Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 29, 2012 - Issue 1
1,366
Views
48
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Effects of Sleep Loss and Circadian Rhythm on Executive Inhibitory Control in the Stroop and Simon Tasks

, , &
Pages 55-61 | Received 04 Aug 2011, Accepted 05 Oct 2011, Published online: 04 Jan 2012

REFERENCES

  • Åkerstedt T, Gillberg M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 52:29–37.
  • Blatter K, Cajochen C. (2007). Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiol. Behav. 90:196–208.
  • Blatter K, Opwis K, Münch M, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. (2005). Sleep loss related decrements in planning performance in healthy elderly depend on task difficulty. J. Sleep Res. 14:409–417.
  • Bratzke D, Rolke B, Ulrich R, Peters M. (2007). Central slowing during the night. Psychol. Sci. 18:456–461.
  • Bratzke D, Rolke B, Steinborn MB, Ulrich R. (2009). The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task switching efficiency. J. Sleep Res. 18:167–172.
  • Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28:193–213.
  • Cain SW, Silva EJ, Chang A-M, Ronda JM, Duffy, JF. (2011). One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 76:37–42.
  • Cajochen C, Knoblauch V, Kräuchi K, Renz C, Wirz-Justice A. (2001). Dynamics of frontal EEG activity, sleepiness and body temperature under high and low sleep pressure. Neuroreport 12:2277–2281.
  • Cousineau D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method. Tut. Quan. Methods Psychol. 1:42–45.
  • Dorrian J, Rogers NL, Dinges, DF. (2005). Psychomotor vigilance performance: neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In Kushida CA (ed.). Sleep deprivation. New York: Marcel Dekker, 39–70.
  • Drummond SPA, Paulus, MP, Tapert SF. (2006). Effects of two nights sleep deprivation and two nights recovery sleep on response inhibition. J. Sleep Res. 15:261–265.
  • Duffy JF, Dijk DJ. (2002). Getting through to circadian oscillators: why use constant routines? J. Biol. Rhythms 17:4–13.
  • Durmer JS, Dinges DF. (2005). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin. Neurol. 25:117–129.
  • Fafrowicz M, Oginska H, Mojsa-Kaja J, Marek T, Golonka K, Tucholska K. (2010). Chronic sleep deficit and performance of a sustained attention task—an electrooculography study. Chronobiol. Int. 27:934–944.
  • Harrison Y, Horne JA. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 6:236–249.
  • Hartley LR, Shirley E. (1976). Colour-name interference at different times of day. J. Appl. Psychol. 61:119–122.
  • Henik A, Salo R. (2004). Schizophrenia and the Stroop effect. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3:42–59.
  • Hommel B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychol. 136:189–202.
  • Jasper I, Roenneberg T, Häußler A, Zierdt A, Marquardt C, Hermsdörfer J. (2010). Circadian rhythm in force tracking and in dual task costs. Chronobiol. Int. 27:653–673.
  • Jennings JR, Monk TH, Van Der Molen MW. (2003). Sleep deprivation influences some but not all processes of supervisory attention. Psychol. Sci. 14:473–479.
  • Killgore WDS, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ. (2006). Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. Sleep Res. 15:7–13.
  • Liu X, Banich MT, Jacobson BL, Tanabe JL. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 22:1097–1106.
  • MacLeod CM. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109:163–203.
  • Marek T, Fafrowicz, Golonka K, Mojsa-Kaja J, Oginska H, Tucholska K, Urbanik A, Beldzik E, Domagalik A. (2010). Diurnal patterns of activity of the orienting and excutive attention neuronal networks in subjects performing a Stoop-like task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Chronobiol. Int. 27:945–958.
  • May CP, Hasher L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24:363–379.
  • McKenna BS, Dickinson DL, Orff HJ, Drummond S. (2007). The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known risk and ambiguous risk decisions. J. Sleep Res. 16:245–252.
  • Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41:49–100.
  • Peterson BS, Kane MJ, Alexander GM, Lacadie C, Skudlarski P, Leung HC, May J, Gore JC. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Cogn. Brain Res. 13:427–440.
  • Portaluppi F, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y. (2010). Ethics and methods for biological rhythm research on animals and human beings. Chronobiol. Int. 27:1911–1929.
  • Pratte MS, Rouder JN, Morey RD, Feng C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72:2013–2025.
  • Ridderinkhof KR. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In Prinz W, Hommel B (eds.). Attention & Performance: Vol. 19. Common mechanisms in perception and action. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 494–519.
  • Riedel M, Berrez S, Pelisse D, Brousse E, Forget C, Marlot M, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y, Reinberg A. (2010). 24-Hour pattern of work-related injury risk of French fireman: nocturnal peak time. Chronobiol. Int. 28:697–705.
  • Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. (2003). Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms. 18:80–90.
  • Sagaspe P, Sanchez-Ortuno M, Charles A, Taillard J, Valtat C, Bioulac B, Philip P. (2006). Effects of sleep deprivation on color-word, emotional, and specific stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain Cogn. 60:76–87.
  • Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, Peigneux P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24:755–789.
  • Steinborn MB, Bratzke D, Rolke B, Gordijn MCM, Beersma DGM, Ulrich R. (2010). The effect of 40 hours of constant wakefulness on number comparison performance. Chronobiol. Int. 27:807–825.
  • Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G., Holcomb H, Mayberg H, Dannals R, Wagner H , Thorne D, Popp K, Rowland L, Welsh A, Balwinski S, Redmond D. (2000). Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J. Sleep Res. 9:335–352.
  • Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G, Hinson JM, Van Dongen HPA. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep 33:47–57.
  • Van Dongen H, Dinges DF. (2003). Investigating the interaction between the homeostatic and circadian processes of sleep–wake regulation for the prediction of waking neurobehavioural performance. J. Sleep Res. 12:181–187.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.