5,141
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Possible selves, strategies and perceived likelihood among adolescents in Hong Kong: desire and concern

&
Pages 135-149 | Received 24 Nov 2014, Accepted 17 Mar 2015, Published online: 23 Apr 2015

Abstract

Possible selves provide a new perspective and a systematic way to learn about adolescents' envisioning of their own future. Literature shows some patterns of possible selves are universal worldwide while some are sensitive to context and culture. This study adapted the open-ended Possible Selves Questionnaire into Chinese and explored the general patterns of possible selves among adolescents in Chinese context. 3078 secondary school students in Hong Kong were surveyed about their possible selves and demographic factors. It is found that adolescents have more career- and school-related hoped-for possible selves and have more drug and risky behaviour feared possible selves. Adolescents have less strategy, especially concrete strategy for their possible selves. Gender and age differences were examined and potential contextual and cultural influences were discussed.

Introduction

As possible selves reflect adolescents' envisioning about their future and serve the motivation and self-regulation function (Hoyle & Sherrill, Citation2006; Markus & Nurius, Citation1986), they become an important psychological construct in adolescence studies. The general pattern of possible selves provides rich information about what people envision as their hoped-for future and feared future, and what strategies they have to attain their hoped-for possible selves (HPS) and to avoid feared possible selves (FPS). Because possible selves may be different across age, gender, ethnicity and cultural context (Consedine, Sabag-Cohen, & Krivoshekova, Citation2007; Cross & Markus, Citation1991; Kao, Citation2000; Knox, Funk, Elliott, & Bush, Citation2000), studying the general pattern of possible selves is a fundamental step of applying possible selves in further research and practice to a new group of people. By far most of these studies were conducted in Western societies. Only a few of them focus on Asian and none was done among Hong Kong adolescents. In Hong Kong, adolescents are facing a lot of challenges, such as higher unemployment rate (Hong Kong Special Aministration Region Government, Citation2012), educational system changes (Curriculum Development Council Hong Kong, Citation2013; Education and Manpower Bureau Hong Kong, Citation2005), and more affordable and easily accessible illicit drugs (Narcotics Division Security Bureau H.K.S.A.R., Citation2010, Citation2013). Studying the general pattern of possible selves among adolescents in Hong Kong provides a new perspective and a systematic way to understand youths' thoughts so as to provide better education and social work service to young people in modern Hong Kong society.

Adolescents and possible selves

During adolescence, people explore their possibilities and develop a sense of self (Erikson, Citation1968). Undergoing fast physical and psychological changes, adolescents start to imagine what they will be. Also gaining increasing self-efficacy, adolescents could consider more possible futures for themselves (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, Citation2001). When adolescents look forward to and imagine their future, they are establishing their possible selves.

Possible selves consist of three parts: what one hopes to be (hoped-for selves), what one expects to be (expected selves) and what one fears to be (feared selves) (Markus & Nurius, Citation1986). Possible selves serve as motivational link between self-identity and behaviours (Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, Citation2006; Oyserman, Fryberg, & Yoder, Citation2007). They are not only ‘pulling’ one toward the positive selves but also ‘dragging’ one against the negative selves. In addition, this future orientation in self-system acts as a self-regulatory function (Hoyle & Sherrill, Citation2006). These functions are based on what possible selves people have and the quality of the strategies they devise (Oyserman et al., Citation2006, Citation2007). For example, if one has a HPS as a university student and has plans in detail to attain this possible self, he/she would be more likely to work harder at school (Oyserman et al., Citation2006).

The content of possible selves shows the direction of attempts while the strategy relates the practice effort that attaining possible selves may require. Some studies explored the pattern of possible selves among different groups of middle school students. Knox et al. (Citation2000) found that adolescents in the United States had more career-related and interpersonal possible selves with mainly white or Caucasian sample. Fryberg and Markus (Citation2003) found American Indians had fewer possible selves and fewer categories of possible selves than European Americans. American Indians had more possible selves in achievement and fewer in interpersonal relations. Leondari, Gonida, and Gialamas (Citation2009) found that the most frequently mentioned HPS of Hellenic adolescents were career, family and material goods, while the most referred to feared selves were career and health issues. Briones Tabernero, and Arenas (Citation2011) found in a sample comprising 938 secondary school students (64.4% Spanish, 19.7% South American and 12.8% African) that most future concerns were related to a satisfactory job, family and economic situation. Some other research focused on various groups of adolescents, such as youth with delinquent behaviours (Oyserman & Markus, Citation1990), young fathers in prison (Meek, Citation2007), students with disability and without disability (Jenkins, Citation1999) or new teachers (Hamman, Gosselin, Romano, & Bunuan, Citation2010). The findings of these studies reflected that the formation of possible selves is closely related to the social context (Oyserman, Ager, & Ager, Citation1995).

In addition to the content and strategy of possible selves, the gender and age differences provide detailed information of the pattern of possible selves. With regard to gender, Knox et al. (Citation2000) explored differences in 14 categories of possible selves among high school students. They found that girls expressed more FPS related to relational functioning, whereas boys generated more FPS related to occupation, general failure and inferiority. They did not find gender differences in content and likelihood of HPS (Knox et al., Citation2000). However, Brown and Diekman (Citation2010) found that genders differed in their distant possible selves in terms of family and career domains. Lips (Citation2004) found larger gender differences in possible selves content of high school students than university students and girls having more possible selves in humanities and less in business. These studies reflected that gender differences exist in possible selves in different domains. However, these gender differences were not consistent among different age and different groups of people. Thus, examining gender difference along with exploring the pattern of possible selves can enrich the understanding of possible selves’ patterns.

With regard to age differences of possible selves, some of the existing studies focused on the difference across life stages (Cross & Markus, Citation1991; Hooker, Citation1992; Ryff, Citation1991) and demonstrated that possible selves would change according to different developmental tasks during life span. Most of the existing research focused on possible selves within one age stage, such as adolescence (e.g., Leondari et al., Citation2009; Oyserman & Fryberg, Citation2006), adulthood (Lee & Oyserman, Citation2007), and old age (e.g., Bardach, Gayer, Clinkinbeard, Zanjani, & Watkins, Citation2010; Waid & Frazier, Citation2003). However, there is little research that explored the changes within one life stage. As young people experience dramatic physical and psychological changes in adolescence, their ability to plan and visualise future may increase steadily from early to late adolescence (Boyd & Bee, Citation2012; Weinberger, Elvevåg, & Giedd, Citation2005). We predict that adolescents' possible selves, its strategies and its likelihood significantly increase through early, middle .

To the knowledge of the authors, published empirical study investigating the possible selves among Hong Kong Chinese adolescents is rare. This study has two preliminary aims: to translate Possible Selves Questionnaire into Chinese and adapt it into Hong Kong Chinese adolescent culture; to collect general information about the pattern of possible selves. We have three research questions. First, what are the content and the number of possible selves, strategies and perceived likelihood among Hong Kong adolescents? Second, how does gender affect the content and the number of possible selves, strategies and perceived likelihood? Last, what is the effect of age on the above-mentioned characteristics of possible selves?

Methods

This study has two phases. Phase one was to adapt Possible Selves Questionnaire (Oyserman, Bybee, Terry, & Hart-Johnson, Citation2004; Oyserman & Markus, Citation1990) into Chinese (PSQ_C). A rigorous Back Translation and Target Language Test Method (Maneesriwongul & Dixon, Citation2004) was used. The process included six steps: (1) double translated by two translators; (2) collected feedback from translation panel and revised; (3) conducted pilot study with 30 secondary school students; (4) revised questionnaires and collected feedback from translation panel and Youth Advisory Group; (5) revised and completed second pilot study with 10 additional secondary students; and (6) confirmed the final version. The translation panel was made up of two professors, a doctoral student and a research assistant. They provided sufficient support in terms of understanding of the theory on possible selves and checking the accuracy of use of language during the translation process. The Youth Advisory Group included four young people who gave feedback on the questionnaire's clarity and the layout from youth's perspectives.

Phase two study, part of a larger study by the Hong Kong Anti-drug Addiction Community Consortium, was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee for Non-Clinical Faculties at The University of Hong Kong. Letters were sent to parents describing the study and its voluntary nature. Parents wishing to exclude their child were asked to sign and return a form. On the day of data collection, students whose parents had not excluded them were asked to assent to participate and again told that the study was voluntary and responses were anonymous.

Sampling procedure

Demographically speaking, the selected district is a good representative of the 18 city districts in Hong Kong (Hong Kong Census and Statistics Department, Citation2006). According to the Census and Statistics Department Report (Hong Kong Census and Statistics Department, Citation2006), the demographic figures of this district were around the medium among all districts in terms of gender ratio, age proportion and school attendance rate. Recruitment strategies used randomised, stratified sampling method. The school types, location and participants' gender were taken into consideration. As such, sampling schools in this district could reflect the general school adolescents in Hong Kong. Eleven (73%) of the 15 secondary schools in the district agreed (the others did not differ on available characteristics – school size and the neighbourhood where they are situated). A total of 3078 students in 99 classes across the 11 schools participated (6–10 classrooms per school, about 30 students per class).Footnote1 The number of participants represented around 10% of the youth population in the district in Hong Kong and provided enough power for relevant statistical analysis.

Data collection procedure

All data were collected in 2010 in classrooms of 25–30 students supervised by a research assistant. Data were collected through self-administered pencil-and-paper questionnaire. Students were given booklets, which took 15–20 min to complete and began with questions about substance use (the focus of the larger study), followed by parental support questions, possible self and self-esteem, and demographics questions.

Measures

Possible selves. PSQ_C measures two sets of HPS and two sets of FPS. Instructions and questionnaire matrices were adopted from Oyserman and colleagues (Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, Citation2002; Oyserman & Markus, Citation1990). Students read: ‘Each of us has some images or pictures in mind of what we will be like or what we want to avoid being like in the future. Think about your coming future years – imagine what you hope to be like and write down two HPS below’ ‘For each hoped-for self, mark yes or no in the column labelled have some plan to attain that HPS and write in what you plan to do.’ For each of possible selves students wrote, they were also asked to rate how likely it was that the possible self would be attained, using a 5-point scale (1 = low likelihood, 5 = high likelihood). Same questions and structures were used to measure FPS.

As PSQ_C is an open-ended questionnaire, the following procedure was performed to explore the validity feature of the instrument. First, face validity and content validity were supported by the affirming from the translation panel, youth advisory group and participants of pilot studies. They agreed that the content and design of PSQ_C is designed to gauge respondents' perceived possible selves and on the whole the questionnaire measures the variables it claims to measure (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, Citation1996; Kwan, Hui, & McGee, Citation2010). Second, the convergent validity was examined (Ervin & Stryker, Citation2001). As previous research showed that the perceived likelihood of possible selves was closely related with global self-esteem (Knox et al., Citation2000; Markus & Nurius, Citation1986; Oyserman & Markus, Citation1990), the convergent validity of PSQ could be examined by comparing the perceived likelihood of possible selves with global self-esteem (personal communication with Daphna Oyserman, December 30, 2009 and November 18, 2013). The applicability of Rosenberg's Self-esteem Scale to Chinese was also examined (Cai, Brown, Deng, & Oakes, Citation2007). In this study, the convergent validity of PSQ_C was established by finding a moderate correlation between the mean of perceived likelihood and the scores on Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (r = .32, p < .001).

Demographics. Age (M = 14.8 years, SD = 1.8 years), gender (53% boys, 46% girls, 1% who did not report their gender) and monthly allowance were obtained from their parents. According to the developmental stages (Compas, Davis, Forsythe, & Wagner, Citation1987), about 50% participants (n = 1531) were in early adolescence (12–14 years old), about 41% participants (n = 1,230) were in middle adolescence (15–17 years old) and about 9% participants (n = 279) were in late adolescence (18–20 years old). Monthly allowance ranged from 1 = none, to 8 = more than HK$3,000 (US$400) using a scale adapted from Currie et al. (Citation1997).Footnote2 Monthly allowance is a measure of the financial resources children receive from parents (Ridge, Citation2002) and has been used as a proxy for family economic support (Shah, Syeda, & Bhatti, Citation2012).

Analysis

Descriptive analysis was used to answer the research question ‘What are the possible selves among adolescents in Hong Kong?’ Open-ended responses of possible selves were counted and content coded by the researcher and a research assistant (agreement rate was 95%). Content of HPS and FPS was coded into nine categories. These categories were school-related, career-related, general achievement, interpersonal, personality traits, physical/health-related, material lifestyles, drugs and alcohol use and delinquency possible selves (Clinkinbeard, Citation2007). The strategies were coded into two types: Concrete and abstract (Clinkinbeard, Citation2007; Oyserman et al., Citation2004). A concrete strategy was defined as any strategy that could be readily replicated by another person. For example, ‘talk more with parents’ could be easily understood and performed by another person. An abstract strategy was referred to the strategy had one of the following characteristics: (1) strategies that did not meet the above definition of concrete; (2) strategies that were not relevant to the possible self listed; (3) strategy that was just a repeat of the possible self or was equivalent to just ‘not’ doing it. For example, one's feared self was drug abuser and the corresponding strategy was ‘do not take drug’ (Clinkinbeard, Citation2007).

Proportions of ordinal factors, mean, standard deviation and normality of continuous variables were examined via SPSS software, version 19.0. The pattern of possible selves was presented in three sections. First, the description of possible selves included the number of possible selves and the categories of possible selves. This part provides information about how much adolescents think about their future and what kinds of future images they consider. Gender and age group differences were examined. Second, description of strategies included the number of strategies and the types of strategies. This part provides information about the plans adolescents have to attain or avoid their possible selves. As participants were required to answer maximally two HPS and two FPS, possible answers to the number of HPS or FPS would be zero, one and two. Therefore, the number of possible selves and the number of strategies were regarded as ordinal variables. The third part was about the perceived likelihood of possible selves. Gender differences, age group differences and category differences were estimated via χ2- or t-test or ANOVAs. As perceived likelihood of FPS was a non-normally distributed variable, all the tests about perceived likelihood were calculated with 2000 bootstrap resampling to avoid the possible effect of non-normal distribution. SPSS software, version 19.0 was used for all above analyses.

Results

Description of possible selves

A majority of participants filled in two HPS (72%) and two FPS (66%); about 15% filled in one HPS and about 18% filled in one feared possible self.

Girls wrote significantly more HPS and more FPS than boys. About 79% girls filled in two HPS while 67% of boys filled in two HPS (χ2(2, n = 3042) = 82.08, p < .001, phi = .16). About 71% of girls filled in two FPS while 61% of boys filled in two FPS (χ2(2, n = 3042) = 59.85, p < .001, phi = .14).

Two χ2-tests for age group differences were conducted, one for the number of hoped-for selves and one for the number of feared selves. The one for HPS, χ2(4, n = 3040) = 3.47, p = .48 revealed that there was no significant difference among age groups. The number of feared possible self was significantly different by age, the χ2 coefficient for FPS, χ2(4, n = 3042) = 13.77, p < .01, phi = .07. Participants in middle adolescence filled in the least FPS (63% filled in two FPS, while 68% of early adolescents and 65% late adolescents filled two FPS; 19% of middle adolescents did not fill in any FPS, while 14% early adolescents and 16% late adolescents did not fill any FPS).

The most common categories of HPS were career possible selves (41%, e.g., doctor, teacher), school possible selves (24%, e.g., a university student, making progress in school) and general achievement (9%, e.g., win a singing contest, professionals). For FPS, the most common categories were drugs and alcohol-related (32%, e.g. drug user, smoker), risky behaviours possible selves (16%, e.g. criminal, triad member) and personal traits (10%, e.g., lazy, hypocrite).

Table shows the selected examples of nine of the categories of HPS and FPS found in this study. Specific examples were cited to illustrate the kinds of possible selves adolescents referred to.

Table 1 Possible selves by categories and selected examples

Four χ2-tests were conducted for gender differences among possible selves categories, two for HPS and two for FPS. The four tests, two for HPS, χ21stHPS (7, n = 2639) = 52.63, p < .001, phi = .14 and χ22ndHPS (7, n = 2, 215) = 19.62, p < .01, phi = .09 and two for FPS, χ21stFPS (8, n = 2506) = 74.15, p < .001, phi = .17, and χ22ndFPS (8, n = 2043) = 84.24, p < .001, phi = .20, revealed that there were significant gender differences in categories of possible selves. Table shows that, for the HPS, boys had more career possible selves and material life possible selves, while girls had more school possible selves and interpersonal possible selves. Boys had more risky behaviour FPS, while girls had more interpersonal and more school-related FPS.

Description of possible selves strategies

A strategy shows whether participants had thought about a path or method to fulfil their possible selves. Fifty-seven percent of participants reported one or more planned strategies for HPS while 30% reported one or more planned strategies to avoid becoming like their feared self.

Gender differences existed in the number of strategies of HPS and FPS, with χ2 coefficient for HPS, χ2 (1, n = 3042) = 151.87, p < .001, phi = .22, χ2 coefficient for FPS, χ2 (1, n = 3042) = 84.36, p < .001, phi = .17. Girls were more likely to write a strategy for their possible selves. About 69% of girls reported at least one strategy for hoped-for selves and 38% reported strategy for feared selves, while only 47% of boys filled in strategy for hoped-for selves and 23% reported strategy for feared selves.

Two χ2 tests for age group differences were conducted, one for the number of hoped-for selves and one for the number of feared selves. Both tests showed significant difference among age groups (χ2HPS (2, n = 3040) = 13.45, p < .01, phi = .07; χ2FPS (2, n = 3042) = 24.37, p < .01, phi = .09). For hoped-for selves, being older had higher percentage of reporting strategy (67% late adolescents, 57% middle adolescents and 56% in early adolescence reported strategy for their hoped-for selves). As for feared selves, participants in middle adolescence reported the least strategies. About only 27% of the middle adolescence group reported at least one strategy, while 42% of late adolescence group and 30% of early adolescence group reported strategy for their feared selves.

Whether a strategy was concrete or abstract reflected the quality of the planned strategy. Concrete strategy referred to detailed or executable method to attain hoped-for or avoid FPS. Abstract strategy referred to indefinite and non-concrete strategy (Clinkinbeard, Citation2007).

Among those who reported a strategy for 1st hoped-for self (n = 1,553), 36% reported a concrete strategy while 64% reported an abstract strategy. Among those who reported a strategy for 2nd hoped-for selves (n = 915), about 47% reported a concrete strategy while 53% filled in an abstract one. Among those who filled in a strategy for 1st feared possible self, (n = 766), about 48% reported a concrete strategy while 52% reported an abstract strategy. Among those who reported a strategy for 2nd FPS (n = 512), about 45% reported a concrete strategy while 55% filled in an abstract one.

Four χ2 tests were conducted for gender differences among the strategy types for each possible self. The four tests showed no significant gender difference within the strategies of possible selves (χ21stHPS(1, n = 1534) = 1.66, p = .20; χ22ndHPS (1, n = 905) = 3.19, p = .07; χ21stFPS(1, n = 757) = 1.38, p = .24; χ22ndFPS (1, n = 509) = 1.78, p = .18).

Another four χ2-tests for age group difference among the strategy types were conducted for each possible self. For the 1st hoped-for and the 1st FPS, χ2 showed no age group difference in their strategies (χ21stHPS (1, n = 1540) = .64, p = .73; χ21stFPS(2, n = 757) = 2.88, p = .24). However, χ2 for the 2nd HPS (χ22ndHPS (2, n = 905) = 7.65, p < .05, phi = .09) and 2nd FPS (χ22ndFPS (2, n = 509) = 7.00, p < .05, phi = .12) showed significant age group differences. For HPS, being older reported more concrete strategy (58% in late adolescence, 48% in middle adolescence and 43% in early adolescents). As for FPS, participants in mid-adolescence reported the least concrete strategies (53% in late adolescence, 37% in middle adolescence and 48% in early adolescents reported concrete strategy).

Perceived likelihood of possible selves

The perceived likelihood of HPS and FPS was above the midpoint (3 out of a Likert scale ranged 1–5). Participants were more likely to think they would succeed in avoiding their feared (M = 4.1, SD = 1.0) than succeed in attaining their hoped-for (M = 3.4, SD = 0.8) possible selves, t (2, 461) = 30.09, p < .001, Cohen's d = .77. Given the skewness of likelihood of FPS (skewness = 1.23, kurtosis  = 1.18), bootstrapping was used in all analysis about likelihood.

Two t-tests were used to examine the gender differences on perceived likelihood. Both tests, tHPS (1, 2430) = 1.94, p = .05; tFPS (1, 2430) = .09, p = .09, indicated that boys and girls had marginally significant differences in perceived likelihood for HPS but not for FPS. Two ANOVAs were conducted to examine the age group differences. Given the possible unequal variances caused by large difference in age group sample size, Welch test was conducted with ANOVA. Age group differences existed in perceived likelihood for HPS (F(2, 643) = 3.31, p < .05), but not for avoidance of FPS (F(2, 648) = .75, p = .48). Post hoc test showed that participants in late adolescence rated significantly lower likelihood than those in mid and early adolescence. The interaction effect between gender and age (F(7, 2175) = 1.72, p>.10) was not statistically significant. No significant interaction effect between gender and age on perceived likelihood was found by two-way between group ANOVA.

Four one-way ANOVAs with Welch tests were calculated to determine whether perceived likelihood differed by categories of possible selves. Results indicated that perceived likelihood differed by categories of both HPS and FPS (F1stHPS (7, 277) = 24.00, p < .001; F2ndHPS (7, 296) = 33.6, p < .001), F1stFPS (8, 452) = 33.15, p < .001, and F2ndFPS (8, 543) = 20.67.04, p < .001). However, no significant interaction effect was found between gender and categories on likelihood ratings. Participants perceived that physical/health-related and personality-related HPS were more likely to be attained, while material-related and school possible selves were least likely to be attained. For FPS, drugs-related possible selves (i.e., high possibility to avoid drug taking) and risky behaviour-related were more likely to be avoided. Physical/health-related and school-related FPS were the least likely to be avoided.

Discussion

This study aimed at exploring the general pattern of possible selves among Hong Kong adolescents, including the content of possible selves, the strategy of possible selves and the perceived likelihood of possible selves. It is found that the most frequently mentioned HPS were career-related and school-related and general achievement. The vast majority of respondents' possible selves reflected normative goals, such as finding a decent and steady job, better school achievement, getting admission to university, and being a useful person to the society. Results from this study are consistent with previous research pointing to the possible selves among adolescents (e.g., Curry, Trew, Turner, & Hunter, Citation1994; Knox et al., Citation2000; Leondari et al., Citation2009; Oyserman, Terry, & Bybee, Citation2002). Many fewer adolescents mentioned physical and health-related possible selves. It seems health is not a major concern among adolescents. With regard to FPS, a lot of adolescents were afraid of becoming lazy, aimless, incapable people and having low achievement, in addition to becoming the drug and risky behaviour-related people. Compared to the studies that were conducted in Western countries, this study found that Hong Kong adolescents expressed fewer interpersonal-related HPS and FPS, which was similar to American Indians (Fryberg & Markus, Citation2003) but was different from European Americans (Knox et al., Citation2000).

Not every possible self had one strategy. It seems adolescents had some envisioning about the future, but did not have well-planned strategies for possible selves, especially for avoiding the FPS (Clinkinbeard & Murray, Citation2012; Clinkinbeard & Zohra, Citation2012; Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, Citation2003). In addition, some of the strategies were rather simple and abstract. For example, many students mentioned ‘study hard’ to attain school achievement. Intervention on how to improve the planning skills and develop strategy would be helpful to increase the motivation function of possible selves (Oyserman et al., Citation2006).

With regard to perceived likelihood, Hong Kong adolescents rated physical/health-related and personality HPS most likely to be attained while drug and risky behaviour FPS were most likely to be avoided. This may be because in general adolescents tend to take health for granted (Harris, Duncan, & Boisjoly, Citation2002) and think they can avoid becoming drug and risky behaviour FPS by not doing it and staying away from the peers who use drugs or conduct delinquent behaviours. It is noteworthy that students perceived relatively low likelihood of school-related possible selves, either of attaining HPS or of avoiding FPS. It seems students have more expectations of their school-related possible selves and fears of failure in school work, but they lack confidence about fulfilling them. More guidance and support would be helpful for adolescents to know the path for attaining their school possible selves.

The study found some gender differences that are consistent with those in previous research. For example, boys expressed more career and material-related possible selves while girls had more school and interpersonal-related possible selves. Generally, girls cared more about interpersonal relationship than boys, while boys cared more about career (Brown & Diekman, Citation2010; Knox et al., Citation2000). In addition, boys had higher perceived likelihood for their FPS (Knox, Funk, Elliot, & Bush, Citation1998). This study also found that girls reported more possible selves strategies than boys. However, girls did not have significantly more concrete strategies than boys.

This study also found possible selves differences between developmental stages in adolescence. Older adolescents were more likely to have more strategies and more concrete strategy for HPS. However, participants in mid-adolescence reported the significantly less FPS, fewer strategies and less concrete strategy for FPS. Mid-adolescence may be a stage in which young people have some difficulties with considering the negative side of some behaviours, such as risky behaviours (Rose, Citation2006). In addition, late adolescents perceived significantly lower likelihood for their HPS than early- and mid-adolescents. Growing up, they may realize more restrictions and difficulty in reality and may doubt the likelihood of realising dreams.

This study was conducted among Hong Kong adolescents. Some of the findings may be related to Chinese culture and Hong Kong context. For example, school-related was the second dominant category of HPS. This may be partly because Chinese culture also values education highly (Hau & Ho, Citation2010). Children are taught that ‘all works are low in status, except study which is the highest’ (wan ban jie xia pin, wei you du shu gao; 萬般皆下品,唯有讀書高) (Wang, CitationSong Dynasty). Among the strategies that participants had, working hard and studying hard were most commonly expressed. This may be due to the emphasis on effort in Chinese culture. Working hard toward a goal even though the goal seems impossible is highly praised (Lau, Citation1996; Leung, Citation2010). For example, ‘if one has the perseverance, one can even remove a hill, the symbolic obstacle, by carrying away the sand with baskets’ (yugong yi shan; 愚公移山) (Lau, Citation1996), which emphasises the importance of effort rather than ability. However, career-related possible selves seem to be universally common possible selves among adolescents across different nationalities and ethnicities (Knox et al., Citation1998).

There were several limitations of this study. First, it was a cross-sectional study measured on only one time point. Therefore, this study could not provide the information of development of possible selves. Future research should examine longitudinally the changes of possible selves over time. Second, the research design of questionnaire survey also led to some shortcomings. The classroom survey had limited time for participants to think about their possible selves and strategies in detail and to fill in more possible selves. Questionnaire interview would be better to collect more information about possible selves if time and resources permitted. Third, this study did not provide the information of balanced possible selves that are the matched HPS and FPS in the same domain (Lee & Oyserman, Citation2009). Because this questionnaire only measured two HPS and two FPS, the match rate was very low in preliminary analysis. Fourth, this study was part of a larger study on adolescents' attitude to drug use. Some drug-related FPS may be more or less influenced by other drug-related questions in the questionnaire.

This research is among the few studies on adolescents' possible selves in Chinese context and the first in Hong Kong. Understanding what the adolescents think possible for their future, how they plan to attain it, and how likely are these possible selves sheds light on the adolescents' motivational aspects of self-system. It also provides insight into ways adolescents perceive their own possibility for unknown future and adapt to development outcomes. The findings from this study are also useful for understanding youths' motivation for their future in the contemporary context so as to provide useful information for intervention programs.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Shimin Zhu

Shimin Zhu is a Post-doctoral Fellow at the Department of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong. She has a major research interest in positive youth development. Dr. Zhu focuses on adolescents' view of future and the potential parental and psychological influences that foster adolescents' confidence towards future and channel their effort towards or away productive actions. She also studies the protective and risk factors of adolescent drug use, academic achievement and school adjustment.

Samson Tse

Samson Tse is Professor and Director of the Master of Social Science in Counseling Program at the Department of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong. He has long-standing interest in supporting individuals including young people affected by severe mental illnesses such as bipolar disorder and gambling disorder by using recovery approach and strengths-based interventions.

Notes

1. Secondary school in 2010 included seven forms. Forms one to six were equivalent to US grades seven to twelve. Form seven was the last high school grade in Hong Kong, which akin to the Canadian system, had an additional high school year. During the time of data collection, students in forms five and seven were attending public examinations and so were not included in the sample. In each participating school, two or three classes were randomly selected from forms one, two, three, four and six (equivalent to grades 7 to 10 and grade 12 in the US system).

2. Intermediate points were 2 =  under HK$500 (US$70), 3 =  HK$500–999 (US$70–140), 4 =  HK$1000–1499 (US$140–210), 5 =  HK$1500–1999 (US$210–280), 6 =  HK$2000–2499 (US$280–350), 7 =  HK$2500–2999, (US$ 350–400). Scale points were set according the feedback of a pilot study and in consultation with the youth advisory group working with the larger study so that the range would fit the amount of monthly allowance children in Hong Kong receive. In Hong Kong, parents provide a monthly allowance for school children's transportation, meals, school-related functions, social activities and entertainment.

References

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (2001). Self-efficacy beliefs as shapers of children's aspirations and career trajectories. Child Development, 72, 187–206. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00273.
  • Bardach, S. H., Gayer, C. C., Clinkinbeard, T., Zanjani, F., & Watkins, J. F. (2010). The malleability of possible selves and expectations regarding aging. Educational Gerontology, 36, 407–424. doi:10.1080/03601270903212393.
  • Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5–33. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5.
  • Boyd, D. R., & Bee, H. L. (2012). Lifespan development (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
  • Briones, E., Tabernero, C., & Arenas, A. (2011). Adolescents' future wishes and fears in their acculturation process. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35, 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2010.06.003.
  • Brown, E. R., & Diekman, A. B. (2010). What will I be? Exploring gender differences in near and distant possible selves. Sex Roles, 63, 568–579. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9827-x.
  • Cai, H., Brown, J. D., Deng, C., & Oakes, M. A. (2007). Self-esteem and culture: Differences in cognitive self-evaluations or affective self-regard?Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 162–170. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00222.x.
  • Clinkinbeard, S. S. (2007). Social feedback perceptions, self-efficacy, and possible selves among adolescent offenders in secured juvenile facilities (PhD). University of Nevada, Reno. Available from ProQuest UMI (No. 3276955).
  • Clinkinbeard, S. S., & Murray, C. I. (2012). Perceived support, belonging, and possible selves strategies among incarcerated juvenile offenders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 1218–1240. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00884.x.
  • Clinkinbeard, S. S., & Zohra, T. (2012). Expectations, fears, and strategies: Juvenile offender thoughts on a future outside of incarceration. Youth & Society, 44, 236–257. doi:10.1177/0044118x11398365.
  • Compas, B. E., Davis, G. E., Forsythe, C. J., & Wagner, B. M. (1987). Assessment of major and daily stressful events during adolescence: The Adolescent Perceived Events Scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 534–541. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.55.4.534.
  • Consedine, N. S., Sabag-Cohen, S., & Krivoshekova, Y. S. (2007). Ethnic, gender, and socioeconomic differences in young adults' self-disclosure: Who discloses what and to whom?Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 254–263. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.3.254.
  • Cross, S., & Markus, H. (1991). Possible selves across the life span. Human Development, 34, 230–255. doi:10.1159/000277058.
  • Curriculum Development Council Hong Kong. (2013). Senior secondary curriculum guide. Hong Kong., Retrieved from http://cd1.edb.hkedcity.net/cd/cns/sscg_web/html/english/index.html,.
  • Currie, C. E., Elton, R. A., Todd, J., & Platt, S. (1997). Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: The WHO health behaviour in school-aged children survey. Health Education Research, 12, 385–397. doi:10.1093/her/12.3.385.
  • Curry, C., Trew, K., Turner, I., & Hunter, J. (1994). The effect of life domains on girls' possible selves. Adolescence, 29, 133–150.
  • Education and Manpower Bureau Hong Kong. (2005). The new academic structure for senior secondary education and higher education: Action plan for investing in the future of Hong Kong. In Hong Kong, Retrieved from http://334.edb.hkedcity.net/EN/consultationreport.php.
  • Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York, NY: Norton.
  • Ervin, L. H., & Stryker, S. (2001). Theorizing the relationship between self-esteem and identity. In T. J.Owens, S.Stryker, & N.Goodman (Eds.), Extending self-esteem research: Sociological and psychological currents (pp. 29–55). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Fryberg, S. A., & Markus, H. R. (2003). On being American Indian: Current and possible selves. Self and Identity, 2, 325–344. doi:10.1080/714050251.
  • Hamman, D., Gosselin, K., Romano, J., & Bunuan, R. (2010). Using possible-selves theory to understand the identity development of new teachers. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26, 1349–1361. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2010.03.005.
  • Harris, K. M., Duncan, G. J., & Boisjoly, J. (2002). Evaluating the role of "nothing to lose" attitudes on risky behavior in adolescence. Social Forces, 80, 1005–1039. doi:10.1353/sof.2002.0008.
  • Hau, K. -T., & Ho, I. T. (2010). Chinese students' motivation and achievement. In M.Bond (Ed.), Handbook of Chinese psychology (2nd ed., pp. 187–205). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Hong Kong Census and Statistics Department. (2006). 2006 Population by-census: Basic tables for district council districts, Hong Kong. Retrieved from http://www.bycensus2006.gov.hk/FileManager/EN/Content_962/06bc_dcca_hk.pdf.
  • Hong Kong Special Aministration Region Government. (2012). Economic special reports: Unemployment of Hong Kong adolescents. Hong Kong.
  • Hooker, K. (1992). Possible selves and perceived health in older adults and college students. Journal of Gerontology, 47, P85–P95. doi:10.1093/geronj/47.2.P85.
  • Hoyle, R. H., & Sherrill, M. R. (2006). Future orientation in the self-system: Possible selves, self-regulation, and behavior. Journal of Personality, 74, 1673–1696. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00424.x.
  • Jenkins, G. E. (1999). The possible selves of college students with and without disabilities (PhD thesis). Mississippi State University. Retrieved from http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=730308731&fmt=7&clientid=17557&rqt=309&vname=pqd(9946327).
  • Kao, G. (2000). Group images and possible selves among adolescents: Linking stereotypes to expectations by race and ethnicity. Sociological Forum, 15, 407–430. doi:10.1023/a:1007572209544.
  • Knox, M. S., Funk, J., Elliot, R., & Bush, E. G. (1998). Adolescents' possible selves and their relationship to global self-esteem. Sex Roles, 39, 61–80. doi:10.1023/A:1018877716225.
  • Knox, M. S., Funk, J., Elliott, R., & Bush, E. G. (2000). Gender differences in adolescents' possible selves. Youth & Society, 31, 287–309. doi:10.1177/0044118x00031003002.
  • Kwan, V. S., Hui, C. -M., & McGee, J. A. (2010). What do we know about the Chinese self. In M.Bond (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 279–294). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Lau, S. (Ed.). (1996). Growing up the Chinese way: Chinese child and adolescent development. Hong Kong: The Chinese University Press.
  • Lee, S. J., & Oyserman, D. (2007). Reaching for the future: The education-focused possible selves of low-income mothers. New Directions for Adult and Continuing Education, 2007, 39–49. doi:10.1002/ace.255.
  • Lee, S. J., & Oyserman, D. (2009). Possible selves theory. In E.Anderman & L.Anderman (Eds.), Psychology of classroom learning: An encyclopedia (pp. 269–288). Detroit, MI: Macmillan.
  • Leondari, A., Gonida, E. N., & Gialamas, V. (2009). “Possible” selves during middle adolescence: Relationships with school achievement and with various demographic factors. Psychology: The Journal of the Hellenic Psychological Society, 16, 342–360.
  • Leung, K. (2010). Beliefs in Chinese culture. In M.Bond (Ed.), Handbook of Chinese psychology (2nd ed., pp. 221–240). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Lips, H. M. (2004). The gender gap in possible selves: Divergence of academic self-views among high school and university students. Sex Roles, 50, 357–371. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000018891.88889.c9.
  • Maneesriwongul, W., & Dixon, J. K. (2004). Instrument translation process: A methods review. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48, 175–186. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03185.x.
  • Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954.
  • Meek, R. (2007). The parenting possible selves of young fathers in prison. Psychology, Crime & Law, 13, 371–382. doi:10.1080/10683160601060614.
  • Narcotics Division Security Bureau H.K.S.A.R.. (2010). The 2008/09 survey of drug use among students. Retrieved from http://www.nd.gov.hk/en/survey_of_drug_use.htm.
  • Narcotics Division Security Bureau H.K.S.A.R. (2013). The 2011/12 survey of drug use among students. Retrieved from http://www.nd.gov.hk/en/survey_of_drug_use_11-12.htm.
  • Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2003). Gendered racial identity and involvement with School. Self and Identity, 2, 307–324. doi:10.1080/714050250.
  • Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves and academic outcomes: How and when possible selves impel action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 188–204. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.188.
  • Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Terry, K., & Hart-Johnson, T. (2004). Possible selves as roadmaps. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 130–149. doi:10.1016/s0092-6566(03)00057-6.
  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3.
  • Oyserman, D., & Fryberg, S. A. (2006). The possible selves of diverse adolescents: Content and function across gender, race and national origin. In C.Dunkel & J.Kerpelman (Eds.), Possible selves: Theory, research and applications (pp. 17–39). New York, NY: Nova Science.
  • Oyserman, D., Fryberg, S. A., & Yoder, N. (2007). Identity-based motivation and health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1011–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1011.
  • Oyserman, D., Gant, L., & Ager, J. (1995). A socially contextualized model of African American identity: Possible selves and school persistence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1216–1232. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1216.
  • Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. R. (1990). Possible selves and delinquency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 112–125. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.112.
  • Oyserman, D., Terry, K., & Bybee, D. (2002). A possible selves intervention to enhance school involvement. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 313–326. doi:10.1006/jado.2002.0474.
  • Ridge, T. (2002). Childhood poverty and social exclusion: From a child's perspective. Bristol: The Policy Press.
  • Rose, J. (2006). You and your mid-adolescent. London: Karnac Books.
  • Ryff, C. D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of shifting horizons. Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.6.2.286.
  • Shah, A. A., Syeda, F., & Bhatti, S. H. (2012). Pocket money as a proxy for family income. International Journal of Social Science & Education, 2, 688–693.
  • Waid, L. D., & Frazier, L. D. (2003). Cultural differences in possible selves during later life. Journal of Aging Studies, 17, 251–268. doi:10.1016/s0890-4065(03)00031-8.
  • Wang, Z. (Song Dynasty). Encourage learning (poem).
  • Weinberger, D. R., Elvevåg, B., & Giedd, J. N. (2005). The adolescent brain. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.