344
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Non-word repetition in 2-year-olds: Replication of an adapted paradigm and a useful methodological extension

, , &
Pages 523-535 | Received 24 Aug 2014, Accepted 11 Mar 2015, Published online: 20 Apr 2015

References

  • Archibald, L. (2008). The promise of nonword repetition as a clinical tool. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 32, 21–28
  • Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173
  • Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary (3rd ed.). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications
  • Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 627–652
  • Chiat, S. (2006). The developmental trajectory of nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 552–556
  • Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 429–443
  • Coady, J., & Evans, J. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 1–40
  • Conlin, J., & Gathercole, S. (2006). Lexicality and interference in working memory in children and in adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 363–380
  • Dollaghan, C., & Campbell T. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146
  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, D. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Publishing/Pearson Assessments
  • Ellis Weismer, S., & Edwards, S. (2006). The role of phonological storage deficits in specific language impairment: A reconsideration. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 556–562
  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, S., & Bates, E. (2006). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes
  • Gathercole, S. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543
  • Gathercole, S., & Adams, A. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770–778
  • Gathercole, S., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., & the ALSPAC team (2005). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short-term memory function in childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598–611
  • Graf Estes, K., Evans, J., & Else-Quest, N. (2007). Differences in nonword repetitions performance of children with and without specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 177–195
  • Hakim, H., & Bernstein Ratner, N. (2004). Nonword repetition abilities of children who stutter: An exploratory study. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29, 179–199
  • Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K. (2008). Non-word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language, 35, 903–916
  • Ibertsson, T., Willstedt-Svensson, U., Radeborg, K., & Sahlen, B. (2008). A methodological contribution to the assessment of nonword repetition – a comparison between children with specific language impairment and hearing-impaired children with hearing aids or cochlear implants. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 33, 168–178
  • Jones, G., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. (2007). Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words. Developmental Science, 10, 853–873
  • Jusczyk, P., Luce, W., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630–645
  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  • Majerus, S., & Boukebza, C. (2013). Short-term memory for serial order supports vocabulary development: New evidence from a novel word learning paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 811–828
  • Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., & Peters, F. (2004). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 297–306
  • Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 95–119
  • Marton, K. (2006). Do repetition errors in children with specific language impairment reflect a weakness in an unidentified skill specific to nonword repetition or a deficit in simultaneous processing? Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 569–573
  • Metsala, J., & Chisholm, G. (2010). The influence of lexical status and neighborhood density on children’s nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 489–506
  • Montgomery, J., & Evans, J. (2006). Commentary on keynote. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 573–577
  • Munson, B., Swenson, C., & Manthei, S. (2005). Lexical and phonological organization in children: Evidence from repetition tasks. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 108–124
  • Polisenska, K., & Kapalkova, S. (2014). Improving child compliance on a computer-administered nonword repetition task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 1060–1068
  • Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword repetition: The relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and reading impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 683–694
  • Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2- to 4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 223–234
  • Schwartz, R., Leonard, L., Frome Loeb, D., & Swanson, L. (1987). Attempted sounds are sometimes not: An expended view of phonological selection and avoidance. Journal of Child Language, 14, 411–418
  • Shriberg, L., Austin, D., Lewis, B., McSweeny, J., & Wilson, D. (1997). The percentage of consonants correct (PCC) metric: Extensions and reliability data. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 708–722
  • Shriberg, L., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders III: A procedure for assessing severity of involvement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 256–270
  • Shriberg, L., Kwiatkowski, J., & Hoffman, K. (1984). A procedure for phonetic transcription by consensus. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 456–465
  • Shriberg, L., Lohmeier, H., Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Green, J., & Moore, C. (2009). A nonword repetition task for speakers with misarticulations: The syllable repetition task (SRT). Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 1189–1212
  • Smith, B. (2006). Precautions regarding nonword repetition tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 584–587
  • Storkel, H. (2004). Do children acquire dense neighborhoods? An investigation of similarity neighborhoods in lexical acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 201–221
  • Storkel, H., Maekawa, J., & Hoover, J. (2010). Differentiating the effects of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on vocabulary comprehension and production: A comparison of preschool children with versus without phonological delays. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 933–949
  • Stokes, S., & Klee, T. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy of a new test of early nonword repetition for differentiating late talking and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 872–882
  • Stokes, S., Moran, C., & George, A. (2013). Nonword repetition and vocabulary use intoddlers. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 224–237
  • Thorn, A., & Frankish, C. (2005). Long-term knowledge effects on serial recall of nonwords are not exclusively lexical. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 729–735
  • Vitevitch, M. (2002). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 735–747
  • Vitevitch, M. (2006). Manipulating the characteristics of words and nonwords to better understand word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 594–598
  • Vitevitch, M.S., & Luce, P.A. (2004). A web-based interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 481–487
  • Vitevitch, M., Luce, P., Charles-Luce, J., & Kemmerer, D. (1997). Phonotactics and syllable stress: Implications for the processing of spoken nonsense words. Language and Speech, 40, 47–62
  • Vitevitch, M., Luce, P., Pisoni, D., & Auer, E. (1999). Phonotactics, neighborhood activation, and lexical access for spoken words. Brain and Language, 68, 306–311

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.