louis-Sylvestre J, Linet N, Rocaboy B, Dalle B, Cheneau F, L'Hinoret D, Guyot L. (1990). Anxiety and food intake in men. Psychosom Med 52:452–7
  • Berthoud H-R, Münzberg H. (2011). The lateral hypothalamus as integrator of metabolic and environmental needs: from electrical self-stimulation to opto-genetics. Physiol Behav 104:29–39
  • Berthoud HR. (2007). Interactions between the “cognitive” and “metabolic” brain in the control of food intake. Physiol Behav 91:486–98
  • Bertiere MC, Sy TM, Baigts F, Mandenoff A, Apfelbaum M. (1984). Stress and sucrose hyperphagia: role of endogenous opiates. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 20:675–9
  • Bewernick BH, Kayser S, Sturm V, Schlaepfer TE. (2012). Long-term effects of nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation in treatment-resistant depression: evidence for sustained efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:1975–85
  • Bhatnagar S, Vining C, Denski K. (2004). Regulation of chronic stress-induced changes in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity by the basolateral amygdala. Ann NY Acad Sci 1032:315–19
  • Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. (1969). Crouching as an index of fear. J Comp Physiol Psychol 67:370–5
  • Blum K, Sheridan PJ, Wood RC, Braverman ER, Chen TJ, Cull JG, Comings DE. (1996). The D2 dopamine receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome. J R Soc Med 89:396–400
  • Bobzean SA, DeNobrega AK, Perrotti LI. (2014). Sex differences in the neurobiology of drug addiction. Exp Neurol 259:64–74
  • Burke AR, Miczek KA. (2013). Stress in adolescence and drugs of abuse in rodent models: role of dopamine, CRF, and HPA axis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 231:1557–80
  • Cannon WB. (1915). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage. New York: Appleton
  • Carter ME, Soden ME, Zweifel LS, Palmiter RD. (2013). Genetic identification of a neural circuit that suppresses appetite. Nature 503:111–14
  • Christiansen AM, Dekloet AD, Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. (2011). “Snacking” causes long term attenuation of HPA axis stress responses and enhancement of brain FosB/deltaFosB expression in rats. Physiol Behav 103:111–16
  • Cole S, Powell DJ, Petrovich GD. (2013). Differential recruitment of distinct amygdalar nuclei across appetitive associative learning. Learn Mem 20:295–9
  • Collier G, Novell K. (1967). Saccharin as a sugar surrogate. J Comp Physiol Psychol 64:401–8
  • Coover G, Ursin H, Levine S. (1973). Corticosterone and avoidance in rats with basolateral amygdala lesions. J Comp Physiol Psychol 85:111–22
  • Cottone P, Sabino V, Roberto M, Bajo M, Pockros L, Frihauf JB, Fekete EM, et al. (2009a). CRF system recruitment mediates dark side of compulsive eating. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:20016–20
  • Cottone P, Sabino V, Steardo L, Zorrilla EP. (2009b). Consummatory, anxiety-related and metabolic adaptations in female rats with alternating access to preferred food. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:38–49
  • Dallman MF, Akana SF, Pecoraro NC, Warne JP, la Fleur SE, Foster MT. (2007). Glucocorticoids, the etiology of obesity and the metabolic syndrome. Curr Alzheimer Res 4:199–204
  • Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, Fleur SEl, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Bell ME, et al. (2003). Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food". Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:11696–701
  • Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, la Fleur SE. (2005). Chronic stress and comfort foods: self-medication and abdominal obesity. Brain Behav Immun 19:275–80
  • Davis JF, Tracy AL, Schurdak JD, Tschop MH, Lipton JW, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. (2008). Exposure to elevated levels of dietary fat attenuates psychostimulant reward and mesolimbic dopamine turnover in the rat. Behav Neurosci 122:1257–63
  • de Castro JM. (1987). Macronutrient relationships with meal patterns and mood in the spontaneous feeding behavior of humans. Physiol Behav 39:561–9
  • de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao X, Foye PE, Danielson PE, Fukuhara C, et al. (1998). The hypocretins: hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:322–7
  • De Souza CT, Araujo EP, Bordin S, Ashimine R, Zollner RL, Boschero AC, Saad MJ, Velloso LA. (2005). Consumption of a fat-rich diet activates a proinflammatory response and induces insulin resistance in the hypothalamus. Endocrinology 146:4192–9
  • Deuster P, Singh A, Hofmann A, Moses F, Chrousos G. (1992). Hormonal responses to ingesting water or a carbohydrate beverage during a 2 h run. Med Sci Sports Exerc 24:72–9
  • Dube L, LeBel JL, Lu J. (2005). Affect asymmetry and comfort food consumption. Physiol Behav 86:559–67
  • Elmquist JK, Elias CF, Saper CB. (1999). From lesions to leptin: hypothalamic control of food intake and body weight. Neuron 22:221–32
  • Epel E, Jimenez S, Brownell K, Stroud L, Stoney C, Niaura R. (2004). Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome? Ann NY Acad Sci 1032:208–10
  • Epel E, Lapidus R, McEwen B, Brownell K. (2001). Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26:37–49
  • Epel ES, McEwen B, Seeman T, Matthews K, Castellazzo G, Brownell KD, Bell J, Ickovics JR. (2000). Stress and body shape: stress-induced cortisol secretion is consistently greater among women with central fat. Psychosom Med 62:623–32
  • Fanselow MS. (1984). What is conditioned fear? Trends Neurosci 7:460–2
  • Feldman S, Siegel RA, Conforti N. (1983). Differential effects of medial forebrain bundle lesions on adrenocortical responses following limbic stimulation. Neuroscience 9:157–63
  • Fendt M, Fanselow MS. (1999). The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23:743–60
  • Fernandez M, Blass EM, Hernandez-Reif M, Field T, Diego M, Sanders C. (2003). Sucrose attenuates a negative electroencephalographic response to an aversive stimulus for newborns. J Dev Behav Pediatr 24:261–6
  • Finger BC, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. (2011). High-fat diet selectively protects against the effects of chronic social stress in the mouse. Neuroscience 192:351–60
  • Finger BC, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. (2012). The temporal impact of chronic intermittent psychosocial stress on high-fat diet-induced alterations in body weight. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:729–41
  • Fontanini A, Grossman SE, Figueroa JA, Katz DB. (2009). Distinct subtypes of basolateral amygdala taste neurons reflect palatability and reward. J Neurosci 29:2486–95
  • Foster MT, Warne JP, Ginsberg AB, Horneman HF, Pecoraro NC, Akana SF, Dallman MF. (2009). Palatable foods, stress, and energy stores sculpt corticotropin-releasing factor, adrenocorticotropin, and corticosterone concentrations after restraint. Endocrinology 150:2325–33
  • Francis LA, Granger DA, Susman EJ. (2013). Adrenocortical regulation, eating in the absence of hunger and BMI in young children. Appetite 64:32–8
  • Francolin-Silva AL, da Silva Hernandes A, Fukuda MT, Valadares CT, Almeida SS. (2006). Anxiolytic-like effects of short-term postnatal protein malnutrition in the elevated plus-maze test. Behav Brain Res 173:310–14
  • Fulton S. (2010). Appetite and reward. Front Neuroendocrinol 31:85–103
  • Gibson EL. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiol Behav 89:53–61
  • Giordano GD, Renzetti P, Parodi RC, Foppiani L, Zandrino F, Giordano G, Sardanelli F. (2001). Volume measurement with magnetic resonance imaging of hippocampus-amygdala formation in patients with anorexia nervosa. J Endocrinol Invest 24:510–14
  • Glowa JR, Gold PW. (1991). Corticotropin releasing hormone produces profound anorexigenic effects in the rhesus monkey. Neuropeptides 18:55–61
  • Godfrey J, Kelly C, Zhang X, Castellanos X, Wilson M, Sanchez MM. (2013). Cingulo-opercular and limbic intrinsic functional connectivity is affected by delayed puberty and social status in female rhesus macaques. Annual Meeting of the Society of Biological Psychiatry (SOBP), San Francisco, CA, May, 2013
  • Goeders NE. (2002). Stress and cocaine addiction. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 301:785–9
  • Goldstein LE, Rasmusson AM, Bunney BS, Roth RH. (1996). Role of the amygdala in the coordination of behavioral, neuroendocrine, and prefrontal cortical monoamine responses to psychological stress in the rat. J Neurosci 16:4787–98
  • Govindan RM, Behen ME, Helder E, Makki MI, Chugani HT. (2010). Altered water diffusivity in cortical association tracts in children with early deprivation identified with Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS). Cereb Cortex 20:561–9
  • Grant KA, Shively CA, Nader MA, Ehrenkaufer RL, Line SW, Morton TE, Gage HD, Mach RH. (1998). Effect of social status on striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding characteristics in cynomolgus monkeys assessed with positron emission tomography. Synapse 29:80–3
  • Groesz LM, McCoy S, Carl J, Saslow L, Stewart J, Adler N, Laraia B, Epel E. (2012). What is eating you? Stress and the drive to eat. Appetite 58:717–21
  • Grunberg NE, Straub RO. (1992). The role of gender and taste class in the effects of stress on eating. Health Psychol 11:97–100
  • Haber SN, Knutson B. (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:4–26
  • Hahn JD, Swanson LW. (2010). Distinct patterns of neuronal inputs and outputs of the juxtaparaventricular and suprafornical regions of the lateral hypothalamic area in the male rat. Brain Res Rev 64:14–103
  • Handa RJ, Burgess LH, Kerr JE, O'Keefe JA. (1994). Gonadal steroid hormone receptors and sex differences in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Horm Behav 28:464–76
  • Hariri N, Thibault L. (2010). High-fat diet-induced obesity in animal models. Nutr Res Rev 23:270–99
  • Harris RB, Zhou J, Youngblood BD, Rybkin II, Smagin GN, Ryan DH. (1998). Effect of repeated stress on body weight and body composition of rats fed low- and high-fat diets. Am J Physiol 275:R1928–38
  • Herod SM, Dettmer AM, Novak MA, Meyer JS, Cameron JL. (2011a). Sensitivity to stress-induced reproductive dysfunction is associated with a selective but not a generalized increase in activity of the adrenal axis. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 300:E28–36
  • Herod SM, Pohl CR, Cameron JL. (2011b). Treatment with a CRH-R1 antagonist prevents stress-induced suppression of the central neural drive to the reproductive axis in female macaques. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 300:E19–27
  • Hotta M, Shibasaki T, Arai K, Demura H. (1999). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 1 mediates emotional stress-induced inhibition of food intake and behavioral changes in rats. Brain Res 823:221–5
  • Hryhorczuk C, Sharma S, Fulton SE. (2013). Metabolic disturbances connecting obesity and depression. Front Neurosci 7:177 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00177
  • Huang YY, Martin KC, Kandel ER. (2000). Both protein kinase A and mitogen-activated protein kinase are required in the amygdala for the macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term potentiation. J Neurosci 20:6317–25
  • Iemolo A, Blasio A, St Cyr SA, Jiang F, Rice KC, Sabino V, Cottone P. (2013). CRF-CRF receptor system in the central and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala differentially mediates excessive eating of palatable food. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:2456–66
  • Izzo E, Sanna PP, Koob GF. (2005). Impairment of dopaminergic system function after chronic treatment with corticotropin-releasing factor. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 81:701–8
  • Jimerson DC, Wolfe BE. (2006). Psychobiology of eating disorders. In: Wonderlich A, Mitchell JE, de Zwaan M,Steiger H, editors. Annual review of eating disorders part 2-2006. Oxford: Radcliffe Publishing. p 1–15
  • Kamara K, Eskay R, Castonguay T. (1998). High-fat diets and stress responsivity. Physiol Behav 64:1–6
  • Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. (2011). Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav 103:59–68
  • Kaplan J, Chen H, Appt SE, Lees CJ, Franke AA, Berga SL, Wilson M, et al. (2010). Impairment of ovarian function and associated health-related abnormalities are attributable to low social status in premenopausal monkeys and not mitigated by a high-isoflavone soy diet. Hum Reprod 25:3083–94
  • Kaufman J, Charney D. (2001). Effects of early stress on brain structure and function: implications for understanding the relationship between child maltreatment and depression. Dev Psychopathol 13:451–71
  • Kaye W. (2008). Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav 94:121–35
  • Kim Y, Yang HY, Kim AJ, Lim Y. (2013). Academic stress levels were positively associated with sweet food consumption among Korean high-school students. Nutrition 29:213–18
  • Kitraki E, Soulis G, Gerozissis K. (2004). Impaired neuroendocrine response to stress following a short-term fat-enriched diet. Neuroendocrinology 79:338–45
  • Klein DA, Walsh BT. (2004). Eating disorders: clinical features and pathophysiology. Physiol Behav 81:359–74
  • Koob G, Kreek MJ. (2007). Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry 164:1149–59
  • Koob GF, Buck CL, Cohen A, Edwards S, Park PE, Schlosburg JE, Schmeichel B, et al. (2014). Addiction as a stress surfeit disorder. Neuropharmacology 76:370–82
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. (2001). Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 24:97–129
  • la Fleur SE, Houshyar H, Roy M, Dallman MF. (2005). Choice of lard, but not total lard calories, damps adrenocorticotropin responses to restraint. Endocrinology 146:2193–9
  • la Fleur SE, Luijendijk MC, van der Zwaal EM, Brans MA, Adan RA. (2014). The snacking rat as model of human obesity: effects of a free-choice high-fat high-sugar diet on meal patterns. Int J Obes (Lond) 38:643–9
  • Land BB, Narayanan NS, Liu RJ, Gianessi CA, Brayton CE, Grimaldi DM, Sarhan M, et al. (2014). Medial prefrontal D1 dopamine neurons control food intake. Nat Neurosci 17:248–53
  • Laugero KD, Falcon LM, Tucker KL. (2011). Relationship between perceived stress and dietary and activity patterns in older adults participating in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. Appetite 56:194–204
  • Lawlor DA, Harbord RM, Tybjaerg-Hansen A, Palmer TM, Zacho J, Benn M, Timpson NJ, et al. (2011). Using genetic loci to understand the relationship between adiposity and psychological distress: a Mendelian Randomization study in the Copenhagen General Population Study of 53,221 adults. J Intern Med 269:525–37
  • LeDoux J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73:653–76
  • LeDoux JE. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–84
  • Levin BE. (2000). Metabolic imprinting on genetically predisposed neural circuits perpetuates obesity. Nutrition 16:909–15
  • Lieberman HR, Wurtman JJ, Chew B. (1986). Changes in mood after carbohydrate consumption among obese individuals. Am J Clin Nutr 44:772–8
  • Lin PY, Su KP. (2007). A meta-analytic review of double-blind, placebo-controlled trials of antidepressant efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids. J Clin Psychiatry 68:1056–61
  • Lisman J, Schulman H, Cline H. (2002). The molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:175–90
  • Lu Q, Tao F, Hou F, Zhang Z, Sun Y, Xu Y, Xu S, Zhao Y. (2014). Cortisol reactivity, delay discounting and percent body fat in Chinese urban young adolescents. Appetite 72:13–20
  • Luppino FS, de Wit LM, Bouvy PF, Stijnen T, Cuijpers P, Penninx BW, Zitman FG. (2010). Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:220–9
  • Macht M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite 50:1–11
  • Maniam J, Morris MJ. (2010). Palatable cafeteria diet ameliorates anxiety and depression-like symptoms following an adverse early environment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:717–28
  • Maniam J, Morris MJ. (2012). The link between stress and feeding behaviour. Neuropharmacology 63:97–110
  • Maniscalco JW, Kreisler AD, Rinaman L. (2013). Satiation and stress-induced hypophagia: examining the role of hindbrain neurons expressing prolactin-releasing peptide or glucagon-like peptide 1. Front Neurosci 6:1–17
  • Maren S. (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci 24:897–931
  • Markus R, Panhuysen G, Tuiten A, Koppeschaar H. (2000). Effects of food on cortisol and mood in vulnerable subjects under controllable and uncontrollable stress. Physiol Behav 70:333–42
  • Martel P, Fantino M. (1996a). Influence of the amount of food ingested on mesolimbic dopaminergic system activity: a microdialysis study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 55:297–302
  • Martel P, Fantino M. (1996b). Mesolimbic dopaminergic system activity as a function of food reward: a microdialysis study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 53:221–6
  • Martinowich K, Manji H, Lu B. (2007). New insights into BDNF function in depression and anxiety. Nat Neurosci 10:1089–93
  • McCann BS, Warnick GR, Knopp RH. (1990). Changes in plasma lipids and dietary intake accompanying shifts in perceived workload and stress. Psychosom Med 52:97–108
  • Mena JD, Sadeghian K, Baldo BA. (2011). Induction of hyperphagia and carbohydrate intake by µ-opioid receptor stimulation in circumscribed region of frontal cortex. J Neurosci 31:3249–60
  • Mena JD, Selleck RA, Baldo BA. (2013). Mu-opioid stimulation in rat prefrontal cortex engages hypothalamic orexin/hypocretin-containing neurons, and reveals dissociable roles of nucleus accumbens and hypothalamus in cortically driven feeding. J Neurosci 33:18540–52
  • Meye FJ, Adan RA. (2014). Feelings about food: the ventral tegmental area in food reward and emotional eating. Trends Pharmacol Sci 35:31–40
  • Micha R, Mozaffarian D. (2010). Saturated fat and cardiometabolic risk factors, coronary heart disease, stroke, and diabetes: a fresh look at the evidence. Lipids 45:893–905
  • Michopoulos V, Higgins M, Toufexis D, Wilson ME. (2012a). Social subordination produces distinct stress-related phenotypes in female rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1071–85
  • Michopoulos V, Loucks T, Berga SL, Rivier J, Wilson ME. (2010). Increased ghrelin sensitivity and calorie consumption in subordinate monkeys is affected by short-term astressin B administration. Endocrine 38:227–34
  • Michopoulos V, Reding KM, Wilson ME, Toufexis D. (2012b). Social subordination impairs hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function in female rhesus monkeys. Horm Behav 62:389–99
  • Michopoulos V, Shepard KN, Arce M, Whitley JA, Wilson ME. (2009). Food History and social status affect food intake in monkeys. Portland, OR: Society for the Study of Ingesive Behavior, 52:848
  • Michopoulos V, Toufexis D, Wilson ME. (2012c). Social stress interacts with diet history to promote emotional feeding in females. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1479–90
  • Michopoulos V, Wilson ME. (2011). Body weight decreases induced by estradiol in female rhesus monkeys are dependent upon social status. Physiol Behav 102:382–8
  • Miller AH, Maletic V, Raison CL. (2009). Inflammation and its discontents: the role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biol Psychiatry 65:732–41
  • Minor TR, Saade S. (1997). Poststress glucose mitigates behavioral impairment in rats in the “learned helplessness” model of psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 42:324–34
  • Moore CJ, Johnson ZP, Toufexis D, Wilson ME. (2015). Antagonism of CRF Type 1 receptors attenuates caloric intake of free feeding subordinate female rhesus monkeys in a rich dietary environment. J Neuroendocrinol 27:33–43
  • Moore CJ, Lowe J, Michopoulos V, Ulam P, Toufexis D, Wilson ME, Johnson Z. (2013). Small changes in meal patterns lead to significant changes in total caloric intake. Effects of diet and social status on food intake in female rhesus monkeys. Appetite 62:60–9
  • Moranis A, Delpech JC, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Aubert A, Guesnet P, Lavialle M, Joffre C, Laye S. (2012). Long term adequate n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid diet protects from depressive-like behavior but not from working memory disruption and brain cytokine expression in aged mice. Brain Behav Immun 26:721–31
  • Morgan D, Grant KA, Gage HD, Mach RH, Kaplan JR, Prioleau O, Nader SH, et al. (2002). Social dominance in monkeys: dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-administration. Nat Neurosci 5:169–74
  • Morton GJ, Cummings DE, Baskin DG, Barsh GS, Schwartz MW. (2006). Central nervous system control of food intake and body weight. Nature 443:289–95
  • Muramoto K, Ono T, Nishijo H, Fukuda M. (1993). Rat amygdaloid neuron responses during auditory discrimination. Neuroscience 52:621–36
  • Nakamura S, Tsumori T, Yokota S, Oka T, Yasui Y. (2009). Amygdaloid axons innervate melanin-concentrating hormone and orexin containing neurons in the mouse lateral hypothalamus. Brain Res 1278:66–74
  • Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA Jr. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol Psychiatry 59:1151–9
  • Oddy WH, Hickling S, Smith MA, O'Sullivan TA, Robinson M, de Klerk NH, Beilin LJ, et al. (2011). Dietary intake of omega-3 fatty acids and risk of depressive symptoms in adolescents. Depress Anxiety 28:582–8
  • O'Doherty JP. (2011). Contributions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to goal-directed action selection. Ann NY Acad Sci 1239:118–29
  • Oliver G, Wardle J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol Behav 66:511–15
  • Packard AE, Ghosal S, Herman JP, Woods SC, Ulrich-Lai YM. (2014). Chronic variable stress improves glucose tolerance in rats with sucrose-induced prediabetes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 47:178–88
  • Panchal SK, Brown L. (2011). Rodent models for metabolic syndrome research. J Biomed Biotechnol 2011:351982
  • Pasquali R, Ambrosi B, Armanini D, Cavagnini F, Uberti ED, Del Rio G, de Pergola G, et al. (2002). Cortisol and ACTH response to oral dexamethasone in obesity and effects of sex, body fat distribution, and dexamethasone concentrations: a dose-response study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 87:166–75
  • Pecoraro N, Reyes F, Gomez F, Bhargava A, Dallman MF. (2004). Chronic stress promotes palatable feeding, which reduces signs of stress: feedforward and feedback effects of chronic stress. Endocrinology 145:3754–62
  • Peet M, Murphy B, Shay J, Horrobin D. (1998). Depletion of omega-3 fatty acid levels in red blood cell membranes of depressive patients. Biol Psychiatry 43:315–19
  • Pelchat ML. (2002). Of human bondage: food craving, obsession, compulsion, and addiction. Physiol Behav 76:347–52
  • Petrovich GD, Canteras NS, Swanson LW. (2001). Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res Rev 38:247–89
  • Petrovich GD, Hobin MP, Reppucci CJ. (2012). Selective Fos induction in hypothalamic orexin/hypocretin, but not melanin-concentrating hormone neurons, by a learned food-cue that stimulates feeding in sated rats. Neuroscience 224:70–80
  • Petrovich GD, Lougee M. (2011). Sex differences in fear cue-induced inhibition of feeding: prolonged effect in female rats. Physiol Behav 104:996–1001
  • Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Mody P, Holland PC, Gallagher M. (2009). Central but not basolateral amygdala is critical for control of feeding by aversive conditioned cues. J Neurosci 29:15205–12
  • Petrovich GD. (2013). Forebrain networks and the control of feeding by environmental learned cues. Physiol Behav 121:10–18
  • Phivilay A, Julien C, Tremblay C, Berthiaume L, Julien P, Giguere Y, Calon F. (2009). High dietary consumption of trans fatty acids decreases brain docosahexaenoic acid but does not alter amyloid-beta and tau pathologies in the 3xTg-AD model of Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience 159:296–307
  • Popper R, Smits G, Meiselman HL, Hirsch E. (1989). Eating in combat: a survey of U.S. Marines. Mil Med 154:619–23
  • Prasad A, Prasad C. (1996). Short-term consumption of a diet rich in fat decreases anxiety response in adult male rats. Physiol Behav 60:1039–42
  • Rada P, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. (2005). Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience 134:737–44
  • Reppucci CJ, Kuthyar M, Petrovich GD. (2013). Contextual fear cues inhibit eating in food-deprived male and female rats. Appetite 69:186–95
  • Reppucci CJ, Petrovich GD. (2012). Neuroanatomical evidence for a topographically organized forebrain network composed of the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral hypothalamus in rats. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience. Program # 283.05
  • Reppucci CJ, Petrovich GD. (2014). Suppressed Fos induction within the central nucleus of the amygdala corresponds with inhibited feeding in the presence of a fear-cue in male and female rats. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience Program # 256. 10
  • Rodrigues SM, Farb CR, Bauer EP, LeDoux JE, Schafe GE. (2004). Pavlovian fear conditioning regulates Thr286 autophosphorylation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II at lateral amygdala synapses. J Neurosci 24:3281–8
  • Rolls BJ, Van Duijvenvoorde PM, Rowe EA. (1983). Variety in the diet enhances intake in a meal and contributes to the development of obesity in the rat. Physiol Behav 31:21–7
  • Rosqvist F, Iggman D, Kullberg J, Cedernaes J, Johansson HE, Larsson A, Johansson L, et al. (2014). Overfeeding polyunsaturated and saturated fat causes distinct effects on liver and visceral fat accumulation in humans. Diabetes 63:2356–68
  • Russo SJ, Nestler EJ. (2013). The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:609–25
  • Ryan KK, Mul JD, Clemmensen C, Egan AE, Begg DP, Halcomb K, Seeley RJ, et al. (2014). Loss of melanocortin-4 receptor function attenuates HPA responses to psychological stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 42:98–105
  • Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, Tanaka H, Williams SC, et al. (1998). Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92:573–85
  • Sanchez MM, Hearn EF, Do D, Rilling JK, Herndon JG. (1998). Differential rearing affects corpus callosum size and cognitive function of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res 812:38–49
  • Schachter S, Goldman R, Gordon R. (1968). Effects of fear, food deprivation, and obesity on eating. J Pers Soc Psychol 10:91–7
  • Sclafani A. (2004). Oral and postoral determinants of food reward. Physiol Behav 81:773–9
  • Seeger G, Braus DF, Ruf M, Goldberger U, Schmidt MH. (2002). Body image distortion reveals amygdala activation in patients with anorexia nervosa – a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett 326:25–8
  • Seymour B, Dolan R. (2008). Emotion, decision making, and the amygdala. Neuron 58:662–71
  • Sharma S, Decarie-Spain L, Hryhorczuk C, Alquier T, Fulton S. (2013a). A saturated but not a monounsaturated high-fat diet increases depressive-like behavior and neuroinflammation. Keystone Symposia: Neuronal Control of Appetite, Metabolism and Weight, Banff, Alberta
  • Sharma S, Fernandes MF, Fulton S. (2013b). Adaptations in brain reward circuitry underlie palatable food cravings and anxiety induced by high-fat diet withdrawal. Int J Obes (Lond) 37:1183–91
  • Sharma S, Fulton S. (2013). Diet-induced obesity promotes depressive-like behaviour that is associated with neural adaptations in brain reward circuitry. Int J Obes (Lond) 37:382–9
  • Shide DJ, Blass EM. (1989). Opioidlike effects of intraoral infusions of corn oil and polycose on stress reactions in 10-day-old rats. Behav Neurosci 103:1168–75
  • Shively CA, Grant KA, Ehrenkaufer RL, Mach RH, Nader MA. (1997a). Social stress, depression, and brain dopamine in female cynomolgus monkeys. Ann NY Acad Sci 807:574–7
  • Shively CA, Laber-Laird K, Anton RF. (1997b). Behavior and physiology of social stress and depression in female cynomolgus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 41:871–82
  • Silva AJ, Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Kida S. (1998). CREB and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 21:127–48
  • Sinha R, Jastreboff AM. (2013). Stress as a common risk factor for obesity and addiction. Biol Psychiatry 73:827–35
  • Smagin GN, Howell LA, Redmann S, Jr, Ryan DH, Harris RB. (1999). Prevention of stress-induced weight loss by third ventricle CRF receptor antagonist. Am J Physiol 276:R1461–8
  • Smith JC, Sclafani A. (2002). Saccharin as a sugar surrogate revisited. Appetite 38:155–60
  • Solomon MB, Jankord R, Flak JN, Herman JP. (2011). Chronic stress, energy balance and adiposity in female rats. Physiol Behav 102:84–90
  • Sominsky L, Spencer SJ. (2014). Eating behavior and stress: a pathway to obesity. Front Psychol 5:434 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00434
  • Soulis G, Kitraki E, Gerozissis K. (2005). Early neuroendocrine alterations in female rats following a diet moderately enriched in fat. Cell Mol Neurobiol 25:869–80
  • Soulis G, Papalexi E, Kittas C, Kitraki E. (2007). Early impact of a fat-enriched diet on behavioral responses of male and female rats. Behav Neurosci 121:483–90
  • Strack AM, Akana SF, Horsley CJ, Dallman MF. (1997). A hypercaloric load induces thermogenesis but inhibits stress responses in the SNS and HPA system. Am J Physiol 272:R840–8
  • Su KP, Lai HC, Yang HT, Su WP, Peng CY, Chang JP, Chang HC, Pariante CM. (2014). Omega-3 Fatty acids in the prevention of interferon-alpha-induced depression: results from a randomized, controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 76:559–66
  • Suchecki D, Antunes J, Tufik S. (2003). Palatable solutions during paradoxical sleep deprivation: reduction of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and lack of effect on energy imbalance. J Neuroendocrinol 15:815–21
  • Swanson LW, Petrovich GD. (1998). What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci 21:323–31
  • Takano A, Shiga T, Kitagawa N, Koyama T, Katoh C, Tsukamoto E, Tamaki N. (2001). Abnormal neuronal network in anorexia nervosa studied with I-123-IMP SPECT. Psychiatry Res 107:45–50
  • Tamashiro KL, Hegeman MA, Sakai RR. (2006). Chronic social stress in a changing dietary environment. Physiol Behav 89:536–42
  • Tamashiro KL, Nguyen MM, Sakai RR. (2005). Social stress: from rodents to primates. Front Neuroendocrinol 26:27–40
  • Tamashiro KL, Sakai RR, Shively CA, Karatsoreos IN, Reagan LP. (2011). Chronic stress, metabolism, and metabolic syndrome. Stress 14:468–74
  • Tannenbaum BM, Brindley DN, Tannenbaum GS, Dallman MF, McArthur MD, Meaney MJ. (1997). High-fat feeding alters both basal and stress-induced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in the rat. Am J Physiol 273:E1168–77
  • Tchernof A, Despres JP. (2013). Pathophysiology of human visceral obesity: an update. Physiol Rev 93:359–404
  • Teegarden SL, Nestler EJ, Bale TL. (2008). Delta FosB-mediated alterations in dopamine signaling are normalized by a palatable high-fat diet. Biol Psychiatry 64:941–50
  • Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP. (2002). Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Psychiatr Clin North Am 25:397–426, vii–viii
  • Thaler JP, Yi CX, Schur EA, Guyenet SJ, Hwang BH, Dietrich MO, Zhao X, et al. (2012). Obesity is associated with hypothalamic injury in rodents and humans. J Clin Invest 122:153–62
  • Tomasi D, Volkow ND. (2013). Striatocortical pathway dysfunction in addiction and obesity: differences and similarities. Crit Rev Biochem Mol Biol 48:1–19
  • Tominaga-Yoshino K, Urakubo T, Okada M, Matsuda H, Ogura A. (2008). Repetitive induction of late-phase LTP produces long-lasting synaptic enhancement accompanied by synaptogenesis in cultured hippocampal slices. Hippocampus 18:281–93
  • Toufexis D, Wilson ME. (2012). Dihydrotestosterone differentially modulates the cortisol response of the hypothalamic-pituitary axis in male and female rhesus macaques, and restores circadian secretion of cortisol in females. Brain Res 1429:43–51
  • Treasure J, Claudino AM, Zucker N. (2010). Eating disorders. Lancet 375:583–93
  • Tryon MS, Carter CS, Decant R, Laugero KD. (2013). Chronic stress exposure may affect the brain's response to high calorie food cues and predispose to obesogenic eating habits. Physiol Behav 120:233–42
  • Tung J, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP, Hansen KD, Michopoulos V, Toufexis D, Michelini K, et al. (2012). Social environment is associated with gene regulatory variation in the rhesus macaque immune system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:6490–5
  • Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, Dalgleish T, Phillips ML, Ng VW, Andrew CM, et al. (2004). Medial prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry 161:1238–46
  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Christiansen AM, Ostrander MM, Jones AA, Jones KR, Choi DC, Krause EG, et al. (2010). Pleasurable behaviors reduce stress via brain reward pathways. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:20529–34
  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Ostrander MM, Herman JP. (2011). HPA axis dampening by limited sucrose intake: reward frequency vs. caloric consumption. Physiol Behav 103:104–10
  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Ostrander MM, Thomas IM, Packard BA, Furay AR, Dolgas CM, Van Hooren DC, et al. (2007). Daily limited access to sweetened drink attenuates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis stress responses. Endocrinology 148:1823–34
  • Utter AC, Kang J, Nieman DC, Williams F, Robertson RJ, Henson DA, Davis JM, Butterworth DE. (1999). Effect of carbohydrate ingestion and hormonal responses on ratings of perceived exertion during prolonged cycling and running. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 80:92–9
  • Vallés A, Martí O, García A, Armario A. (2000). Single exposure to stressors causes long-lasting, stress-dependent reduction of food intake in rats. Am J Physiol Reg Int Comp Physiol 279:R1138–44
  • van Reedt Dortland AK, Giltay EJ, van Veen T, Zitman FG, Penninx BW. (2013a). Longitudinal relationship of depressive and anxiety symptoms with dyslipidemia and abdominal obesity. Psychosom Med 75:83–9
  • van Reedt Dortland AK, Vreeburg SA, Giltay EJ, Licht CM, Vogelzangs N, van Veen T, de Geus EJ, et al. (2013b). The impact of stress systems and lifestyle on dyslipidemia and obesity in anxiety and depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:209–18
  • Verwey M, Khoja Z, Stewart J, Amir S. (2007). Differential regulation of the expression of Period2 protein in the limbic forebrain and dorsomedial hypothalamus by daily limited access to highly palatable food in food-deprived and free-fed rats. Neuroscience 147:277–85
  • Viau V, Bingham B, Davis J, Lee P, Wong M. (2005). Gender and puberty interact on the stress-induced activation of parvocellular neurosecretory neurons and corticotropin-releasing hormone messenger ribonucleic acid expression in the rat. Endocrinology 146:137–46
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. (2003). The addicted human brain: insights from imaging studies. J Clin Invest 111:1444–51
  • Volkow ND, Wise RA. (2005). How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci 8:555–60
  • Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Logan J, Pappas NR, Wong CT, Zhu W, Netusil N, Fowler JS. (2001). Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet 357:354–7
  • Warne JP. (2009). Shaping the stress response: interplay of palatable food choices, glucocorticoids, insulin and abdominal obesity. Mol Cell Endocrinol 300:137–46
  • Weinstein SE, Shide DJ, Rolls BJ. (1997). Changes in food intake in response to stress in men and women: psychological factors. Appetite 28:7–18
  • Wiedenmann B, Franke WW. (1985). Identification and localization of synaptophysin, an integral membrane glycoprotein of Mr 38,000 characteristic of presynaptic vesicles. Cell 41:1017–28
  • Willner P, Benton D, Brown E, Cheeta S, Davies G, Morgan J, Morgan M. (1998). “Depression” increases “craving” for sweet rewards in animal and human models of depression and craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 136:272–83
  • Wilson ME, Fisher J, Fischer A, Lee V, Harris RB, Bartness TJ. (2008). Quantifying food intake in socially housed monkeys: social status effects on caloric consumption. Physiol Behav 94:586–94
  • Yamamoto T, Sako N, Sakai N, Iwafune A. (1997). Gustatory and visceral inputs to the amygdala of the rat: conditioned taste aversion and induction of c-fos-like immunoreactivity. Neurosci Lett 226:127–30
  • Yazbek SN, Spiezio SH, Nadeau JH, Buchner DA. (2010). Ancestral paternal genotype controls body weight and food intake for multiple generations. Hum Mol Genet 19:4134–44
  • Yoshida K, McCormack S, España RA, Crocker A, Scammell TE. (2006). Afferents to the orexin neurons of the rat brain. J Comp Neurol 494:845–61
  • Zeeni N, Daher C, Fromentin G, Tome D, Darcel N, Chaumontet C. (2013). A cafeteria diet modifies the response to chronic variable stress in rats. Stress 16:211–19
  • Zorrilla EP, Logrip ML, Koob GF. (2014). Corticotropin releasing factor: a key role in the neurobiology of addiction. Front Neuroendocrinol 35:234–44

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.