243
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Compound reading in Hebrew text-based neglect dyslexia: The effects of the first word on the second word and of the second on the first

&
 

Abstract

In many Hebrew compounds, which are two-word phrases, the first word is marked morphophonologically, and often also orthographically, as the head of the compound. Because Hebrew is read from right to left, this allowed us to ask whether a right-hand word that is marked orthographically as a compound-head, and hence signals that another word is expected, causes readers with text-based neglect to continue shifting attention to the left and read the second word. We also asked whether the second, left-hand, word affects the reading of the first word. The effect of the second word was assessed in a condition in which the second word semantically disambiguated the first word, a biased heterophonic homograph, and a condition in which the second word formed a compound with the first and hence required reading the first in the morphophonological form of a compound-head. The two participants were Hebrew-speaking men with acquired left text-level neglect dyslexia, without word-based neglect dyslexia. They read 294 two-word compounds and control phrases, composed of five conditions that assessed the effect of the first word on the second word, and of the second on the first.

The results indicated that morphosyntax modulates reading in neglect dyslexia. When the first, right-hand, word included an orthographic cue indicating that a second word follows, fewer words on the left were omitted than when no such cue existed. The second word, however, did not affect the reading of the first, and the first word was read as if the patients did not look ahead to the second.

We are grateful to Claudio Luzzatti for thinking together about this research and for making this study actually happen. We thank Lilach Khentov-Kraus for her thoughtful comments, Aviva Fattal-Valevski for her help with reading the MRI scans, and Ilil Dayan and Noga Balaban for the detection of Micha's neglect and his initial assessment.

This research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation [grant number 1296/06, Friedmann] and by the Australian Research Council (ARC) Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders (CCD), Macquarie University.

Notes

1 The phonological change on the head of the construct results from lack of stress, which is caused by the fact that the head and its complement constitute a prosodic word with one primary stress, on the complement (Doron & Meir, Citation2013). Some nouns do not undergo the morphophonological change, for phonological reasons, and in these cases, the free and construct forms of the noun are identical.

2 For example, instead of reading חתולת רחוב, xatulat rexov, “female-cat-of street”, he read “חתול”, xatul, cat—he omitted the second word, street, and hence could not stay with the head in its construct form and changed the female cat to a masculine cat, which is a free form.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.