229
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Word-learning performance of children with and without cochlear implants given synchronous and asynchronous cues

&
Pages 777-790 | Received 09 Jun 2016, Accepted 14 Apr 2017, Published online: 19 May 2017

References

  • Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R., & Flom, R. (2004). Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 99–102.
  • Bergeson, T. R., Houston, D. M., & Miyamoto, R. (2010). Effects of congenital hearing loss and cochlear implantation on audiovisual speech perception in infants and children. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 157–165.
  • Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Davidson, L. S., Geers, A. E., & Nicholas, J. G. (2014). The effects of audibility and novel word learning ability on vocabulary level in children with cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants International, 15, 211–221.
  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, S., & Bates, E. (2006). The MacArthur Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
  • Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60, 1497–1510.
  • Gogate, L. J., & Bahrick, L. E. (1998). Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in seven-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 133–148.
  • Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., & Watson, J. D. (2000). A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development, 71, 878–894.
  • Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. H., & Betancourt, E. A. (2006). Attention to maternal multimodal naming by 6- to 8-month-old infants and learning of word-object relations. Infancy, 9, 259–288.
  • Heibeck, T. H., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Word learning in children: An examination of fast mapping. Child Development, 58, 1021–1034.
  • Houston, D. M., Beer, J., Bergeson, T., Chin, S. B., Pisoni, D., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2012). The ear is connected to the brain: Some new directions in the study of children with cochlear implants at Indiana University. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 23, 446–463.
  • Houston, D. M., Stewart, J., Moberly, A., Hollich, G., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2012). Word learning in deaf children with cochlear implants: Effects of early auditory experience. Developmental Science, 15, 448–461.
  • Houston-Price, C., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. (2005). ‘Word learning wizardry’ at 1;6. Journal of Child Language, 32, 175–189.
  • Kühn-Inacker, H., Weichbold, V., Tsiakpini, L., Coninx, S., & D’Haese, P. (2004). LittlEars: Auditory questionnaire. Innsbruck, Austria: MED-EL.
  • Landry, S., Levesque, J., & Champoux, F. (2012). Brain plasticity an obstacle for cochlear implant rehabilitation. The Hearing Journal, 65, 26–28.
  • Lund, E. (2016). Vocabulary knowledge of children with cochlear implants: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 21, 107–121.
  • Lund, E., & Schuele, C. M. (2015). Synchrony of maternal auditory and visual cues about unknown words to children with and without cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 36(2), 229–238.
  • Matatyaho, D. J., & Gogate, L. J. (2008). Type of maternal object motion during synchronous naming predicts preverbal infants’ learning of word-object relations. Infancy, 13, 172–184.
  • Matatyaho-Bullaro, D. J., Gogate, L. J., Mason, Z., Cadavid, S., & Abdel-Mottaleb, M. (2014). Type of object motion facilitates word mapping by preverbal infants. Journal of Experimental Child Pscyhology, 118, 27–40.
  • Singleton, N. C. (2012). Can semantic enrichment lead to naming in a word extension task? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21, 279–292.
  • Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 1558–1568.
  • Squires, J., & Bricker, D. (2009). The ages & stages questionnaire. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
  • Storkel, H. L. (2013). A corpus of consonant-vowel-consonant real words and nonwords: Comparison of phonotactic probability, neighborhood density, and consonant age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, 44. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0309-7
  • Tomblin, J. B., Barker, B. A., & Hubbs, S. (2007). Developmental constraints on language development in children with cochlear implants. International Journal of Audiology, 46, 512–523.
  • Walker, E., & McGregor, K. (2013). Word learning processes in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 375–387.
  • Wilstedt-Svensson, U., Lofqvist, A., Almqvist, B., & Birgitta, S. (2004). Is age at implant the only factor that counts? The influence of working memory on lexical and grammatical development in children with cochlear implants. International Journal of Audiology, 43, 506–515.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.