944
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Role of sentence-final particles and prosody in irony comprehension in Cantonese-speaking children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorders

, , &
Pages 18-32 | Received 04 Jun 2012, Accepted 25 Sep 2012, Published online: 13 Dec 2012

References

  • Ackerman, B. P. (1982). Contextual integration and utterance interpretation: The ability of children and adults to interpret sarcastic utterances. Child Development, 53, 1075–1083.
  • Ackerman, B. P. (1983). Form and function in children's understanding of ironic utterances. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 487–508.
  • Ackerman, B. P. (1986). Children's sensitivity to comprehension failure in interpreting a nonliteral use of an utterance. Child Development, 57, 485–497.
  • Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Infantino, M. G. (2000). Irony as a game of implicitness: Acoustic profiles of ironic communication. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 275–311.
  • Attardo, S. J., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I. (2003). Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. Humor-International Journal of Humor Research, 16, 243–260.
  • Barbe, K. (1995). Irony in context. New York: John Benjamins.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
  • Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of mind” in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–895.
  • Bryant, G., & Fox-Tree, J. (2005). Is there an ironic tone of voice? Language & Speech, 48, 257–277.
  • Capelli, C. A., Nakagawa, N., & Madden, C. M. (1990). How children understand sarcasm: The role of context and intonation. Child Development, 61, 1824–1841.
  • Chan, M. K. M. (1996). Gender-marked speech in Cantonese: The case of sentence-final particles je and jek. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 26, 1–38.
  • Chan, M. K. M. (2001). Gender-related use of sentence-final particles in Cantonese. In M. Hellinger & H. Bussmann (Eds.), Gender across languages: The linguistic representation of women and men (pp. 57–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Co.
  • Chao, Y. R. (1930). A system of tone-letters. Le Maître Phonétique, 45, 24–27.
  • Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
  • Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2009). Acoustic markers of sarcasm in Cantonese and English. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 126, 1394–1405.
  • Cheung, K. H. (1986). The phonology of present-day Cantonese. Doctoral Dissertation, University College, London.
  • Creusere, M. A. (2000). A developmental test of theoretical perspectives on the understanding of verbal irony: Children's recognition of allusion and pragmatic insincerity. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 15, 29–45.
  • Culter, A. (1974). On saying what you mean without meaning what you say. In M. Lagaly, R. Fox, & A. Bruck (Eds.), Papers from the Tenth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 117–127). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.
  • Culter, A., & Isard, S. D. (1980). The production of prosody. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production, Vol. 1. (pp. 245–269). London: Academic Press.
  • Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). Inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 47–54.
  • Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1997). Attribution meaning to deliberately false remarks: The case of irony. In C. Mandell & A. McCabe (Eds.), The problem of meaning: Behavioral and cognitive perspective (pp. 377–414). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Dews, S., Winner, E., Kaplan, J., Rosenblatt, E., Hunt, M., Lim, K., McGovern, A., Qualter, A., & Smarsh, B. (1996). Children's understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony. Child Development, 67, 3071–3085.
  • Fox, A., Luke, K. K., & Nancarrow, O. (2008). Aspects of intonation in Cantonese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 36, 321–367.
  • Fung, R. S. Y. (2000). Final particles in standard Cantonese: Semantic extension and pragmatic inference. PhD dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.
  • Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L. (2001). The risks and rewards of ironic communication. In L. Anolli, R. Ciceri, & G. Riva (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 181–194). Amsterdam: IOS press.
  • Green, H., & Tobin, Y. (2009). Prosodic analysis is difficult … but worth it: A study in high functioning autism. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 308–315.
  • Hancock, J. T., Dunham. J., & Purdy, K. (2000). Children's comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 227–248.
  • Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119.
  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.
  • Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66 (3), 843–855.
  • Imaizumi, S., Furuya, I., & Yamasaki, K. (2009). Voice as a tool communicating intentions. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 34, 196–199.
  • Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 29–42.
  • Klin, A. (1992). Listening preferences in regard to speech in four children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 763–769.
  • Kreuz, R. J. (1996). The use of verbal irony: Cues and constraints. In J. S. Mio & A. N. Katz (Eds.), Metaphor: Implications and applications (pp. 23–38). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Kreuz, R. J., & Caucci, G. M. (2009). Social aspects of verbal irony use. In H. Pishwa (Ed.), Language and social cognition: Expression of the social mind (pp. 325–346). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1995). Two cues for verbal irony: Hyperbole and the ironic tone of voice. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 21–31.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science, 8, F1–F12.
  • Kwok, H. (1984). Sentence particles in Cantonese. Hong Kong: Centre of Asian Studies, The University of Hong Kong.
  • Laval, V., & Bert-Erboul, A. (2005). French-speaking children's understanding of sarcasm: The role of intonation and context. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 610–620.
  • Law, S. P. (1990). The syntax and phonology of Cantonese sentence-final particles. Doctoral Dissertation, Boston University, Boston, MA.
  • Lee, H. T., & Law, A. (2001). Epistemic modality and the acquisition of Cantonese final particles. In M. Nakayama (Ed.), Issues in East Asian language acquisition (pp. 67–128). Tokyo: Kuroshio Publishers.
  • Lee, H. T., Wong, C. H., Leung, C. S., Man, P., Cheung, P., Szeto, K., & Wong, C. S. P. (1996). The development of grammatical competence in Cantonese-speaking children: Report of Hong Kong RGC. Ear-marked grant, 1991–1994. Hong Kong: RGC.
  • Lee, T. H., & Yiu, C. (1998). Final “de” and “ge3” – a nominalization analysis for cleft sentences in Mandarin and Cantonese. Paper presented at the 1998 Annual Research Forum, Linguistic Society of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Polytechnic University.
  • Lee, T. H., & Yiu, C. (1999). Temporal marking, verbalization and nominalization in the Cantonese particles ‘lei4’ and ‘ge3’. Paper presented at the Annual Research Forum of the Linguistic Society of Hong Kong, Chinese University of Hong Kong.
  • Leech, G. (1983). Principles of pragmatics. London: Longman.
  • Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a meta-representational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.
  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
  • Loveland, K., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Brelsford, K., & Ortegon, J. (1995). Intermodal perception of affect by persons with autism or Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 409–418.
  • Luke, K. K. (1990). Utterance particles in Cantonese conversation. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.
  • Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (1994). Cantonese: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge.
  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic children: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1106.
  • Papafragou, A. (1998). The acquisition of modality: Implications for theories of semantic representation. Mind and Language, 13, 370–399.
  • Peppé, S. (2011). Assessment of prosodic ability in atypical populations with special reference to high-functioning autism. In V. Stojanovik, & J. Setter (Eds.), Speech prosody in atypical populations: Assessment and remediation (pp. 1–23). Guildford, UK: J&R Press Ltd.
  • Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O'Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50, 1015–1028.
  • Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 689–700.
  • Pike, K. L. (1945). The intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
  • Pike, K. L. (1948). Tone languages. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
  • Reed, T., & Peterson, C. (1990). A comparative study of autistic subjects' performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 555–568.
  • Rockwell, P. (2000). Lower, slower, louder: Vocal cues of sarcasm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 483–95.
  • Sperber, D. (1984). Verbal irony: Pretense or echoic mention? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 130–165.
  • Swettenham, J. (1996). Can children with autism be taught to understand false belief using computers? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 157–165.
  • Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Gomez, J. C., & Walsh, S. (1996). What's inside a person's head? Conceiving of the mind as a camera helps children with autism develop an alternative theory of mind. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 73–88.
  • Tang, S. W. (1998). Parametrization of features in syntax. PhD Dissertation, University of California, Irvine.
  • T'sou, B., Lee, H. T., Tung, P., Man, Y., Chan, A., To, C. K. S., Cheung, P. S. P., Ng, A., & Chan, Y. (2006). Hong Kong Cantonese Oral Language Assessment Scale. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong.
  • Utsumi, A. (2000). A verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 33, 1777–1806.
  • Wakefield, J. (2010). The English equivalents of Cantonese sentence-final particles: A contrastive analysis. Doctoral Dissertation, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong.
  • Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129, 932–943.
  • Winner, E., & Leekam, S. (1991). Distinguishing irony from deception: Understanding the speaker's second-order intention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 257–270.
  • Winner, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and the constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
  • Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.