1,528
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Effects of Total and Partial Sleep Deprivation on Reflection Impulsivity and Risk-Taking in Deliberative Decision-Making

ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon show all
Pages 309-324 | Published online: 27 May 2020

References

  • Sleep and sleep disorders. Centers for disease, control and prevention; 2015. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/sleep/index.html. Accessed May 11, 2020.
  • Walker MP. Why We Sleep. London, UK: Penguin; 2017.
  • Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997;20(4):267–277.
  • Krause AJ, Simon EB, Mander BA, et al. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18(7):404–418. doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.55
  • Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6(3):236–249. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.6.3.236
  • Killgore WD, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ. Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(1):7–13. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00487.x
  • McKenna BS, Dickinson DL, Orff HJ, Drummond SPA. The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. J Sleep Res. 2007;16(3):245–252. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00591.x
  • Venkatraman V, Huettel SA, Chuah LY, Payne JW, Chee MWL. Sleep deprivation biases the neural mechanisms underlying economic preferences. J Neurosci. 2011;31(10):3712–3718. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4407-10.2011
  • Acheson A, Richards JB, de Wit H. Effects of sleep deprivation on impulsive behaviors in men and women. Physiol Behav. 2007;91(5):579–587. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.03.020
  • Killgore WD. Effects of sleep deprivation on morningness-eveningness traits on risk-taking. Psychol Rep. 2007;100(2):613–626. doi:10.2466/pr0.100.2.613-626
  • Yang H, Carmon Z, Kahn B, et al. The hot–cold decision triangle: a framework for healthier choices. Mark Lett. 2012;23:457–472. doi:10.1007/s11002-012-9179-0
  • Damasio AR. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam; 1994.
  • Shafir E, Simonson I, Tversky A. Reason-based choice. Cognition. 1993;49(1–2):11–36. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034
  • Seguin JR, Arseneault L, Tremblay RE. The contribution of “cool” and “hot” components of decision-making in adolescence: implications for developmental psychopathology. Cogn Dev. 2007;22(4):530–543. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.006
  • Jackson ML, Gunzelmann G, Whitney P, et al. Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17(3):215–225. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2012.06.007
  • Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2008.
  • Dickinson DL, Drummond SPA. The effects of total sleep deprivation on Bayesian updating. Judgm Decis Mak. 2008;3:181–190.
  • Anderson C, Dickinson DL. Bargaining and trust: the effects of 36-h total sleep deprivation on socially interactive decisions. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:54–63. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00767.x
  • Dickinson DL, McElroy T. Sleep restriction and circadian effects on social decisions. Eur Econ Rev. 2017;97:57–71. doi:10.1016/j.euroecorev.2017.05.002
  • Güroğlu B, van den Bos W, Rombouts SA, Crone EA. Unfair? It depends: neural correlates of fairness in social context. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2010;5(4):414–423. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq013
  • Chang LJ, Sanfey AG. Great expectations: neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision-making. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8(3):277–284. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr094
  • Lauriola M, Weller J. Personality and risk: beyond daredevils-risk taking from a temperament perspective. In: Raue M, Lermer E, Streicher B, editors. Psychological Perspectives on Risk and Risk Analysis: Theory, Models, and Applications. Cham: Springer; 2018:3–36. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-92478-6_1
  • Levin I, Xue G, Weller J, Reimann M, Lauriola M, Bechara A. A neuropsychological approach to understanding risk-taking for potential gains and losses. Front Neurosci. 2012;6:15. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00015
  • Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G, Hinson JM, Van Dongen HP. Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 2010;33(1):47–57. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.1.47
  • Whitney P, Hinson JM. Measurement of cognition in studies of sleep deprivation. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:37–48. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00003-8
  • Huq SF, Garety PA, Hemsley DR. Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1988;40(4):801–812. doi:10.1080/14640748808402300
  • Averbeck BB, Evans S, Chouhan V, Bristow E, Shergill SS. Probabilistic learning and inference in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;127(1–3):115–122. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.009
  • Djamshidian A, O’Sullivan SS, Sanotsky Y, et al. Decision making, impulsivity, and addictions: do parkinson’s disease patients jump to conclusions? Mov Disord. 2012;27(9):1137–1145. doi:10.1002/mds.25105
  • Ross RM, McKay R, Coltheart M, Langdon R. Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(5):1183–1191. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu187
  • Banca P, Lange I, Worbe Y, et al. Reflection impulsivity in binge drinking: behavioural and volumetric correlates. Addict Biol. 2016;21(2):504–515. doi:10.1111/adb.12227
  • Figner B, MacKinlay R, Wilkenin F, Weber EU. Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: age differences in risk taking in the Columbia card task. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009;35(3):709–730. doi:10.1037/a0014983
  • Markiewicz L, Kubińska E. Information use differences in hot and cold risk processing: when does information about probability count in the Columbia card task? Front Psychol. 2015;6:1727. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01727
  • Womack SD, Hook JN, Reyna SH, Ramos M. Sleep loss and risk-taking behavior: a review of the literature. Behav Sleep Med. 2013;11(5):343–359. doi:10.1080/15402002.2012.703628
  • Killgore WD. Sleep deprivation and behavioral risk-taking. In: Watson RR, editor. Modulation of Sleep by Obesity, Diabetes, Age, and Diet. London, UK: Academic Press; 2015:279–287. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-420168-2.00030-2
  • Panno A, Lauriola M, Figner B. Emotion regulation and risk taking: predicting risky choice in deliberative decision making. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(2):326–334. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.707642
  • Lowe CJ, Safati A, Hall PA. The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:586–604. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.010
  • Reynolds AC, Banks S. Total sleep deprivation, chronic sleep restriction and sleep disruption. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:91–103. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00006-3
  • Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4
  • Curcio G, Tempesta D, Scarlata S, et al. Validity of the Italian version of the pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI). Neurol Sci. 2013;34(4):511–519. doi:10.1007/s10072-012-1085-y
  • Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2011;2(4):297–307. doi:10.1016/s1389-9457(00)00065-4
  • Castronovo V, Galbiati A, Marelli S, et al. Validation study of the Italian version of the insomnia severity index (ISI). Neurol Sci. 2016;37(9):1517–1524. doi:10.1007/s10072-016-2620-z
  • Ghisi M, Flebus G, Montano A, Sanavio E, Sica C. Beck Depression Inventory-II: Edizione Italiana. Firenze, IT: Giunti Editore; 2006.
  • Moroni L, Bettinardi O, Vidotto G, et al. Anxiety and depression short scale: norms for its use in rehabilitation. Monaldi Arch Chest Dis. 2006;66:255–263. doi:10.4081/monaldi.2006.516
  • Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci. 1990;52(1–2):29–37. doi:10.3109/00207459008994241
  • Stern RA, Arruda JE, Hooper CR, Wolfner GD, Morey CE. Visual analogue mood scales to measure internal mood state in neurologically impaired patients: description and initial validity evidence. Aphasiol. 1997;11:59–71. doi:10.1080/02687039708248455
  • Clark L, Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Sahakian BJ. Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(5):515–522. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.007
  • Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69(4):680–694. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020
  • Djamshidian A, Sanotsky Y, Matviyenko Y, et al. Increased reflection impulsivity in patients with ephedrone-induced parkinsonism. Addiction. 2013;108(4):771–779. doi:10.1111/add.12080
  • Voon V, Irvine MA, Derbyshire K. Measuring “waiting” impulsivity in substance addictions and binge eating disorder in a novel analogue of rodent serial reaction time task. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(2):148–155. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.013
  • Garety PA, Freeman D, Jolley S, et al. Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(3):373–384. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.373
  • Jacoby RJ, Abramowitz JS, Buck BE, Fabricant LE. How is the beads task related to intolerance of uncertainty in anxiety disorders? J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(6):495–503. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.05.005
  • Jacoby RJ, Abramowitz JS, Reuman L, Blakey SM. Enhancing the ecological validity of the beads task as a behavioral measure of intolerance of uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;41:43–49. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.003
  • Figner B, Weber EU. Who takes risks when and why? Determinants of risk taking. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20(4):211–216. doi:10.1177/0963721411415790
  • Holper L, Murphy RO. Hemodynamic and affective correlates assessed during performance on the Columbia Card Task (CCT). Brain Imaging Behav. 2014;8(4):517–530. doi:10.1007/s11682-013-9265-9
  • Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (3.5.1). Computer Software. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018.
  • Luke SG. Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behav Res Methods. 2017;49(4):1494–1502. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y
  • Te Lindert BHW, Van Someren EJW. Sleep estimates using microelectromechanical systems (MEMS). Sleep. 2013;36(5):781–789. doi:10.5665/sleep.2648.7
  • Pilcher JJ, Huffcutt AI. Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep. 1996;19(4):318–326. doi:10.1093/sleep/19.4.318
  • Evans JSBT. Thinking Twice: Two Minds in One Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2010.
  • Kahneman D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2011.
  • Stanovich KE. Rationality and the Reflective Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011.
  • Evans JSBT, Stanovich KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8(3):223–241. doi:10.1177/1745691612460685
  • Ross RM, Pennycook G, McKay R, Gervais WM, Langdon R, Coltheart M. Analytic cognitive style, not delusional ideation, predicts data gathering in a large beads task study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2016;21(4):300–314. doi:10.1080/13546805.2016.1192025
  • Brosnan M, Hollinworth M, Antoniadou K, Lewton M. Is empathizing intuitive and systemizing deliberative? Pers Individ Differ. 2014;66:39–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid
  • Smith ME, McEvoy LK, Gevins A. The impact of moderate sleep loss on neurophysiological signals during working-memory task performance. Sleep. 2002;25:784–794. doi:10.1093/sleep/25.7.56
  • Chee MWL, Chuah YML, Venkatraman V, Chan WY, Philip P, Dinges D. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. NeuroImage. 2006;31:419–428. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.001
  • Frenda SJ, Fenn KM. Sleep less, think worse: the effect of sleep deprivation on working memory. JARMAC. 2016;5(4):463–469. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.10.001
  • Gosselin D, De Koninck J, Campbell K. Novel measures to assess the effects of partial sleep deprivation on sensory, working, and permanent memory. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1607. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01607
  • Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Stricker JL, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Gillin JC. Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport. 1999;10:3745–3748. doi:10.1097/00001756-199912160-00004
  • Garety P, Joyce E, Jolley S, et al. Neuropsychological functioning and jumping to conclusions in delusions. Schizophr Res. 2013;150(2–3):570–574. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.035
  • Krężołek M, Pionke R, Banaszak B, Kokoszka A, Gawęda Ł. The relationship between jumping to conclusions and neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2019;273:443–449. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.035
  • Esslinger C, Braun U, Schirmbeck F, et al. Activation of midbrain and ventral striatal regions implicates salience processing during a modified beads task. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58536. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058536
  • Voon V, Chang-Webb YC, Morris LS, et al. Waiting impulsivity: the influence of acute methylphenidate and feedback. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;19(1):pyv074. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyv074
  • Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, et al. Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2001;21(2):RC121. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-j0001.2001
  • Volkow ND, Tomasi D, Wang GJ, et al. Evidence that sleep deprivation downregulates dopamine D2R in ventral striatum in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2012;32(19):6711–6717. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0045-12.2012
  • Whitney P, Hinson JM, Nusbaum AT. A dynamic attentional control framework for understanding sleep deprivation effects on cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2019;246:111–126. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.03.015
  • Rausch F, Mier D, Eifler S, et al. Reduced activation in the ventral striatum during probabilistic decision-making in patients in an at-risk mental state. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40(3):163–173. doi:10.1503/jpn.140191
  • Reeve S, Emsley R, Sheaves B, Freeman D. Disrupting sleep: the effects of sleep loss on psychotic experiences tested in an experimental study with mediation analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(3):662–671. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx103
  • Venkatraman V, Chuah YM, Huettel SA, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep. 2007;30(5):603–609. doi:10.1093/sleep/30.5.603
  • Buelow MT. Predicting performance on the Columbia card task: effects of personality characteristics, mood, and executive functions. Assessment. 2015;22(2):178–187. doi:10.1177/1073191114539383
  • Penolazzi B, Gremigni P, Russo PM. Impulsivity and reward sensitivity differentially influence affective and deliberative risky decision making. Pers Individ Dif. 2012;53(5):655–659. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.018
  • Ferrara M, Bottasso A, Tempesta D, Carrieri M, De Gennaro L, Ponti G. Gender differences in sleep deprivation effects on risk and inequality aversion: evidence from an economic experiment. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0120029. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120029
  • Greer SM, Goldstein AN, Knutson B, Walker MP. A genetic polymorphism of the human dopamine transporter determines the impact of sleep deprivation on brain responses to rewards and punishments. J Cogn Neurosci. 2016;28(6):803–810. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00939
  • Smith DV, Hayden BY, Truong TK, Song AW, Platt ML, Huettel SA. Distinct value signals in anterior and posterior ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30(7):2490–2495. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3319-09.2010
  • Libedinsky C, Smith DV, Teng CS, et al. Sleep deprivation alters valuation signals in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011;5:70. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00070
  • Menz MM, Buchel C, Peters J. Sleep deprivation is associated with attenuated parametric valuation and control signals in the midbrain during value-based decision making. J Neurosci. 2012;32(20):6937–6946. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3553-11.2012