15,338
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Young children’s emotional experiences

ORCID Icon &

The Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) (Citation2015) posits that positive lifetime outcomes rely on the acquisition of a balance of cognitive, social and emotional skills during childhood, while securing emotional skills, along with social skills, has been shown to correlate positively with academic achievement (Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger, Citation2011; Gutman & Schoon, Citation2013). Further research on young children's emotional development and experiences emphasizes that their emotional competencies are ‘critical aspects of the development of overall brain architecture, and that it has enormous consequences over the course of a lifetime’ (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, Citation2011, p. 1) and ‘critical for children's success in school, as well as in other settings and in later phases of life into adulthood’ (Darling-Churchill & Lippman, Citation2016, p. 2). Among researchers in the field of early childhood education and care, there is a growing acknowledgement of the importance of emotional development in children's overall well-being and how it affects young children's lives on multiple domains of their development as well as their ability to form and sustain secure and successful relationships through life (Bowlby, Citation1988; Denham, Bassett, Brown, Way, & Steed, Citation2015; Dowling, Citation2010; Dunn, Citation1993; Gerhardt, Citation2014; Thompson & Lagattuta, Citation2006). Evidence from extant research therefore suggests that positive emotional experiences in early childhood may provide an important foundation for a fulfilling life.

On these lines, Yates et al. (Citation2008, p. 2) regard emotional development in young children (0–5) as the ability to ‘form close and secure adult and peer relationships; experiences, regulate, and express emotions in socially and culturally appropriate ways; and explore the environment and learn-all in the context of family, community, and culture’. This view echoes the Vygotskian idea that emotions do not exist and develop in a vacuity: ‘the emotions are not “a state within a state”’, but rather need to be examined and understood as part of the ‘[…] dynamic human life. It is within this context that the emotional processes acquire their meaning and sense’ (Vygotsky, Citation1987, p. 333).

From early research, therefore, emotional development and health has been closely linked to the environment and the emotional characteristics in which children live, including families and the broader context of their communities, such as early childhood education and care (Davies & Forman, Citation2002; Denham et al., Citation2003; Glaser, Citation2000). Extending discussion on the importance of the environment, Steiner and Perry (Citation1997, p. 11) proposes we should rethink emotional development through the lenses of the concept of emotional literacy as

the ability to understand your emotions, the ability to listen to others and empathise with their emotions, and the ability to express emotions productively. To be emotionally literate is to be able to handle emotions in a way that improves your personal power and improves the quality of life around you. Emotional literacy improves relationships, creates loving possibilities between people, makes co-operative work possible, and facilitates the feeling of community.

More recent contributions examine the role of the environment in relation to the emotional development of young children (e.g. Andrew, Citation2015; Salamon, Sumsion, & Harrison, Citation2017) and build on Bourdieu’s (Citation1986) notion of symbolic capital. This introduces the idea of ‘emotional capital’ that focuses on emotions ‘as resources, or a set of assets, which can be circulated, accumulated and exchanged for other forms of capital within a particular field that allows those resources to “count”’ (Colley, Citation2006, p. 25). The notion of emotional capital takes into consideration children's repertoire of emotional skills they use to relate to and interact with the environment around them (Andrew, Citation2015; Salamon et al., Citation2017).

Thus, a consensus emerges among researchers that early childhood education and care must create an equilibrium between academic achievements, such as literacy and numeracy, and attention to the ‘emotional capital’ of young children. At a policy level where measurement, school readiness, time-bound outcomes and economic imperatives tend to be foregrounded, the potential value of nourishing affective engagements for infants and young children may be overlooked. Indeed, while the OECD's measurement and testing approach introduced in 2017 focuses broadly on young children's acquisition of skills not only in early language, literacy and numeracy, but also in social behaviour, empathy, memory and self-regulation, the International Early Learning and Child Wellbeing Study (IELS) has been identified as highly problematic (Moss et al., Citation2016), in part because its key rationale is economic. Research evidence highlights the paucity of adequate measurement tools designed to test emotional development (Campbell et al., Citation2016).

By drawing on new empirical research focused on children's emotional experiences, this special issue is therefore timely. Its articles concern challenges and opportunities inherent in young children's emotional experiences in twenty-first century contexts of early childhood education and care. They are intended to provoke debate, discussion and critique as well as asking significant questions of policymakers, practitioners and carers who may influence young children's emotional experiences. As a collection, the articles promote the idea that we must continue to further our understanding of children's emotional experiences. Nevertheless, the findings they highlight indicate that a test-based approach may detract from young children's emotional development and the positive emotional experiences in early childhood which have potential to provide an important foundation for a fulfilling life.

The special issue features eight articles. The first two papers address early childhood practitioners’ views of affective aspects of their work in England. Peter Wood's article draws on evidence from case study interviews conducted with early years and primary school practitioners in England to argue that practitioners’ recognition of the complex nature of young children's emotional and social skill acquisition is preferable to attempts to fit these skills into a normative model. In the second paper concerned with practitioners’ and managers’ reflections on emotions as they present in nursery interactions, Peter Elfer, Sue Greenfield, Sue Robson, Dilys Wilson and Antonia Zachariou share early findings from their evaluation of a Work Discussion intervention in England. Elfer et al. refer to their findings in order to explore the potential of Work Discussion for enhancing how practitioners engage with children and their families.

The three articles that follow are all concerned with early childhood practitioners’ interactions with young children. Belinda Davis and Rosemary Dunn discuss findings from interviews they conducted with educators in Australia about emotional relationships the educators built with infants aged up to two years. Davis and Dunn suggest that while such relationships may characterize the complex nature of pedagogy for infants, they are not always recognized in statutory curriculum documentation. In another article based on case study narratives contributed by practitioners about their nurturing touch practices with toddlers in Danish Early Childhood Education and Care (ECEC) settings, Lone Svinth investigates how such practices affect toddlers’ emotional well-being and learning and the practitioners’ relationships with the children. The final article in this section highlights findings from an ethnographic case study conducted with children younger than three years in two Greek day-care settings. Eleni Katsiada, Eirini Roufidou, Jonathan Wainwright and Varvar Aggeli found that these very young children were agentic in forming interpersonal relationships with adults in their day-care settings.

The final set of articles in this special issue addresses young children's self-regulation as an aspect of their emotional experiences. Lorna Arnott draws on data from two studies conducted in pre-schools in Scotland for which child-centred play-based methodologies were adopted. Arnott identifies ways that young children interpreted cues in their settings to decide how to manage and regulate socio-emotional interactions and their play and her findings support identification of four elements of early childhood education and care pedagogic culture. In the second article in this section, David Ferrier, Samantha Karalus, Susanne Denham and Hideko Bassett report on key aspects of the Teachers As Socialisers of Social-Emotional Learning (TASSEL) project conducted in the USA. The TASSEL project investigated the affordance of emotional competence through cognitive self-regulation in young children. Ferrier and his team found young children's abilities to express and experience their emotions were significantly affected, both directly and indirectly, by cognitive self-regulation and the ability to understand and identify emotions. The final article of the special issue – ‘The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors’ – by Mel McCree, Roger Cutting and Dean Sherwin focuses on the potential of outdoor play and learning to affect young children's biophilia, wellbeing and academic attainment. This longitudinal mixed methods study conducted in south west England reveals that young children's outdoor experiences may support the development of their emotional learning and wellbeing as well as their academic development.

We have been privileged to edit this special issue. Each article contributes further empirical evidence to the burgeoning corpus that indicates the vital role of young children's emotional experiences in laying strong foundations for fulfilling lives. The research findings reported in them also reveal ways that adults can support this process and suggest that policymakers would be wise to exercise caution before attempting to apply simplistic measurement to young children's emotional capital.

References

  • Andrew, Y. (2015). What we feel and what we do: Emotional capital in early childhood work. Early Years, 35(4), 351–365. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2015.1077206
  • Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. G. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education (pp. 241–258). New York: Greenwood Press.
  • Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London: Routledge.
  • Campbell, S. B., Denham, S. A., Howarth, G. Z., Jones, S. M., Whittaker, J. V., Williford, A. P., … Darling-Churchill, K. (2016). Commentary on the review of measures of early childhood social and emotional development: Conceptualization, critique, and recommendations. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 45, 19–41. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.01.008
  • Colley, H. (2006). Learning to labour with feeling: Class, gender and emotion in childcare education and training. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 7(1), 15–29. doi: 10.2304/ciec.2006.7.1.15
  • Darling-Churchill, K., & Lippman, L. (2016). Early childhood social and emotional development: Advancing the field of measurement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 45, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.002
  • Davies, P. T., & Forman, E. M. (2002). Children’s patterns of preserving emotional security in the interparental subsystem. Child Development, 73, 1880–1903. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00512
  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., Brown, C. A., Way, E., & Steed, J. (2015). “I know how you feel”: Preschoolers’ emotion knowledge contributes to early school success. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 13(3), 252–262. doi: 10.1177/1476718X13497354
  • Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence?. Child Development, 74, 238–256. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00533
  • Dowling, M. (2010). Young children’s personal, social and emotional development (3e). London: SAGE.
  • Dunn, J. (1993). Young children’s close relationships: Beyond attachment. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  • Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. P., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x
  • Gerhardt, S. (2014). Why love matters: How affection shapes a baby’s brain. Abingdon: Routledge.
  • Glaser, D. (2000). Child abuse and neglect and the brain—a review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 97–116. doi: 10.1017/S0021963099004990
  • Gutman, L. M., & Schoon, I. (2013). The impact of non-cognitive skills on outcomes for young people. London: Education Endowment Foundation.
  • Moss, P., Dahlberg, G., Grieshaber, S., Mantovani, S., May, H., Pence, A., … Vandenbroeck, M. (2016). The organisation for economic co-operation and development’s international early learning study: Opening for debate and contestation. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 17(3), 343–351. doi: 10.1177/1463949116661126
  • National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2011). Children’s emotional development is built into the architecture of their brains. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Center on the Developing Child.
  • Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). (2015). Skills for social progress: The power of social and emotional skills, OECD skills studies. Paris: OECD.
  • Salamon, A., Sumsion, J., & Harrison, L. (2017). Infants draw on ‘emotional capital’ in early childhood education contexts: A new paradigm. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 18(4), 362–374. doi: 10.1177/1463949117742771
  • Steiner, C. with Perry, P. (1997). Achieving emotional literacy. London: Bloomsbury.
  • Thompson, R. A., & Lagattuta, K. (2006). Feeling and understanding: Early emotional development. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 317–337). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Yates, T., Ostrosky, M. M., Cheatham, C. A., Fettig, A., Shaffer, L., & Santos, R. M. (2008). Research synthesis on screening and assessing social-emotional competence. Urbana, AZ: University of Illinois, Center of the Social Emotional Foundations for Early Learning.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.