314
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Brief Article

Does sunshine prime loyal … or summer? Effects of associative relatedness on the evaluative priming effect in the valent/neutral categorisation task

, , &
Pages 222-230 | Received 09 Sep 2016, Accepted 11 Jan 2017, Published online: 02 Feb 2017
 

ABSTRACT

After 30 years of research, the mechanisms underlying the evaluative priming effect are still a topic of debate. In this study, we tested whether the evaluative priming effect can result from (uncontrolled) associative relatedness rather than evaluative congruency. Stimuli that share the same evaluative connotation are more likely to show some degree of non-evaluative associative relatedness than stimuli that have a different evaluative connotation. Therefore, unless associative relatedness is explicitly controlled for, evaluative priming effects reported in earlier research may be driven by associative relatedness instead of evaluative relatedness. To address this possibility, we performed an evaluative priming study in which evaluative congruency and associative relatedness were manipulated independently from each other. The valent/neutral categorisation task was used to ensure evaluative stimulus processing in the absence of response priming effects. Results showed an effect of associative relatedness but no (overall) effect of evaluative congruency. Our findings highlight the importance of controlling for associative relatedness when testing for evaluative priming effects.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes

1. Distributed memory models (e.g. Masson, Citation1995) do not solve this problem, since valence itself accounts for a limited fraction of the activation pattern of a valent concept only (Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, & Eelen, Citation2007, Footnote 8).

2. FSAA can be also more generally applied to semantic dimensions other than valence (e.g. Spruyt et al., Citation2009; Spruyt, De Houwer, Everaert, & Hermans, Citation2012).

3. We decided to use this method in order to be able to generate a large set of associated prime/target pairs that (a) are both valent, (b) contain equal numbers of same and opposite valence pairs, and (c) are of comparable associative strengths (so that quantitative indicators for associative relatedness were required). Unfortunately, a database of information on associative strength between different word pairs does not exist for the German language. Therefore, self-report data on selective items was the most promising and straightforward approach, as it allowed us to validate and equate the selection of prime/target pairs with regard to their associative strengths.

4. “Blank screens” are always meant as “blank apart from the category labels”.

5. Using more conservative outlier criteria (exclusion of reaction times below 400 ms or above 1.5 interquartile distances above the 75th percentile of the individual response-time distributions) did not change the pattern of results. Likewise, our findings were not contingent upon the inclusion or exclusion of trials on which a target was presented that was often misclassified.

Additional information

Funding

Preparation of this paper was supported by Methusalem Grant BOF16/MET_V/002 of Ghent University.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.