105
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Response

Six good reasons why understanding religion requires a multidisciplinary approach: response to commentators

References

  • Aiello, L. C. (1996). Hominine preadaptations for language and cognition. In P. Mellars & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Modelling the early human mind (pp. 89–99). McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
  • Andreoni, J., & Croson, R. (2008). Partners versus strangers: Random rematching in public goods experiments. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results, Vol. 1, pp. 776–783. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1574-0722(07)00082-0
  • Bannan, N., Bamford, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2023). The evolution of gender dimorphism in the human voice: the role of octave equivalence. Current Anthropology (in press).
  • Barrio, R., Govezensky, T., Dunbar, R. I. M., Iñiguez, G., & Kaski, K. (2015). Dynamics of deceptive interactions in social networks. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 12(112), 20150798. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2015.0798
  • Bettridge, C. M. (2010). Reconstructing australopithecine socioecology: Strategic modelling based on modern primates [DPhil dissertation]. University of Oxford.
  • Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., Codding, B. F., & Zeanah, D. W. (2019). Variability in the organization and size of hunter-gatherer groups: Foragers do not live in small-scale societies. Journal of Human Evolution, 131(1), 96–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.03.005
  • Charles, S., von Mulukom, V., Saraswati, A., Watts, F., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Farias, M. (2023). Bending and bonding: A randomized controlled trial on the socio-psychological effects of spiritual vs secular yoga practice on social bonding. Current Psychology (in press).
  • Dal Pesco, F., & Fischer, J. (2020). On the evolution of baboon greeting rituals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375B(1805), 20190420. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0420
  • Dàvid-Barrett, T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Bipedality and hair loss in human evolution revisited: the impact of altitude and activity scheduling. Journal of Human Evolution, 94(1), 72–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.02.006
  • DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A., & Higham, J. P. (2017). Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(5), 5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0112
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–694. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014a). Human evolution. Pelican Press.
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014b). Why only humans have language. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The prehistory of language (pp. 12–35). Oxford University Press.
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018a). Social structure as a strategy to mitigate the costs of group-living: a comparison of gelada and guereza monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 136(1), 53–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.005
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018b). The anatomy of friendship. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 32–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2020a). Structure and function in human and primate social networks: Implications for diffusion, network stability and health. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 476A(2240), 20200446. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspa.2020.0446
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2020b). Religiosity and religious attendance as factors in wellbeing and social engagement. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 11(1), 17–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2020.1712618
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2021). Virtual touch and the human social world. Current Opinion in Behavioral Science, 44(1), 14–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.06.009
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2022a). Managing the stresses of group-living in the transition to village life. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 4, e40. doi:10.1017/ehs.2022.39
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2022b). Laughter and its role in the evolution of human social bonding. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, 289B, 20210176. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0176
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Clark, A., & Hurst, N. L. (1995). Conflict and cooperation among the Vikings: Contingent behavioral decisions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(3), 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(95)00022-D
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Frangou, A., Grainger, F., & Pearce, E. (2021). Laughter influences social bonding but not prosocial generosity to friends and strangers. PLoS One, 16(8), e0256229. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256229
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Korstjens, A. H., & Lehmann, J. (2009). Time as an ecological constraint. Biological Reviews, 84(3), 413–429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00080.x
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., MacCarron, P., & Shultz, S. (2018). Primate social group sizes exhibit a regular scaling pattern with natural attractors. Biology Letters, 14(1), 20170490. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2017.0490
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2017). Why are there so many explanations for primate brain evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, 372B(1727), 201602244. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0244
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2021a). The infertility trap: The fertility costs of group-living in mammalian social evolution. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 634664. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2021.634664
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2021b). Social complexity and the fractal structure of group size in primate social evolution. Biological Reviews, 96(5), 1889–1906. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12730
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. & Shultz, S. (2023). Four errors and a fallacy: pitfalls for the unwary in comparative brain analyses. (under review).
  • Eliade, M. (1985). A history of religious ideas, Vols. 1-3. Blackwells.
  • Fischer, R., Callander, R., Reddish, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation? An experimental field study comparing nine ritual types. Human Nature, 24(2), 115–125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9167-y
  • Hamilton, M. J., Milne, B. T., Walker, R. S., Burger, O., & Brown, J. H. (2007). The complex structure of hunter-gatherer social networks. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 274B(1622), 2195–2202. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.0564
  • Henrich, J., Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilová, J. & Purzycki, B. G. (2019). War increases religiosity.  Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 129–135. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0512-3
  • Hill, R. A., Bentley, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biology Letters, 4(6), 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0393
  • Huxley, J. S. (1914). The courtship-habits of the Great Crested Greebe (Podiceps cristatus); with an addition to the theory of sexual selection. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 84(3), 491–562. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1914.tb07052.x
  • Johnson, A. W., & Earle, T. K. (2001). The evolution of human societies: From foraging group to agrarian state (2nd ed.). Stanford University Press.
  • Kirk, R. (1994). Raw feeling: A philosophical account of the essence of consciousness. Oxford University Press.
  • Kuntoro, I. A., Saraswati, L., Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2013). Micro-cultural influences on theory of mind development: A comparative study of middle-class and pemulung children in Jakarta, Indonesia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(3), 266–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413478258
  • Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Fission-fusion social systems as a strategy for coping with ecological constraints: A primate case. Evolutionary Ecology, 21(5), 613–634. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-006-9141-9
  • Machin, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2011). The brain opioid theory of social attachment: A review of the evidence. Behaviour, 148(9-10), 985–1025. https://doi.org/10.1163/000579511X596624
  • Madsen, E., Tunney, R., Fieldman, G., Plotkin, H., Dunbar, R. I. M., Richardson, J., & McFarland, D. (2007). Kinship and altruism: a cross-cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 339–359. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X129213
  • Nummenmaa, L., Tuominen, L., Dunbar, R. I. M., Hirvonen, J., Manninen, S., Arponen, E., Machin, A., Hari, R., Jääskeläinen, I. P., & Sams, M. (2016). Reinforcing social bonds by touching modulates endogenous µ-opioid system activity in humans. NeuroImage, 138, 242–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.063
  • Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., & Vallbo, A. (2010). The neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 185–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.011
  • O’Reilly, J., & Peterson, C. C. (2014). Scaling theory of mind development in indigenous- and Anglo-Australian toddlers and older children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(9), 1489–1501. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022114542285
  • Pearce, E., Shuttleworth, A., Grove, M. & Layton, R. (2014). The costs of being a high latitude hominin. In R. Dunbar, C. Gamble & J. Gowlett, (Eds), Lucy to language: The benchmark papers (pp. 356-379). Oxford University Press.
  • Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2017). Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 114(20), 5300–5305. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1700712114
  • Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: A replication study. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 4(4), 400–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-018-0101-8
  • Prewitt-Freilino, J. L., Caswell, T. A., & Laakso, E. K. (2012). The gendering of language: A comparison of gender equality in countries with gendered, natural gender, and genderless languages. Sex Roles, 66(3-4), 268–281. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0083-5
  • Reeves, B. O. (1978). Head-Smashed-In: 5500 years of bison jumping in the Alberta Plains. Plains Anthropologist, 23(82), 151–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/2052546.1978.11908913
  • Schirmer, A., & McGlone, F. (Eds.). 2022. Affective touch. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 43(special issue).
  • Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1239–1247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023899
  • Shultz, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2022). Socioecological complexity in primate groups and its cognitive correlates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1860), 20210296. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0296
  • Slaughter, V., & Perez-Zapata, D. (2014). Cultural variations in the development of mind Reading. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 237–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12091
  • Smith, E. A. (1991). Inujjuamiunt foraging strategies: Evolutionary ecology of an Arctic hunting economy. Transaction Publishers.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.