153
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Telephone Communication: Synthetic and Dysarthric Speech Intelligibility and Listener Preferences

, &
Pages 103-112 | Published online: 12 Jul 2009

References

  • Beliveau, C., Hodge, M., & Hagler, P. (1995). Effect of supplemental linguistic cues on the intelligibility of severely dysarthric speakers. Augmentative and Alterna-tive Communication, 11, 176–186.
  • Beukelman, D., Fager, S., Ullman, C., Hanson, E., & Logemann, J. (2002). The impact of speech supplementa-tion and clear speech on the intelligibility and speaking rate of people with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Medical Speech Language Pathology, 10, 237–242.
  • Borden, G., Harris, K., & Raphael, L. (2003). Speech Science Primer: Physiology, Acoustics, and Perception of Speech (4th edn.). Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.
  • Crabtree, M., Mirenda, P., & Beukelman, D. R. (1990). Age and gender preferences for synthetic and natural speech. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5, 256–261.
  • Darley, F. L., Aronson, A. S., & Brown, J. R. (1969). Differential diagnostic patterns of dysarthria. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 12, 246–269.
  • DePaul, R., & Kent, R. D. (2000). A longitudinal case study of ALS: Effects of listener familiarity and proficiency on intelligibility judgments. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 9, 230–240.
  • Drager, K. D. R., & Reichle, J. E. (2001a). Effects of age and divided attention on listeners' comprehension of synthe-sized speech. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 17, 109–119.
  • Drager, K. D. R., & Reichle, J. E. (2001b). Effects of discourse context on the intelligibility of synthesized speech for young adult and older adult listeners: Applications for AAC. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44, 1052–1057.
  • Duffy, S. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1992). Comprehension of synthetic speech produced by rule: A review and theoretical interpretation. Language and Speech, 35, 351–389.
  • Fucci, D., Reynolds, M. E., Bettagere, R., & Gonzales, M. D. (1995). Synthetic speech intelligibility under several experimental conditions. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 11, 113–117.
  • Garcia, J., & Cannito, M. (1996). Influence of verbal and nonverbal contexts on the sentence intelligibility of a speaker with dysarthria. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 750–760.
  • Garcia, J. M., & Dagenais, P. A. (1998). Dysarthric sentence intelligibility: Contribution of iconic gestures and mes-sage predictiveness. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1282–1293.
  • Hoover, J., Reichle, J., Van Tassell, D., & Cole, D. (1987). The intelligibility of synthesized speech: Echo II versus Votrax. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 425–431.
  • Hustad, K. C., & Beukelman, D. R. (2001). Effects of linguistic cues and stimulus cohesion on intelligibility of severely dysarthric speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 407–510.
  • Hustad, K. C., & Beukelman, D. R. (2002). Listener comprehension of severely dysarthric speech: Effects of linguistic cues and stimulus cohesion. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 545–558.
  • Hustad, K. C., & Cahill, M. A. (2003). Effects of presentation mode and repeated familiarization on intelligibility of dysarthric speech. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 12, 198–208.
  • Hustad, K. C., & Garcia, J. M. (2002). The influences of alphabet supplementation, iconic gestures, & predictive messages on intelligibility of a speaker with cerebral palsy. The Journal of Medical Speech Language Pathol-ogy, 10, 279–285.
  • Hustad, K. C., Beukelman, D. R., & Yorkston, K. M. (1998). Functional outcome assessment in dysarthria. Seminars in Speech and Language, 19, 291–302.
  • Hustad, K. C., Jones, T., & Dailey, S. (2003). Implementing speech supplementation strategies: Effects on intelligibil-ity and speech rate of individuals with chronic severe dysarthria. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 462–474.
  • Hustad, K. C., Kent, R. D., & Beukelman, D. R. (1998). DECTalk and MacinTalk speech synthesizers: Intellig-ibility differences for three listener groups. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 744–752.
  • Kent, R. (1993). Speech intelligibility and communicative competence in children. In A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing Children's Communication: Foundations for Intervention (Vol. 2, pp. 223–239). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
  • Kent, R. D., & Read, C. D. (2002). Acoustic Analysis of Speech (2nd edn). Albany, NY: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
  • Kent, R., Weismer, G., Kent, J., & Rosenbek, J. (1989). Toward phonetic intelligibility testing in dysarthria. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 482–499.
  • Koul, R. K., & Allen, G. D. (1993). Segmental intelligibility and speech interference thresholds of high-quality syn-thetic speech in presence of noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 790–798.
  • Luce, P., Feustel, T., & Pisoni, D. (1983). Capacity demands in short-term memory for synthetic and natural speech. Human Factors, 25(1), 17–32.
  • Marics, M. A., & Williges, B. H. (1988). The intelligibility of synthesized speech in data inquiry systems. Human Factors, 30, 719–732.
  • McNaughton, D., Fallon, K., Tod, J., Weiner, F., & Neisworth, J. (1994). Effect of repeated listening experi-ences on the intelligibility of synthesized speech. Aug-mentative and Alternative Communication, 10, 161–168.
  • Mirenda, P., & Beukelman, D. R. (1987). A comparison of speech synthesis intelligibility with listeners from three age groups. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 3, 120–128.
  • Mirenda, P., & Beukelman, D. R. (1990). A comparison of intelligibility among natural speech and seven speech synthesizers with listeners from three age groups. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 6, 61–68.
  • Mitchell, P., & Atkins, C. (1988). A comparison of the single word intelligibility of two voice output communication aids. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5(2), 84–88.
  • Nakamura, K., Arima, M., Sadamoto, A., & Toyota, R. (1993). Telephoning with a voice output device: Listener reactions. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 9, 251–258.
  • Nilsson, M., & Kleijn, W. B. (2001). Avoiding over-estimation in bandwidth extension of telephony speech. Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 2, 869–872.
  • Nilsson, M., Soli, S. D., & Sullivan, J. A. (1994). Develop-ment of the hearing in noise test for the measurement of speech recognition thresholds in quiet and in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95, 1085–1099.
  • Oshrin, S. E., & Siders, J. A. (1987). The effect of word predictability on the intelligibility of computer synthe-sized speech. Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 14, 89–90.
  • Ratcliff, A., Coughlin, S., & Lehman, M. (2002). Factors influencing ratings of speech naturalness in augmentative and alternative communication. Augmentative and Alter-native Communication, 18,11–19.
  • Riley, J. K., & Fries, B. (2000, August). "Hello, hello" Synthetic versus natural speech over the telephone. Poster session presented at the biennial conference of the International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication (ISAAC), Washington, DC.
  • Schwab, E. C., Nusbaum, H. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1985). Some effects of training on the perception of synthetic speech. Human Factors, 27, 395–408.
  • Silverman, F. H., & Schauer, S. L. (1996). Why do so few persons who are severely speech impaired for reasons other than deafness use telecommunication relay services? Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 12, 59.
  • Slowiaczek, L. M., & Nusbaum, H. C. (1985). Effects of speech rate and pitch contour on the perception of synthetic speech. Human Factors, 27, 701–712.
  • Tjaden, K. K., & Liss, J. M. (1995). The role of listener familiarity in the perception of dysarthric speech. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 9, 139–154.
  • Venkatagiri, H. S. (1994). Effect of sentence length and exposure on the intelligibility of synthesized speech. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 10, 96–104.
  • Yorkston, K., & Beukelman, D. (1980). A clinician-judged technique for quantifying dysarthric speech based on single-word intelligibility. Journal of Communication Disorders, 13, 15–31.
  • Yorkston, K. M., & Beukelman, D. R. (1983). The influence of judge familiarization with the speaker on dysarthric speech intelligibility. In W. Berry (Ed.), Clinical Dysar-thria (pp. 155–164). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Yorkston, K., Beukelman, D., & Tice, R. (1996). Sentence Intelligibility Test for Macintosh. Lincoln: Communica-tion Disorders Software. Distributed by Tice Technology Services, Lincoln, NE.
  • Yorkston, K., Strand, E., & Kennedy, M. (1996). Compre-hensibility of dysarthric speech: Implications for assess-ment and treatment planning. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 5, 55–66.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.