120
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Influence of graphic symbol use on reading comprehension

, &
Pages 269-281 | Published online: 12 Jul 2009

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Anderson, R., & Freebody, P. (1979). Vocabulary knowledge. (Technical Report No. 136). Urbana-Champaign, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
  • Beck, I., Perfetti, C., & McKeown, M. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading compre-hension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 506–521.
  • Berninger, V. W., & Gans, B. M. (1986a). Assessing word proc-essing capability of the nonvocal, nonwriting. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 2, 56–63.
  • Berninger, V. W., & Gans, B. M. (1986b). Language profiles in nonspeaking individuals of normal intelligence with severe cerebral palsy. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 2, 45–50.
  • Biemiller, A. (1970). The development of the use of graphic and contextual information as children learn to read. Reading Re-search Quarterly, 6,75–96.
  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1985). Spelling ability in congenital dysarthria: Evidence against articulatory coding in translating between phonemes and graphemes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 229–251.
  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Robson, J. (1989). Accurate non-word spell-ing despite congenital inability to speak: Phoneme-grapheme conversion does not require subvocal articulation. British Jour-nal of Psychology, 80, 1–13.
  • Bishop, K., Rankin, J., & Mirenda, P. (1994). Impact of graphic symbol use on reading acquisition. Augmentative and Alterna-tive Communication, 10, 113–125.
  • Blechman, B. (1984). Language analysis skills and early reading acquisition. In G. Wallach & K. Butler (Eds.), Language learn-ing disabilities in school-age children (pp. 271–287). Balti-more: Williams & Wilkins.
  • Bohannon, J. N., Warren-Leubecker, A., & Hepler, N. (1984). Word order awareness and early reading. Child Development, 55,1541–1548.
  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learn-ing to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.
  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 313–321.
  • Carlson, F. (1985). Picsyms categorical dictionary. Lawrence, KS: Baggeboda Press.
  • Ehri, L. C. (1991). Development of the ability to read words. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.), Hand-book of reading research: Vol II (pp. 383–417). New York: Longman.
  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1985). Movement into reading: Is the first stage of printed word learning visual or phonetic? Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 163–179.
  • Ferguson, N. (1975). Pictographs and prereading skills. Child Development, 46, 786–789.
  • Foley, B. E. (1993). The development of literacy in individuals with severe congenital speech and motor impairments. Topics in Language Disorders, /3(2), 16–32.
  • Fuller, D., Lloyd, L., & Schlosser, R. (1992). Further development of an augmentative and alternative communication symbol taxonomy. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 8, 67–74.
  • Harris, A. J., & Sipay, E. R. (1990). How to increase reading ability (9th ed.). White Plains, NY: Longman.
  • Harris, T. L., & Hodges, R. E. (Eds.). (1981). A dictionary of reading and related terms. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
  • Hehner, B. (1980). Blissymbols for use. Toronto: Easter Seal Communication Institute.
  • Herber, H. (1978). Teaching reading in content areas (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Hillocks Jr., G., & Ludlow, L. H. (1984). A taxonomy of skills in reading and interpretation of fiction. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 7–24.
  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking: From childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books. Johnson, R. (1981). The picture communication symbols book: Book I. Solana Beach, CA: Mayer-Johnson Co.
  • Johnson, R. (1985). The picture communication symbols book: Book II. Solana Beach, CA: Mayer-Johnson Co.
  • Johnson, R. (1992). The picture communication symbols book: Book III. Solana Beach, CA: Mayer-Johnson Co.
  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1987). The psychology of reading and language comprehension. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
  • Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53,316–327.
  • Keeton, A. (1977). Children's cognitive integration and memory processes for comprehending written sentences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 459–471.
  • Ledger, G. W., & Ryan, E. B. (1982). The effects of semantic integration training on recall for pictographic sentences. Jour-nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33,39–54.
  • Lorge, I., & Chall, J. (1963). Estimating the size of vocabularies of children and adults: An analysis of methodological issues. Journal of Experimental Education, 32, 147–157.
  • Mann, V., Shankweiler, D., & Smith, S. T. (1984). The association between comprehension of spoken sentences and early read-ing ability: The role of phonetic representation. Journal of Child Language, 11, 627–643.
  • McNaughton, S. (1993). Graphic representational systems and literacy learning. Topics in Language Disorders, /3(2), 58–75.
  • Menyuk, P., & Flood, J. (1981). Linguistic competence, reading, writing problems and remediation. Bulletin of the Orton Soci-ety, 31, 13–28.
  • Mezynski, K. (1983). Issues concerning the acquisition of knowl-edge: Effects of vocabulary training on reading comprehen-sion. Review of Educational Research, 53,253–279.
  • Miller, G. A. (1951). Language and communication. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Musselwhite, C. R., & St. Louis, K.W. (1988). Communication programming for persons with severe handicaps (2nd ed.). Boston: College-Hill.
  • Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.
  • Nesdale, A. R., Pratt, C., & Tunmer, W. E. (1985). Young chil-dren's detection of propositional inconsistencies in oral com-munications. Australian Journal of Psychology, 37,289–296.
  • Nesdale, A. R., Tunmer, W. E., & Clover, J. (1985). Factors influencing young children's ability to detect logical inconsis-tencies in oral communications. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 39–49.
  • Patterson, C. J., O'Brien, C., Kister, M. C., Carter, D. B., & Kotsonis, M. E. (1981). Development of comprehension moni-toring as a function of context. Developmental Psychology, 17, 379–389.
  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford Univer-sity Press.
  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319.
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. W. (1975). The relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67,461–479.
  • Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1989). Higher-order language processes and reading disabilities. In A. G. Kamhi & H. W. Catts (Eds.), Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective (pp. 159–197). Boston: College Hill Press.
  • Ruddell, R. B. (1978). Developing comprehension abilities: Impli-cations from research for an instructional framework. In S. J. Samuels (Ed.), What research has to say about reading in-struction (pp. 108–120). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
  • Ryan, E. B., & Ledger, G. W. (1984). Learning to attend to sentence structure: Links between metalinguistic development and reading. In J. Downing, & R. Valtin (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 149–171). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Ryan, E. B., Ledger, G. W., & Robine, D. M. (1984). Effects of semantic integration training on the recall for pictographic sentences of kindergarten and first grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76,387–398.
  • Sawyer, D. J. (1992). Language abilities, reading acquisition, and developmental dyslexia: A discussion of hypothetical and observed relationships. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 82–95.
  • Schuele, C. M., & VanKleeck, A. (1987). Precursors to literacy: Assessment and intervention. Topics in Language Disorders, 7(2), 32–44.
  • Shibles, B. H. (1959). How many words does a first grade child know? Elementary English, 31, 42–47.
  • Smith, M. E. (1926). An investigation of the development of the sentence and the extent of vocabulary in young children. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 3(5), 92.
  • Smith, M. M. (1989). Reading without speech: A study of children with cerebral palsy. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 10, 601–614.
  • Smith, M. M. (1992). Reading abilities of nonspeaking students: Two case studies. Augmentative and Alternative Communica-tion, 8, 57–66.
  • Smith, N. B. (1972). The quest for increased reading compe-tency. In H. H. Klein (Ed.), The quest for competency in teach-ing reading (pp. 45–56). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
  • Smith, R. J., & Barrett, T. C. (1974). Teaching reading in the middle grades. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Snyder, L. S., & Downey, D. M. (1991). The language-reading relationship in normal and reading-disabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(1), 129–140.
  • Stackhouse, J. (1990). Phonological deficits in developmental reading and spelling disorders. In P. Grunwell (Ed.), Develop-mental speech disorders (pp. 163–182). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some con-sequences of individual differences in the acquisition of liter-acy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–406.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Word recognition: Changing perspec-tives. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research: Vol II (pp. 418–452). New York: Longman.
  • Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A., & Freeman, D. (1984). Rela-tion between early reading acquisition and word decoding with and without context: A longitudinal study of first-grade chil-dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 668–677.
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1979). Mechanisms of sentence context effects in reading: Automatic activation and conscious attention. Memory & Cognition, 7,77–85.
  • Thorndike, R. L. (1973). Reading comprehension education in fifteen countries. New York: Wiley.
  • Tierney, R. J., & Cunningham, J. W. (1984). Research on teach-ing reading comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research: Vol. '(pp. 609–641). New York: Longman.
  • Tunmer, W. E. (1990). The role of language prediction skills in beginning reading. New Zealand Journal of Educational Stud-ies, 25, 95–114.
  • Tunmer, W. E., & Cole, P. G. (1985). Learning to read: A meta-linguistic act. In C. Simon (Ed.), Communication skills and classroom success (pp. 293–312). San Diego: College Hill Press.
  • Tunmer, W. E., & Herriman, M. L. (1984). The development of metalinguistic awareness: A conceptual overview. In W. E. Tunmer, C. Pratt, & M. L. Herriman (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children: Theory, research and implications (pp. 12–35). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Tunmer, W. E., Herriman, M. L., & Nesdale, A. R. (1988). Meta-linguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 134–158.
  • Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Pratt, C. (1983). The develop-ment of young children's awareness of logical inconsistencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36,97–108.
  • Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Wright, A. D. (1987). Syntactic awareness and reading acquisition. British Journal of Devel-opmental Psychology, 5,25–34.
  • Tunmer, W. E., & Hoover W. A. (1992). Cognitive and linguistic factors in learning to read. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1982). Verbal processing in poor and normal readers. In C. J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.). Verbal processes in children (pp. 189–264). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Viorst, J. (1972). Alexander and the terrrible, horrible, no good, very bad day. New York: Scholastic Inc.
  • Weber, R. (1970). First graders' use of grammatical context in reading. In H. Levin & J. Williams (Eds.), Basic studies in reading. New York: Basic Books.
  • Yoder, D. E., & Koppenhaver, D. A. (1993). Literacy learning and persons with severe speech impairments [Special issue]. Top-ics in Language Disorders, 13(2).

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.