8,406
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Special Issue: Inequalities and the Curriculum

The role of schools in explaining individuals’ subject choices at age 14

Abstract

The subjects that young people study from age 14 onwards may have important consequences for their future academic and labour market outcomes. These decisions are shaped by the schools in which they find themselves. Schools also face constraints of their own. This paper explores the extent to which individuals’ decisions are affected by the school they attend and to what extent this is affected by the composition of schools in terms of academic attainment, gender, and socioeconomic background. We use multi-level variance decomposition models applied to administrative data on the subjects that young people in mainstream state-funded schools in England study between ages 14 and 16. Our results highlight the important role that constraints on schools play in subject choice decisions. We also note the particular role of attending a non-selective school within a selective schooling district.

1. Introduction

Young people’s subject choices at age 14 may have important consequences for their future academic and labour market outcomes, since they in turn affect the qualifications to which they can easily continue in post-compulsory education. Choosing the ‘wrong’ set of options at this point may have long-term consequences (Iannelli, Citation2013). This is a particularly important issue in an English context, where specialisation of the curriculum occurs earlier than in many other countries (Hodgson & Spours, Citation2008), and was at the height of the education policy of the New Labour government (1997–2010), which promoted diversity and flexibility in the 14–16 curriculum, including in ways that have been criticised for allowing the possibility of ‘gaming’ of performance in school accountability measures (Burgess, Propper, Slater, & Wilson, Citation2005).

The choices that individuals face are shaped by the schools in which they find themselves at this point in time, just as previous work has found that pupils’ options are restricted depending on where in the country they live (Open Public Services Network, Citation2015). Schools may not offer certain subjects (Jin, Muriel, & Sibieta, Citation2011) and often guide their pupils towards certain paths (McCrone, Morris, & Walker, Citation2005), for example requiring that a wider set of core subjects be studied, or preventing pupils from taking certain combinations of options. This implies that schools potentially have an important influence in this regard (Woods, Citation1976). However, schools do not set such requirements in isolation. They face significant constraints most obviously from government policy but also, potentially, in responding to what they can offer given the make-up of their student body. For example, they cannot viably offer an optional subject that only a handful of pupils wish to study. Similarly, the local education market may influence schools’ behaviour (Davies, Adnett, & Turnbull, Citation2003), especially the presence of selective schools.

This paper explores this important issue by assessing the extent to which the subjects that individuals study from age 14 to 16 are associated, not only with their personal characteristics, but also with the school they attend. It also builds on this, to provide evidence about the extent to which schools’ provision (and, hence, the options open to their pupils) is driven by their composition in terms of prior academic attainment, socioeconomic background, and gender mix. These are all factors that previous work has found to influence the subjects that individuals study (Davies, Telhaj, Hutton, Adnett, & Coe, Citation2008), but given Jin et al.’s (Citation2011) observation that the most common reason an individual reports as to why they cannot study a subject is that it is not available, it seems important to explore to what extent individual subject choices are driven by the curriculum offered by schools.

After an initial descriptive exploration, we apply hierarchical regression modelling in order to explore how variance is partitioned between and within schools, conditional on other factors. The main results are estimated using administrative data from the National Pupil Database (NPD). Overall, our results demonstrate the important role that schools seem to play in subject choice decisions, with significant variation in subjects studied attributable to the school level. However, they also highlight the potential role of contextual factors for schools, with a significant proportion of this school-level variation being explained by school composition and differences in subjects studied among pupils in non-selective schools in local areas with selective schooling.

The paper proceeds as follows. Section 2 reviews the previous literature on the role of schools in shaping young people’s subject choices. Section 3 introduces the datasets used to explore this issue, highlighting the advantages available from using both survey and administrative data to ensure the robustness of the results. Section 4 explores the patterns of variation in subject choices and the extent to which they vary within and between schools. This is developed using hierarchical regression modelling, which is introduced in Section 5; the results of this modelling are reported in Section 6. Finally, Section 7 concludes.

2. Background and research questions

Previous studies that have explored the determinants of subjects studied between ages 14 and 16 have tended to highlight that three important characteristics explaining subject choices at this age are gender (Bell, Citation2001; Francis, Citation2000; Jin et al., Citation2011; Sullivan, Zimdars, & Heath, Citation2010), prior attainment (Davies et al., Citation2008; Jin et al., Citation2011), and socioeconomic background (Davies et al., Citation2008; Jin et al., Citation2011).

Bell (Citation2001) considered changes in the uptake of combinations of age 14–16 subjects by gender and prior attainment and how this changed with the introduction of the National Curriculum. Davies et al. (Citation2008) used the 1998 Year 11 Information System (Ye11IS) data to examine the probability of taking GCSEs in optional subjects (specifically: Business Studies, French, Geography, German, History, and Home Economics), finding that ‘ability’ has the strongest influence on subject choice but for some subjects social class exerts more of an effect than gender. Using a more recent cohort, Jin et al. (Citation2011) find that girls are more likely to study modern foreign language at school and less likely to study all three sciences separately; these associations remain after taking into account prior attainment. Furthermore, those with more educated parents are more likely to study triple science and to stay on in full-time education after Year 11, however these effects are not significant after controlling for prior attainment. Sullivan et al. (Citation2010) also make use of Next Steps data to examine the social structure of the Key Stage 4 curriculum in England. They examine how the subjects that young people like or dislike shape the choices that they make and how they are influenced by those around them. In addition, they note a gender difference in vocational subjects and differences by ethnicity for triple science and religious study participation.

Previous work on the importance of subject choice during secondary school has also focused on specific elements of the decision, for example considering whether young people study science, technology, engineering, and maths (STEM)-related subjects (Codiroli, Citation2015; Tripney et al., Citation2010). In the case of STEM, this reflects a concern that there is a gender gap in the uptake of such subjects, although Codiroli (Citation2015) highlights that this is not the case among individuals from advantaged backgrounds.

Aspects of the role that schools play in shaping subject choice have also been considered. Jin et al. (Citation2011), using Next Steps, document significant variation in the kinds of qualifications offered by different schools. In addition, they note that ‘19% of pupils were unable to take subjects they would like to study at Key Stage 4’ (Jin et al., Citation2011, p. 63), with the most common reason being that their school did not offer the subject (just over 30% of such cases). They identified large differences by school, where some schools offer courses in both academic and vocational choices in Year 10 while others only offer academic courses. Davies et al. (Citation2008) also considered the influence of school context, noting associations between school cohorts and probability of taking subjects, for example the proportion of children who are eligible for free school meals in the school has an effect on the probability of taking certain subjects.

There is a small amount of international evidence on the effects of the sex composition of co-educational schools and classes. Hoxby (Citation2000) uses data on schools in Texas to show that a higher proportion of boys in the class depresses the attainment of both male and female students in both maths and English. Hoxby suggests various possible mechanisms for such peer effects, including classroom disruption and changes in classroom atmosphere. Israeli research also suggests that a high proportion of boys in a year group is linked to worse academic outcomes for both girls and boys (Lavy & Schlosser, Citation2011). Van Houtte (Citation2004) produces similar findings for Belgium. Proud (Citation2014) uses PLASC/NPD data for the UK, and finds that a higher proportion of girls in the class has a negative effect on boys’ attainment in English, while a higher proportion of girls has a positive effect on both girls’ and boys’ science attainment. Sullivan (Citation2009) found that teenage girls in the 1970s rated their abilities in maths and sciences higher if they went to an all-girls school. Boys on the other hand rated their abilities in English higher if they went to an all-boys school (Sullivan, Citation2009). Similarly, boys and girls who attended single-sex schools showed increased attainment in gender-atypical subject areas, with Sullivan, Joshi, and Leonard (Citation2009) suggesting that single-sex schools may contribute to breaking down gender stereotypes.

It is well established that students attending schools with a high proportion of peers of low social status or low academic ability are at a disadvantage (Coleman et al., Citation1966; Henderson, Mieszkowski, & Sauvageau, Citation1978; Mortimore, Citation1988; Rutter, Citation1982; Smith, Tomlinson, Bonnerjea, Hogarth, & Thomes, Citation1989; Willms, Citation1986). Recent research has suggested that school SES has no direct effect on individual-level attainment except via the academic composition of the school (Marks, Citation2015). The mechanisms behind school composition effects have not been established empirically. School composition effects may reflect peer group processes (for example if lower-attaining peers are more disruptive). School composition may also influence teachers and the curriculum, as teachers seek to provide a curriculum and pedagogical style which they deem appropriate for the population of the school as a whole (Woods, Citation1976).

This paper builds on these previous studies by focusing specifically on the role that schools play in explaining subject choice at age 14. We seek to address the following research questions:

How do the subjects that young people study from ages 14 to 16 vary depending on the school’s composition in terms of prior attainment, socioeconomic status, and gender?

What proportion of the variation in young people’s subjects of study is between schools, rather than within schools?

To what extent is the proportion of variation between schools explained by school composition in terms of prior attainment, socioeconomic status, and gender?

3. Data

This paper uses data from mainstream English state-funded schools for the academic year 2005–2006 available in the National Pupil Database (an administrative dataset owned by the UK’s Department for Education) in order to explore these questions. This includes comprehensive academic attainment data from national examinations in England. Rather than self-reports about subjects of study we use the observed information about which GCSEs (or equivalents) young people have entered at age 16. The advantages of administrative data are clear, in that we have information about full cohorts within schools. The NPD also includes some basic data providing a proxy for young people’s socioeconomic background.

However, this is obviously less fine-grained than the background characteristics available in survey data. To test the robustness of the results to this limitation, we replicated the analysis (with the exception of the selective schooling area analyses) using Next Steps (a longitudinal survey that seeks to represent the same population). Full details of this replication are reported in Anders, Henderson, Moulton, and Sullivan (Citation2017). While the results do not replicate exactly (especially regarding school-level relationships), which is perhaps unsurprising given differences in the measurement instruments and the fact that only a small number of students within each school are surveyed, many of the same broad patterns are evident. Where this is not the case, differences are noted and our confidence in these findings is reduced.

We construct a continuous measure of the academic selectivity of the subjects that a pupil studies from age 14 to 16, based on the prior academic performance of the pupils that choose to study each subject. We assign each subject the average score in Key Stage 3 (KS3) compulsory tests at age 14 of those pupils that report they are studying that subject. KS3 tests are taken roughly contemporaneously with subject choice decisions, so they seem the most appropriate measure to use in this way. The score for a range of subjects is reported in Table . We see that those with the highest levels of KS3 attainment are more likely to study subjects such as languages, while those with lower levels are more likely to take applied subjects of various types.

Table 1. Average academic performance at age 14 of pupils studying each GCSE subject ranked in ascending order.

We next convert this into an individual-level, rather than a subject-level, measure. To do so, we sum the top eight most academically selective subjects that each individual studies. A maximum of eight subjects is used to create this measure in order to stop individuals taking a large number of low-selectivity subjects ending up with a high selectivity score. This follows the logic used in the construction of ‘capped’ GCSE points scores. Thus, individuals who take a combination of academically selective subjects end up with a high score, while individuals who take a combination of less selective subjects are assigned a low score. For ease of interpretation, we standardise this score among the sample used in this paper, so that it has a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one. Henderson, Sullivan, Anders, and Moulton (Citation2018) provide further discussion of the development of this measure. Most reassuringly, we note that this method produces similar results to more complex approaches aimed at capturing subject ‘difficulty’ (Coe, Searle, Barmby, Jones, & Higgins, Citation2008).

In addition, we explore the role of schools in explaining the variation in young people making specific sets of decisions that have previously been argued to be important for future outcomes.

Since 2011, the Russell Group (a self-selecting group of highly selective English universities) has produced an annual document called Informed choices providing advice on the kind of subject choices that will give young people ‘the most options’ when it comes to accessing such universities (Russell Group, Citation2013). Although the cohort we analyse predate the publication of this guidance and its primary focus on advice regarding post-16 subjects (highlighting the importance of the ‘facilitating’ subjects of English literature, maths, history, geography, languages, physics, chemistry, and biology) rather than subject choices at age 14–16, we nevertheless think it useful to analyse which students were already following the spirit of this advice. We analyse whether young people are studying at least three of what might be considered pre-cursors to these post-16 ‘facilitating’ subjects: English, maths, history, geography, languages, physics, chemistry, and biology.

Concern has been expressed about the value to pupils of studying ‘applied’ subjects at GCSE level, especially during the period of our cohort when there were particular worries that schools were using such subjects to ‘game’ school accountability measures (Burgess et al., Citation2005). They are certainly among the least academically selective subjects, which is hardly surprising as they were used as alternatives to more academically focused subjects for individuals for whom these seemed less appropriate. We analyse whether individuals study any of these applied subjects.

Partly in response to the concerns raised in the previous paragraph, more recent government policy has sought to incentivise young people to take a combination of subjects known as the English Baccalaureate (EBacc) (made up of English, maths, history or geography, at least two sciences and a modern or ancient foreign language) arguing that this provides young people with the skills they need for the future. Others have countered that this approach may be harmful because the focus on ensuring passes in these subjects may be to the detriment of other subjects. As with Informed choices, our cohort predates the designation of the EBacc and there was no performance measure incentivising schools to focus on these subjects. Nevertheless, it is useful to analyse which students were already following a curriculum of this type. We analyse whether young people study the full set of these subjects.

Finally, we consider whether young people study all three separate sciences (physics, chemistry, and biology). This full set seems particularly important for progression to STEM subjects, where there has been particular concern about inequality in uptake between males and females.

Given the focus of this paper on the variation between schools the set of independent variables to be used in the model is deliberately parsimonious.

We include young people’s academic attainment at age 11. This age is used since it is at this point that young people sort into secondary schools. The attainment measure at age 11 is based on young people’s performance in Key Stage 2 (KS2) tests in English, maths, and science. We standardise this variable to be a Z-score (i.e. mean of zero, standard deviation of one) to aid interpretation. The distribution of pupils by quintile group of their KS2 performance and the quintile group of their school’s KS2 intake is reported in Table . While schools with low KS2 intakes have, by definition, larger numbers of pupils whose KS2 performance is in the bottom quintile group, there are still individuals in such schools with high levels of KS2 performance.

Table 2. Distribution of pupils from each quintile group of KS2 performance and the quintile group of the school’s intake as measured by KS2.

The NPD includes two proxies for individuals’ socioeconomic status, namely whether they are eligible for free school meals (FSM) and the deprivation status of their neighbourhood (Chowdry, Crawford, Dearden, Goodman, & Vignoles, Citation2013). These are combined using principal components analysis with a polyserial correlation matrix (Kolenikov & Angeles, Citation2009; Olsson, Citation1979) to construct a single index of SES (alternative methods, such as factor analysis, yield very similar results). This explains roughly three-quarters of the variation in the two individual measures. Again, we ensure that this has a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one. There is a similar pattern of the SES distribution within and between schools as that described above regarding prior attainment (Table ).

Table 3. Distribution of pupils from each quintile group of SES and the quintile group of the school’s SES intake.

On gender, we find that 60% of pupils are in broadly gender balanced schools, while 13% are in single gender schools (8% female-only and 5% male-only), 18% are in mainly female schools, and 8% are in mainly male schools (Table ).

Table 4. Distribution of pupils by gender and group of the school’s gender distribution.

4. Subject choice, individual characteristics, and school composition

We begin with a descriptive exploration of differences in young people’s subject choices depending not only on their personal characteristics, but also on the composition of their school in terms of prior attainment, socioeconomic status, and gender. In the interests of space, this descriptive exploration focuses only on our subject academic selectivity measure; we broaden our focus to other specific subject choices in Section 5.

Average academic subject selectivity is positively associated with both an individual’s prior attainment and the average prior attainment of their schoolmates (Table ). However, this is not just because the school-level association picks the average change in individuals’ prior attainment within the school. Individuals with low prior attainment, in schools with high average prior attainment, are more likely to study more academically selective subjects than their counterparts in lower average prior attainment schools. In other words, student intake appears to matter for improving opportunities for students to study more academically selective subjects.

Table 5. Average academic subject selectivity of individuals by their KS2 performance and the average KS2 performance of their schoolmates.

There is a similar overall pattern when it comes to socioeconomic status, perhaps in part because there is a correlation (r = 0.28) between the measures of SES and performance at KS2. On an individual level, SES is, reassuringly, less predictive of the academic selectivity of subjects studied than young people’s prior attainment. Notably, the SES intake of a school appears just as important a predictor as the individual-level indicator (Table ).

Table 6. Average academic subject selectivity of individuals by their SES background and the average SES of their schoolmates.

Academic selectivity of subjects studied also varies somewhat by gender, with girls studying a set of courses with a slightly higher academic selectivity, on average, than boys (Table ). However, in this case, the selectivity of subjects studied varies much more by the gender composition of their school. Most noticeably, pupils in single-sex schools study a set of subjects of significantly higher academic selectivity than those in mixed schools.

Table 7. Average academic subject selectivity of individuals by their gender and the gender composition of schoolmates.

However, it is important to recognise that the analysis above only attempts to explore one aspect at a time of the association between young people’s characteristics, schools’ composition, and the subjects studied by its pupils. There are reasons to think that some of the associations are due to other confounding factors, especially in relatively small groups such as single-sex schools. In order to alleviate this issue, and to allow us to explore the importance of these factors for specific subject choices in a manageable way, we now turn to hierarchical regression modelling.

5. Hierarchical regression modelling

In order to explore the variation in subject choice in a more formal framework, attempting to decompose the influence of prior attainment, SES, and gender, we fit hierarchical regression models of the outcomes of interest. Hierarchical models (also known as multi-level models or regression models with random effects) explicitly partition the variance in the model between units and within units (in this case schools). Marks (Citation2006) uses a similar technique when looking at within- and between-school variation in academic performance.

Hierarchical regression models of the following form are estimated. For continuous outcome variable (index of subject selectivity):

(1)

For dichotomous outcome variables:

(2)

where y is the outcome variable, i indicates the individual, j indicates the school which they attend, X represents a vector of individual-level regressors, represents a vector of the school-level averages of the individual-level regressors, and β and γ represent fixed-effects coefficients. Finally, there are two error terms: η represents a normally distributed school-level random intercept, and ε represents an individual-level error term (normally distributed in the case of models with a continuous outcome and with a logistic distribution in the case of models with a dichotomous outcome).

Models including random effects make assumptions about relationships in the data, which are typically unlikely to be strictly justified. First, it is assumed that the school-level effects are normally distributed. Second, it is assumed that there is no correlation between the individual-level and the school-level error terms. However, one strategy for relaxing this latter assumption is modelling this relationship by including group-level means of the regressors (Mundlak, Citation1978). Given their substantive interest, these are being included in the model in any case. As such, this gives us increased confidence in the estimated relationships between the individual-level covariates and the outcome variables.

Given our interest in the role of schools, an important element of the results is the proportion of variance explained simply by the school which an individual attends, i.e. not the proportion explained by within-school variation or the proportion explained by school-level variation in the make-up of the pupils. This is known as the intra-cluster correlation (ICC or ρ) and is calculated from the hierarchical models as follows:

(3)

Covariates included in the model are added in a sequential manner as follows. First, an empty model (M0) is estimated. This performs the important function of providing a baseline unconditional intra-cluster correlation against which the conditional intra-cluster correlations in later models may be compared.

In the first model including covariates (M1), we add an individual’s KS2 standardised score (Z-score) as well as the school’s average KS2 Z-score. This provides results on the conditional association between young people’s prior attainment after controlling for the school’s performance. It aims to capture not only whether individuals with higher prior attainment study more academically selective subjects but also whether there is an additional increase in the average subject mix of individuals in schools with an intake of more highly attaining pupils.

The next two models (M2 and M3) add in covariates related to gender and SES, respectively; as with M1 these capture both whether these individual-level characteristics are relevant for subjects studied and whether the school context in these terms has additional predictive power. In M2 we include a dummy variable for an individual’s gender. Given the findings of Section 4, we allow for a more flexible relationship between the school’s gender balance and subject choice than a simple linear relationship. We add dummy variables that categorise schools into gender balanced (our baseline), mainly male, mainly female, all-male, and all-female. M3 adds the standardised index of young people’s SES, along with the school’s average value of this index. This provides evidence on whether there is an independent association between SES and subject choice once prior attainment has been held constant.

Our final model (M4) looks specifically at a possible constraint on schools. We include an indicator for whether individuals are in a school located in a local authority (LA) in which more than 5% of pupils attend selective (grammar) schools and for whether they are in a selective school themselves. While much of the dynamic of being in a school with high prior attainment will be captured through the covariates introduced in M1, we are particularly interested to see whether being in a non-selective school within an area in which selective schools are present affects the subjects studied.

6. Results

The results tables focus on the conditional associations between individual-level characteristics and subject choices, school-level compositions and subject choices, and the proportion of variance explained at the school level conditional on included covariates (ρ). For models of the continuous index of academic selectivity we report the regression coefficients, which are interpreted as the expected change in the subject selectivity index in standard deviations.

We consider the results for the subject selectivity score first (Table ). In M0, in which there are no regressors, the only estimate to be interpreted is the proportion of variance between schools rather than within schools, which is estimated to be 0.32. This suggests that approximately two-thirds of the variation in individuals’ subject selectivity is between different pupils within the same school. After conditioning on school intake in terms of prior attainment (M1) this proportion of variation explained by schools is reduced to 27% in model M1; this is only reduced marginally further after conditioning on gender or socioeconomic status in M2 and M3.

Table 8. Variation in subject selectivity.

Turning to associations between characteristics and subject choices, on adding controls for prior attainment at age 11 (M1), we find that a one standard deviation increase in an individual’s prior attainment is associated with roughly a 0.3 standard deviation change in subject selectivity score. The school’s intake attainment profile is associated with subject selectivity even more strongly, with a one standard deviation change in school intake being associated with 0.4 of a standard deviation increase in the average academic selectivity of the subjects its pupils study.

Adding gender and school’s gender balance to the model (M2) makes little difference to the previously described relationships between prior attainment and subject selectivity. Nevertheless, we see a statistically-significant association between an individual’s gender and the academic selectivity of the subjects they study, with girls on average studying a set of subjects that are 0.13 standard deviations more academically selective than their comparable male peers. School-level gender effects are particularly marked when comparing single-sex schools to gender balanced ones with both kinds of single-sex school entering pupils for more academically selective subjects.

Adding socioeconomic status to the model (M3) we find that both individuals’ SES and the average SES in their school are positively associated with the academic selectivity of the subjects they study; these seem of similar qualitative importance. Furthermore, adding the controls for SES reduces the association between schools KS2 intake and subject choice markedly, although it remains an important factor.

Finally, in M4, we consider selective schools and those in local areas where academically selective schools take a significant proportion of individuals (specifically, local authority areas where at least 5% of the population attend a selective school). Including indicators for whether an individual is in a selective area and whether they are in a selective school in the model makes little difference to the other covariates. However, pupils in a school in a selective area study for a statistically significantly less academically selective mix of subjects. Those actually in selective schools see an approximately offsetting effect. This suggests that this result is driven by pupils in non-selective schools in selective areas studying less academically selective subjects, even after individual- and school-level factors have been taken into account.

Turning to our binary models of individuals studying particular combinations of subjects (Table ), we restrict our discussion to highlighting important differences between the general subject selectivity model and these more specific models. We only report the models including the full set of covariates we consider (M4). We also report the intra-cluster correlation from this model (Cond. ρ) and from the model not including any covariates (Uncond. ρ) for comparison. For these models we report odds ratios (exponentiated logistic regression coefficients), which may be interpreted as the expected change in the odds of studying the relevant set of subjects.

Table 9. Odds ratios from logistic regression models of probability that individuals study specific combinations of subjects at age 14.

We start our discussion with whether individuals study at least three ‘facilitating’ subjects, something that we find 81% of the sample to do. As robustness checks we have also run models of whether individuals study at least four or five ‘facilitating’ subjects and find broadly similar results at these alternative margins. The pattern of significant coefficients is broadly similar to the model of our academic selectivity measure, although we cannot compare the magnitudes with the selectivity model due to its linear outcome. One standard deviation higher prior attainment by an individual at age 11 is associated with twice the odds of studying at least three facilitating subjects. Much less dramatically, individuals with one standard deviation higher SES have 28% increased odds of studying three or more facilitating subjects, while males have just slightly reduced odds of doing so.

There is a large and statistically significant association with school intake in terms of prior attainment: a one standard deviation increase in the intake of the school on this measure is associated with a more than doubling of the odds of studying at least three facilitating subjects. The overall importance of schools, as measured by the intra-cluster correlation, is similar to that in the overall model, both unconditionally and conditional on the set of controls. There is also evidence of increased odds of studying these subjects in single-sex schools of either type, especially all-female schools. There is also an association with the SES intake of the school of a similar magnitude of that seen for the individual-level SES association. Even after accounting for these other characteristics, those in areas with selective schools see a significant reduction in the odds of studying at least three facilitating subjects, while those actually in selective schools have almost four times the odds of doing so.

Whether individuals study any applied subjects, something that is true of 31% of the sample, has a contrasting set of associations. There is a negative relationship between individuals’ prior attainment at age 11 and the odds of studying any applied subjects: individuals with a one standard deviation higher KS2 score have 14% lower odds of studying an applied subject. There is no significant difference between individual’s SES and the odds of studying an applied subject, although a negative relationship was observed in our survey data model, perhaps due to the stronger measurement of SES in that setting. One area of similarity between the overall selectivity model and the applied subjects model is that male students have reduced odds of studying any applied subjects. Likewise, individuals in all-male schools see one-quarter the odds of studying applied subjects compared to their peers in gender balanced schools; a similar (but not quite as large) reduction is also evident in all-female schools. Again, the differences associated with selective schooling are stark: those in selective schools have over 90% lower odds of studying any applied subjects than peers in non-selective schools.

Another difference that stands out is the much greater value of intra-cluster correlation, compared to the general selectivity score model. Less than half of the variance is between individuals within the same schools, perhaps suggesting schools play a particularly large role in whether or not individuals study such subjects. Furthermore, this influence of schools seems divorced from the make-up of schools in this case, since the conditional intra-cluster correlation is only marginally lower than the unconditional one.

We next turn to the model of whether individuals are studying the full set of subjects that would be necessary to be eligible for the English Baccalaureate (EBacc) (if they were in a later cohort and also go on to reach the required standard in the qualifications). Just over a third (37%) of the sample met this criteria. Individuals with one standard deviation higher KS2 scores have over twice the odds of studying the EBacc subjects, while those in schools with one standard deviation higher KS2 intake see their odds of studying all EBacc subjects increase by over two and a half times. Individuals with one standard deviation higher SES have approximately 25% increased odds; those in schools with a one standard deviation higher intake in terms of SES see their odds increase by just over 50%. These are broadly similar patterns to those seen in the models of general subject selectivity in terms of the significance of individuals’ prior attainment and SES, and schools’ average intake in terms of these two characteristics.

The individual-level gender difference is much smaller than for the overall selectivity score but, once again, there is a much increased chance of taking EBacc subjects in all-male or all-female schools. Again, the presence of selective schooling in an area appears to be associated with a reduced probability of taking a full set of subjects, except in selective schools themselves where the opposite is true. Finally, schools also appear to play a bigger role in explaining whether young people study EBacc subjects than they did in explaining our general selectivity score, with an conditional intra-cluster correlation of 0.37.

We next consider variation in whether individuals study three separate sciences. This is the smallest group that we consider, at only 8% of the sample. Once again, those in selective schools are more likely to study triple sciences, even after taking into account other characteristics in the model. Students in all-male schools see 10% increased odds of studying this subject combination, relative to those in gender balanced schools while, in contrast to other subject combinations we have considered, there is no significant change in the odds among pupils in all-female schools. Even holding these influences of school constant, boys are a little more likely to study three separate sciences than their female peers. This is in contrast to findings for overall academic subject selectivity, where male pupils were more likely to study less academically selective subjects.

Conditional on the covariates in the model, and in contrast to the other subject combination models, whether individuals study triple sciences sees a lower proportion of variation explained by schools to that estimated in overall selectivity score, with a conditional intra-cluster correlation of 0.22.

7. Conclusions

This paper has explored the issue of variation in young people’s subject choices by assessing the extent to which individuals’ decisions are associated with the school they attend. However, it also goes beyond this, to explore the importance of constraints on schools, especially how schools’ provision (and, hence, the options open to their pupils) is shaped by their composition in terms of academic attainment, socioeconomic background, and gender mix.

This work includes no analysis of later outcomes following from studying more academically selective subjects or, indeed, the particular combinations of subjects on which we have focused. This paper does not offer evidence on whether there are particular benefits from particular combinations of subjects choice. It is, however, something considered in other contexts by other articles in this issue, which suggest that subject choices do affect educational transitions (Dilnot, Citation2018; Moulton, Henderson, Anders, & Sullivan, Citation2018), and to which we plan to return in future work. As such, our conclusions in this paper offer no judgement on whether we should consider the differences that we document to be positive or negative, only that they exist.

Our results replicate previous findings that young people’s prior attainment, socioeconomic background, and gender are all associated with the subjects they study at age 14–16 (Henderson et al., Citation2018). However, our novel contribution is to consider the separate associations between the composition of their school in these terms and subjects studied. We find that individuals in schools with more advantaged intakes are more likely to study more academically selective subjects, even after conditioning on individuals’ own SES. Individuals’ prior attainment is associated with studying more academically selective subjects as, again, is the prior attainment of the school more generally. Boys are less likely to study academically selective subjects than their female counterparts, although there is also evidence of higher subject selectivity in single-sex schools of either type.

Overall, schools explain about a third of the variation in the academic selectivity of the subjects that young people study; once we take into account the demographics of the school this is reduced to closer to a quarter. We suggest two potential mechanisms. First, schools may try to offer a curriculum which they deem appropriate for the socioeconomic composition of the school; in other words, schools may deliberately take school SES into account when choosing the subjects offered. Second, we know that schools serving poor children face difficulties in recruiting and retaining highly qualified staff (Lupton, Citation2005; Lupton & Thrupp, Citation2012; Ofsted, Citation2013) particularly in shortage areas such as languages and sciences. This may constrain the curriculum that schools with disadvantaged intakes are able to offer. The strength of the school SES effect is surprising, and has potential policy implications, as it suggests an additional pathway through which school SES influences academic outcomes for pupils. The negative association between being in a local authority in which a significant proportion of pupils attend selective schools and the academic selectivity of subjects that individuals study raises the possibility that local education markets (for pupils and teachers) also impose constraints on the subjects that schools may viably offer. Qualitative evidence presented elsewhere in this special issue provides insights into the ways in which the social class composition of the school can influence school practices (Barrance & Elwood, Citation2018; Smyth, Citation2018).

There are some important differences when we consider some examples of whether individuals study for specific sets of subjects that it has been argued may be important for future academic outcomes. The odds of studying any applied subjects are lower among pupils in schools with a higher prior attaining intake. While it is the case that in most single-sex schools pupils are more likely to study a more academically selective set of subjects, for studying triple sciences this is only true in all-male schools and not all-female ones.

Overall, this paper has highlighted the important role that schools seem to play in many subject choice decisions, with significant variation in subjects studied attributable to the school level. However, it also highlights that, in many cases, what we might see as schools’ actions are, in fact, strongly associated with contextual factors, as captured by their intake in terms of prior attainment, gender, and socioeconomic status; one plausible pathway for this would be that intake imposes constraints on what schools can viably offer. The socioeconomic composition of the school is a powerful predictor of individual choices in our models, even controlling for the academic composition of the school. In fact, the SES composition of the school has a similar strength of relationship with subject selectivity as the individual’s family SES.

These findings have a number of implications. First, they highlight that we should be sceptical of considering young people’s subjects of study purely in terms of ‘choice’ (Woods, Citation1976). They are, at most, constrained choices, potentially both for individuals and for schools. Second, the findings regarding subjects studied in schools within selective local authorities suggest that expanding selective education will increase inequality in, and decrease the average level of, the academic selectivity of the subjects that young people study.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes on contributors

Jake Anders is Senior Research Fellow in Education, Evaluation, and Inequality in the Department of Learning and Leadership, UCL Institute of Education. His research focuses on the causes and consequences of educational inequality and the evaluation of policies and programmes aiming to reduce it.

Morag Henderson is a Senior Lecturer in Sociology at the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Department of Social Science, UCL Institute of Education. Her research interests focus on explaining educational differentials, these explanations include mental health and wellbeing, academic self-concept, schooling differences, and the influence of the family.

Vanessa Moulton is a Research Fellow at the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Department of Social Science, UCL Institute of Education. Her research interests focus on the influence of children’s cognition, aspirations, behaviour, and family background on later educational, economic, and mental health outcomes.

Alice Sullivan is Professor of Sociology at the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Department of Social Science, UCL Institute of Education. She is also Director of the 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70). Her work focuses on social and educational inequalities.

Acknowledgements

Supported by The Nuffield Foundation under grant EDU/42169. The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed charitable trust that aims to improve social wellbeing in the widest sense. It funds research and innovation in education and social policy and also works to build capacity in education, science, and social science research. The Nuffield Foundation has funded this project, but the views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the Foundation. More information is available at www.nuffieldfoundation.org. Morag Henderson, Vanessa Moulton, and Alice Sullivan’s time on this work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (Grant number: ES/M008584/1). Thanks to Catherine Dilnot, among others, for helpful comments and suggestions.

References

  • Anders, J., Henderson, M., Moulton, V., & Sullivan, A. (2017). The role of schools in explaining individuals’ subject choices at age 14 ( CLS Working Paper). London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, UCL Institute of Education.
  • Barrance, R. & Elwood, J. (2018). Inequalities and the curriculum: Young people’s views on choice and fairness through their experiences of curriculum as examination specifications at GCSE. Oxford Review of Education, 44(1).
  • Bell, J. F. (2001). Patterns of subject uptake and examination entry 1984–1997. Educational Studies, 27, 201–219.
  • Burgess, S., Propper, C., Slater, H., & Wilson, D. (2005). Who wins and who loses from school accountability? The distribution of educational gain in English secondary schools ( CMPO Working Paper 05/128). Bristol: Centre for Market and Public Organisation, University of Bristol.
  • Chowdry, H., Crawford, C., Dearden, L., Goodman, A., & Vignoles, A. (2013). Widening participation in higher education: Analysis using linked administrative data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A, 176, 431–457.
  • Codiroli, N. (2015). Inequalities in students’ choice of STEM subjects ( CLS Working Paper 2015/6). London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, UCL Institute of Education.
  • Coe, R., Searle, J., Barmby, P., Jones, K., & Higgins, S. (2008). Relative difficulty of examinations in different subjects (CEM Centre Summary Report). Durham: Centre for Evaluation and Monitoring, Durham University.
  • Coleman, J. S., Campbell, E. Q., McPartland, J., Mood, A. M., Weinfeld, F. D., & York, R. L. (1966). Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics, US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
  • Davies, P., Adnett, N., & Turnbull, A. (2003). Market forces and diversity: Some evidence from the 14–19 curriculum. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 35, 479–498.
  • Davies, P., Telhaj, S., Hutton, D., Adnett, N., & Coe, R. (2008). Socioeconomic background, gender and subject choice in secondary schooling. Educational Research, 50, 235–248.
  • Dilnot, C. (2018). The relationship between A-level subject choice and league table score of university attended: The ‘facilitating’, the ‘less suitable’ and the counter-intuitive. Oxford Review of Education, 44(1).
  • Francis, B. (2000). The gendered subject: Students’ subject preferences and discussions of gender and subject ability. Oxford Review of Education, 26, 35–48.
  • Henderson, M., Sullivan, A., Anders, J., & Moulton, V. (2018). Social class, gender and ethnic differences in subjects taken at age 14. The Curriculum Journal. doi:10.1080/09585176.2017.1406810.
  • Henderson, V., Mieszkowski, P., & Sauvageau, Y. (1978). Peer group effects and educational production functions. Journal of Public Economics, 10, 97–106.
  • Hodgson, A., & Spours, K. (2008). 14–19 education and training: Curriculum, qualifications and organisation. London: Sage.
  • Hoxby, C. (2000). Peer effects in the classroom: Learning from gender and race variation ( NBER Working Paper 7867). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.
  • Iannelli, C. (2013). The role of the school curriculum in social mobility. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 34, 907–928.
  • Jin, W., Muriel, A., & Sibieta, L. (2011). Subject and course choices at ages 14 and 16 amongst young people in England: Insights from behavioural economics (DfE Research Report DFE-RR160). London: Department for Education.
  • Kolenikov, S., & Angeles, G. (2009). Socioeconomic status measurement with discrete proxy variables: Is principal component analysis a reliable answer? Review of Income and Wealth, 55, 128–165.
  • Lavy, V., & Schlosser, A. (2011). Mechanisms and impacts of gender peer effects at school. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 3(2), 1–33.
  • Lupton, R. (2005). Social justice and school improvement: Improving the quality of schooling in the poorest neighbourhoods. British Educational Research Journal, 31, 589–604.
  • Lupton, R., & Thrupp, M. (2012). Headteachers’ readings of and responses to disadvantaged contexts: Evidence from English primary schools. British Educational Research Journal, pp. 1–20.
  • Marks, G. N. (2006). Are between- and within-school differences in student performance largely due to socio-economic background? Evidence from 30 countries. Educational Research, 48, 21–40.
  • Marks, G. N. (2015). Are school-SES effects statistical artefacts? Evidence from longitudinal population data. Oxford Review of Education, 41, 122–144.
  • McCrone, T., Morris, M., & Walker, M. (2005). Pupil choice at Key Stage 3—Literature review (NFER Report). Slough: National Foundation for Educational Research.
  • Mortimore, P. (1988). School matters. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
  • Moulton, V., Henderson, M., Anders, J., & Sullivan, A. (2018). Does what you study at age 14–16 matter for educational transitions post-16? Oxford Review of Education, 44(1).
  • Mundlak, Y. (1978). On the pooling of time series and cross section data. Econometrica, 46, 69–85.
  • Ofsted. (2013). The report of Her Majesty’s Chief Inspector of Education, Children’s Services and Skills 2013/14: Schools (Annual Report). London: Ofsted.
  • Olsson, U. (1979). Maximum likelihood estimation of the polychoric correlation coefficient. Psychometrika, 44, 443–460.
  • Open Public Services Network. (2015). Lack of options: How a pupil’s academic choices are affected by where they live (OPSN Report). London: Open Public Services Network.
  • Proud, S. (2014). Girl power? An analysis of peer effects using exogenous changes in the gender make-up of the peer group. Regional and Sectoral Economic Studies, 14, 5–18.
  • Russell Group. (2013). Informed choices: A Russell Group guide to making decisions about post-16 education. Russell Group guide.
  • Rutter, M. (1982). Fifteen thousand hours: Secondary schools and their effects on children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Smith, D. J., Tomlinson, S., Bonnerjea, L., Hogarth, T., & Thomes, H. (1989). The school effect: A study of multi-racial comprehensives. London: Policy Studies Institute.
  • Smyth, E. (2018). Working at a different level? Curriculum differentiation in Irish lower secondary education. Oxford Review of Education, 44(1).
  • Sullivan, A. (2009). Academic self concept, gender and single sex schooling. British Educational Research Journal, 35, 259–288.
  • Sullivan, A., Joshi, H., & Leonard, D. (2009). Single-sex schooling and academic attainment at school and through the lifecourse. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 6–36.
  • Sullivan, A., Zimdars, A., & Heath, A. (2010). The social structure of the 14–16 curriculum in England. International Studies in Sociology of Education, 20, 5–21.
  • Tripney, J., Newman, M., Bangpan, M., Niza, C., MacKintosh, M., & Sinclair, J. (2010). Subject choice in STEM: Factors influencing young people (aged 14–19) in education. A systematic review of the UK literature ( Wellcome Trust Report). London: EPPI Centre, Social Science Research Unit, Institute of Education, University of London.
  • Van Houtte, M. (2004). Gender context of the school and study culture, or how the presence of girls affects the achievement of boys. Educational Studies, 30, 409–423.
  • Willms, J. D. (1986). Social class segregation and its relationship to pupils’ examination results in Scotland. American Sociological Review, 51, 224–241.
  • Woods, P. (1976). The myth of subject choice. British Journal of Sociology, 27, 130–149.