352
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Affect and Sexual Responsivity in Men With and Without a History of Sexual Aggression

, ORCID Icon, , &
 

Abstract

Despite increased attention to understanding risk factors for sexual aggression, knowledge regarding the emotional and sexual arousal patterns of sexually aggressive men remains limited. The current study examined whether sexually aggressive men exhibit unique profiles of affective responsivity, in particular to negatively valenced stimuli, as well as sexual arousal patterns that differentiate them from nonaggressive men. We presented 78 young men (38 sexually aggressive; 40 nonaggressive) with a series of videos designed to induce positive, sad, or anxious affect. Affect and subjective sexual arousal were assessed following each film and erectile responses were measured continuously. Sexually aggressive men reported significantly higher levels of sexual arousal following both the positive and negative conditions as compared to nonaggressive men. Erectile responses of sexually aggressive men were significantly greater than nonaggressive men’s following the positive affect induction. Self-reported positive affect, but not negative affect, was a significant predictor of subjective sexual arousal for both groups of men. Compared to nonaggressive men, sexually aggressive men showed significantly weaker correlations between subjective and physiological sexual arousal. Findings suggest that generalized heightened propensity for sexual arousal may be a risk factor for sexually aggressive behavior.

Funding and Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant (R21HD055831; Zoe Peterson, PI) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) at the National Institutes of Health. This article’s contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NICHD. The authors would like to thank George Adair, Cam Brown, Marie Danh, Jodie Fisher, Jessica Kershaw, Neil Maxwell, Ian SerVaas, and Heather Sperry for their assistance with participant recruitment and data collection.

Additional information

Funding

This research was supported by a grant (R21HD055831; Zoe Peterson, PI) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) at the National Institutes of Health. This article’s contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NICHD. The authors would like to thank George Adair, Cam Brown, Marie Danh, Jodie Fisher, Jessica Kershaw, Neil Maxwell, Ian SerVaas, and Heather Sperry for their assistance with participant recruitment and data collection.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.