Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 27, 2010 - Issue 2
589
Views
30
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Papers

SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTIONS OF THE EFFECTS OF SUSTAINED PERFORMANCE UNDER SLEEP-DEPRIVATION CONDITIONS

, &
Pages 318-333 | Received 29 May 2009, Accepted 24 Sep 2009, Published online: 06 Apr 2010

REFERENCES

  • Åkerstedt T, Kecklund G, Axelsson J. (2008). Effects of context on sleepiness self-ratings during repeated partial sleep deprivation. Chronobiol. Int. 25:271–278.
  • Dinges DF, Kribbs NB, Steinber KN, Powell JW. (1992). Do we lose the willingness to perform during sleep deprivation? Sleep Res. 21:318.
  • Dinges DF, Gillen Powell JW, Pack F, Williams KS, Kribbs NB, Orne EC, Carlin MC, Ott GW, Rosekind MR, Orne MT, Pack AI. (1995). Mood reports during total and partial sleep deprivation: Is anger inevitable? Sleep Res. 24:441.
  • Dorrian J, Lamond N, Dawson D. (2000). The ability to self-monitor performance when fatigued. J. Sleep Res. 3:138–144.
  • Fischer FM, Nagai R, Teixeira LR. (2008). Explaining sleep duration in adolescents: The impact of socio-demographic and lifestyle factors and working status. Chronobiol. Int. 25:359–372.
  • Gander P, Millar M, Webster C, Merry A. (2008). Sleep loss and performance of anaesthesia trainees and specialists. Chronobiol. Int. 25:1077–1091.
  • Grier RA, Warm JS, Dember WN, Matthews G, Galinsky TL. (2003). The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Hum. Factors 45:349–359.
  • Helton WS, Warm JS. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychol. 129:18–25.
  • Horne JA, Pettitt AN. (1985). High incentive effects on vigilance performance during 72 hours of total sleep deprivation. Acta Psychol. 58:123–139.
  • Jaffe MP, Smokensky MH, Wun CC. (1996). Sleep quality and physical and social well-being in North American petrochemical shift workers. South Med. J. 89:305–312.
  • Jones CB, Dorrian J, Jay SM, Lamond N, Ferguson S, Dawson D. (2006). Self-awareness of impairment and the decision to drive after an extended period of wakefulness. Chronobiol. Int. 23:1253–1263.
  • Kane MJ, Engle RW. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 9:637–671.
  • Kjellberg A. (1977). Sleep deprivation and some aspects of performance: I. Problems of arousal changes. Waking Sleeping 1:139–143.
  • Lamond N, Dawson S, Roach GD. (2005). Fatigue assessment in the field: Validation of a hand-held electronic psychomotor vigilance task. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 76:486–489.
  • Lamond N, Jay SM, Dorrian J, Ferguson SA, Roach GD, Dawson D. (2008). The sensitivity of palm-based psychomotor vigilance task to severe sleep loss. Behav. Res. Methods 40:347–352.
  • Loh S, Lamond N, Dorrian J. (2004). The validity of psychomotor vigilance tasks of less than 10-minute duration. Behav. Res. Methods 36:339–346.
  • Lorist MM. (2008). Impact of top-down control during mental fatigue. Brain Res. 1232:113–123.
  • Manly T, Robertson IH, Galloway M, Hawkins K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia 37:661–670.
  • Matthews G, Campbell SE, Falconer S, Joyner LA, Huggins J, Gilliland K, Grier R, Warm JS. (2002). Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: Task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion 2: 315–340.
  • McClelland LE, Pilcher JJ. (2007). Assessing subjective sleepiness during a night of sleep deprivation: Examining the internal state and behavioral dimensions of sleepiness. Behav. Med. 33:17–26.
  • Monk TH. (1989). A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect. Psychiatry Res. 27:89–99.
  • O'Hare D. (1997). Cognitive ability determinants of elite pilot performance. Hum. Factors 39:540–552.
  • Pattyn N, Neyt X, Hendrickx D, Soetens E. (2008). Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiol. Behav. 93:369–378.
  • Phillips B, Magan L, Gerhardstein C, Cecil B. (1991). Shift work, sleep quality, and worker health: A study of police officers. South Med. J. 84:1176–1184.
  • Pilcher JJ, Huffcutt AI. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep 19:318–326.
  • Pilcher JJ, Odle-Dusseau HN. (2005). Impact on self-reported sleepiness, performance, effort, and motivation. In Kushida C. (ed.). Sleep deprivation: Clinical issues, pharmacology, and sleep loss effects. New York: Marcel Dekker, 251–260.
  • Pilcher JJ, Walters A. (1997). How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables related to college students’ cognitive performance. J. Am. Coll. Health 46:121–126.
  • Pilcher JJ, Band D, Odle-Dusseau HN, Muth ER. (2007). Human performance under sleep deprivation conditions: A model of task engagement. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 78( Suppl.):B15–B24.
  • Portaluppi F, Touitou Y, Smolensky MH. (2008). Ethical and methodological standards for laboratory and medical biological rhythm research. Chronobiol. Int. 25:999–1016.
  • Reinberg A, Ashkenazi I. (2008). Internal desychronization of circadian rhythms and tolerance to shift work. Chronobiol. Int. 25:625–643.
  • Roach GD, Dawson D, Lamond N. (2006). Can a shorter psychomotor vigilance task be used as a reasonable substitute for the ten-minute psychomotor vigilance task? Chronobiol. Int. 23:1379–1387.
  • Roscoe SN, Corl L, LaRoche J. (2001). Predicting human performance. 5th ed. Saint Laurent, QC, Canada, Helio Press.
  • Steyvers FJ. (1987). The influence of sleep deprivation and knowledge of results on perceptual encoding. Acta Psychol. 66:173–187.
  • Steyvers FJ, Gaillard AW. (1993). The effects of sleep deprivation and incentives on human performance. Psychol. Res. Psychol. Forsch. 55:64–70.
  • Walker AD, Muth ER, Odle-Dusseau HN, Moore DW, Pilcher JJ. (2009). The effects of 28 hours of sleep deprivation on respiratory sinus arrhythmia during tasks with low and high controlled attention demands. Psychophysiology 46:217–224.
  • Waugh CE, Gotlib IH. (2008). Motivation for rewards as a function of required effort: Dissociating the ‘liking’ from the ‘wanting’ system in humans. Motiv. Emotion 32:323–330.
  • Wever RA. (1988). Order and disorder in human circadian rhythmicity: Possible relations to mental disorders. In Kupfer DJ, Monk TH, Barchas JD (eds.). Biological rhythms and mental disorders. New York: Guilford Press, 253–346.
  • Wilkinson RT. (1964). Effects of up to 60 hours’ sleep deprivation on different types of work. Ergonomics 7:175–186.
  • Zijlstra FR. (1993). Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. Doctoral dissertation. Delft, The Netherlands: Delft University Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.