4,303
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Recognising autism in girls within the education context: reflecting on the internal presentation and the diagnostic criteria

ORCID Icon &
Pages 561-581 | Received 27 Jun 2023, Accepted 14 Sep 2023, Published online: 24 Sep 2023

ABSTRACT

It is widely acknowledged that girls are less likely to receive an autism diagnosis compared to boys (Gould, J. 2017. “Towards Understanding the Under-Recognition of Girls and Women on the Autism Spectrum.” Autism 21 (6): 703–705), with growing recognition that girls are underserved by the current clinical diagnostic criteria, potentially due to differences in presentation (Duvekot, J., J. van der Ende, F. C. Verhulst, G. Slappendel, E. van Daalen, A. Maras, and K. Greaves-Lord. 2017. “Factors Influencing the Probability of a Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Girls Versus Boys.” Autism 21 (6): 646–658). Lack of awareness of the female profile of autism among teachers and other professionals can have implications in terms of recognition, diagnostic referrals, formal identification and support. A narrative review approach was adopted to review extant literature pertinent to sex differences in autism, using the diagnostic criteria outlined in DSM-5 as a framework (American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington: American Psychiatric Association). The aim of this paper is to raise awareness of the varied presentation exhibited by some autistic girls. The findings reveal differences in the presentation of autism characteristics among girls with a more internal presentation apparent. The findings of the review indicate the need for revisions to diagnostic tools to reflect the more nuanced female presentation, increased training and guidance for professionals working with autistic girls and continued research. It is hoped that this review will further knowledge in this area to support the recognition of autism in females and mitigate the risk of females being missed, misdiagnosed or diagnosed later in life.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum DisorderFootnote1, henceforth autism, is characterised by differences in social communication and interaction and restricted interests and repetitive behaviours (APA Citation2013). Historically, autism was perceived as a ‘male disorder’ (Estrin et al. Citation2021) and up until recently it could be argued that the production of knowledge about autism could be regarded as the production of knowledge about male autism (Moore et al. Citation2022; Saxe Citation2017). This perspective has been challenged of late with attention drawn to the androcentrism of the diagnostic criteria (Kopp and Gillberg Citation1992) and the sampling bias in research (Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013).

SexFootnote2 differences in the prevalence of autism have been evident since the seminal reports into autism were published and the initial conceptualisations of autism were largely informed by research on male dominated samples (Asperger Citation1944; Kanner Citation1943). This has ultimately influenced the development of an autism profile based on male presentations. It is this profile that has informed the diagnostic criteria and screening tools used today (Carpenter, Happé, and Egerton Citation2019), however, it has been suggested that some autistic females do not fit this profile of autism (Wing Citation1981). As a result, it has been noted that the diagnostic process may lack validity in the identification of autistic girls (Cheslack-Postava and Jordan-Young Citation2012). It can be argued that a gender disparity exists in autism diagnosis with a review of epidemiological studies (Fombonne Citation2005) indicating male to female ratios ranging from 1.4:1 to 15.7:1, with 4:1 often cited (Fombonne Citation2009). More recent statistics estimate that the gender ratio may be closer to 3:1 (Loomes, Hull, and Mandy Citation2017). It is reported that girls with a co-occurring intellectual disability are more likely to be diagnosed with the ratio reducing to 2:1 (Fombonne Citation2003). Meanwhile, those with an average to higher IQ or with no language delays may go unidentified (APA Citation2013) with a more pronounced gender gap evident for this cohort of girls (Fombonne Citation1999; Fombonne Citation2009; Pisula et al. Citation2017; Tierney, Burns, and Kilbey Citation2016).

It is apparent that there exists a gender bias in autism diagnosis with autistic females being missed, misdiagnosed or diagnosed later in life (Carpenter, Happé, and Egerton Citation2019; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). There is evidence to suggest that the disparity in diagnosis places the individual at risk of not getting access to the necessary support (Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011), and can have significant implications for development and life outcomes (Taylor and DaWalt Citation2020). This paper will consider the gender bias in autism with reference to the education context. While a diagnosis is ultimately made by multidisciplinary teams, teachers play an important role in identifying children for a referral for a diagnosis (Farmer et al. Citation2003; O’Byrne Citation2018). Research states that autistic boys are referred for diagnosis 10 times more often than autistic girls (Wilkinson Citation2008). The aforementioned raises questions in relation to teacher awareness of how autism may present in girls. It is necessary to reflect on whether gaps in teacher knowledge and understanding are playing a role in the under identification of autistic girls.

There has been a positive shift in autism research to include a focus on sex/gender differences in autism in recent times (Mandy and Lai Citation2017). This research has enabled the development of a more comprehensive profile of autism in females (Wassell and Burke Citation2022), a profile that is not currently reflected in the DSM-5 or ICD-11 criteria. This may be one reason why girls remain under researched, under-identified and under-supported (Haney and Cullen Citation2017). Furthermore, it could be argued that there exists a discrepancy between developments in research and practice on the ground, which may account for inconsistencies in teacher knowledge.

This paper focuses on exploring an internal presentation of autism often linked to females diagnosed at Level 1 (APA Citation2013), who may display average to higher IQ with no language delays. The purpose of refining the discussion with reference to this cohort is that it is typically reflective of those who are educated in mainstream education settings. The overarching aim is to demonstrate the ways in which this internal presentation can be mapped onto the diagnostic criteria to better support educators recognise and support autistic girls in the education context.

Methodology

A narrative review approach was employed for the current paper as it was deemed appropriate to synthesise the extant literature on gender differences in autism and more specifically the presentation of autism in females (West and Martin Citation2023). It is hoped that this review could provide clarity for professionals, and researchers alike, in relation to how the female presentation of autism maps onto the diagnostic criteria.

The narrative review approach facilitated the inclusion of a broad range of literature types including research studies, literature reviews papers, theoretical and discussion papers, books and book chapters (Coughlan and Cronin Citation2021). An initial literature search was conducted for articles discussing sex/gender differences in autism and the female presentation of autism. Key terms included in the search were ‘Autism’, ‘Autism Spectrum Disorder’, ‘Autism Spectrum’, ‘girls’, ‘women’, ‘female phenotype’, ‘autism diagnosis’, ‘sex/gender differences’. Additional articles were identified through snowballing (Wohlin Citation2014) and consultation with other researchers. In keeping with a narrative review design, the authors did not apply strict eligibility criteria (Ferrari Citation2015), with the search and selection phase focused on the relevance of the articles (West and Martin Citation2023). Relevant content from books was included and autobiographical accounts for autistic individuals in books and/or book chapters were cited to ensure the inclusion of the autistic voice.

Findings

The literature search identified a body of research describing sex/gender differences in autism. The following will discuss the concept of a female profile of autism and the implications of gender-biased knowledge in relation to this. The body of the review will then explore the characteristics of autism in girls with reference to the diagnostic criteria.

Autism and girls: a different presentation

There is some debate around the notion of whether there exists a female phenotype of autism or are the differences in presentation due to the socialisation of females compared to males (Kreiser and White Citation2014). While it is beyond the scope of this paper to explore this debate, it is apparent that autistic girls can exhibit a different presentation of autism compared to autistic boys (Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011; Lai et al. Citation2011). It has been posited that some autistic girls experience a more internal presentation of autism whereby their autistic traits are more inconspicuous compared to a more ‘stereotypical’ presentation (Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013; Rivet and Matson Citation2011; Wassell and Burke Citation2022). In addition to this, there is also evidence to suggest that autistic girls engage with camouflaging or masking to hide their autistic traits and/or use compensatory behaviours to fulfil neurotypical social ‘norms’ (Attwood Citation2007; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). While this practice is not exclusive to autistic females, it has been reported as more prevalent in females compared to autistic males (Hull et al. Citation2017).

The characteristics associated with the internal presentation of autism and engagement with camouflaging undeniably plays a role in the ability of others to recognise and support autistic girls (Carpenter, Happé, and Egerton Citation2019), which may increase the likelihood of autistic girls being diagnosed later or in some cases missed altogether (Gould Citation2017) These concerns are grounded in research with reports highlighting that autistic females are consistently diagnosed later than their male counterparts (Begeer et al. Citation2012; Happé et al. Citation2016; Russell et al. Citation2022), and that more autistic women are being referred for diagnosis as adults compared to autistic men (Giarelli et al. Citation2010). It is worthwhile to reflect on these findings with reference to the education context, considering the role of educators in recognising autism in girls, gender-biased knowledge and its implications.

Gender biased knowledge in the education context

Recognition precedes diagnosis (Driver and Chester Citation2021) and as previously discussed, teachers play a significant role in recognising and referring students for autism diagnoses. Research examining teacher knowledge in relation to recognising autism in girls is sparse, however, two studies, both adopting a vignette method, aimed to investigate whether teachers could identify autism in girls (Ward, Curtis, and Seehagen Citation2022; Whitlock et al. Citation2020). Contradictory findings presented, however, the limitation of one study may account for the differences. A United Kingdom study by Whitlock et al. (Citation2020) suggests that primary school educational staff were more likely to identify autism in males than females and appeared to demonstrate greater awareness of the male presentation. Moreover, the results indicated that the participants were better able to identify autistic girls displaying a male-based, traditional profile of autism compared to girls with an internal presentation. Interestingly, a second study based on a cohort of teachers in New Zealand suggested that the perceived gender of the child did not influence the identification of autism, indicating that teacher knowledge was not gender biased (Ward, Curtis, and Seehagen Citation2022). However, it was noted that the inclusion of externalising behaviours, typically associated with the male-based profile, in the vignettes may have skewed the results. These studies imply that while teachers may have some understanding of autism, their knowledge is somewhat influenced by a gendered profile of autism, and they may be less aware of the internal presentation of autism and lack knowledge of how autistic girls can meet the diagnostic profile in a different way to autistic boys. A third study conducted by Gray et al. (Citation2021) used a self-report questionnaire to gather the views of SENCosFootnote3 in the United Kingdom on their knowledge of the presentation of autism in girls and their ability to recognise autism in girls and support their needs. The majority of SENCos were aware that autism presented differently in males and females, however, when detailing the characteristics they associated with autism in girls, a gendered knowledge base became apparent (Gray et al. Citation2021).

If educators are unable to recognise autistic girls who experience an internal presentation of autism, disparities in referral rates will persist and girls will continue be marginalised in terms of accessing a diagnosis and in turn, post-diagnostic supports. Moreover, a lack of teacher knowledge and understanding in relation to autism in girls may result in inadequate or non-existent support provision which may have negative implications in terms of opportunity to benefit from, experience success in and thrive in their education. These concerns are reflected in research conducted with autistic girls in relation to their educational experiences. Difficulties accessing the curriculum, transitions across and within settings, the interactional and sensory environments and feelings of anxiety are consistently reported in literature (Cridland et al. Citation2014; Goodall and MacKenzie Citation2019; Jarman and Rayner Citation2015; Moyse and Porter Citation2015; Sproston, Sedgewick, and Crane Citation2017; Tomlinson, Bond, and Hebron Citation2021). Moreover, the significance of teacher knowledge and understanding in relation to autism in girls was identified as a significant factor influencing school experiences (Tomlinson, Bond, and Hebron Citation2020), consistent with the concerns of the author and the aforementioned research.

Taken together, these findings indicate that gaps in teacher knowledge impact autistic girls’ school experiences in terms of recognition of characteristics, understanding of needs and the provision of support. The author concludes that specific guidance in terms of how the characteristics of autism present for autistic girls is required for teachers to ensure equitable and appropriate provision for autistic girls.

Interrogating the dyad of difference in autism diagnosis

Autism is diagnosed from observation of specific behaviours as detailed in the diagnostic criteria.Footnote4 These ‘behaviours’ exist in two core domainsFootnote5, namely differences in social communication and interaction and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests, or activities (RRBI).

Despite the disparity between the sexes in the prevalence of autism, it is only in recent years that research has placed a specific focus on sex/gender differences in autism (Lai et al. Citation2015). This research has revealed differences in the clinical presentation of autism between the sexes (Bond and Hebron Citation2019; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011; Lai et al. Citation2015). However, there is no consideration afforded to these potential differences in the diagnostic criteria (Giarelli et al. Citation2010; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). It is clear that the historical male bias in autism knowledge together with the lack of consideration of sex differences in presentation in the DSM-5, have hugely influenced the diagnostic process for autistic females (Estrin et al. Citation2021; Mandy and Lai Citation2017). Moreover, there is evidence to suggest that some professionals, in both clinical and education contexts, are unaware of how autism may present in autistic girls compared to their male counterparts (Estrin et al. Citation2021; Gray et al. Citation2021; Kopp and Gillberg Citation1992; Ward, Curtis, and Seehagen Citation2022; Whitlock et al. Citation2020), adding a further barrier for autistic girls in terms of referral and diagnosis.

The following section will explore the way in which autistic girls can meet the different criterion within the two core domains of difference, as identified in the DSM-5 (APA Citation2013) to support recognition and identification of autism in girls.

Social communication and interaction

Social Communication and Interaction is the first domain of difference and individuals must experience differences in all three diagnostic criteria (Table 1 – Supplementary Material). There is a body of evidence to support the differences in social communication and interaction between autistic males and females, however, there are some conflicting findings (Andersson, Gillberg, and Miniscalco Citation2012; Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013; Rivet and Matson Citation2011; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. Citation2014). The criteria within this domain will now be discussed individually with reference to extant literature on gender differences in autism.

Social and emotional reciprocity

Reciprocity refers to the back-and-forth action between communication partners required for social communication and interaction. A review of gender differences in autism carried out by Rivet and Matson (Citation2011) claimed that autistic girls exhibit fewer difficulties with reciprocal interaction than boys, while a systematic review and meta-analysis by Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. (Citation2014) found no significant differences in terms of social interactions and communication. However, considering that autistic girls with an average IQ are diagnosed at a later stage/misdiagnosed, it was suggested by the authors that the studies included in the meta-analysis may have missed this cohort and therefore a true conclusion could be that females with ASD show better social communication and social behaviour than males with autism (Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. Citation2014). This was also alluded to in another study of pre-school autistic children where it was suggested that perhaps girls with a higher IQ, better language and superficially better social skills will only be identified later in life (Andersson, Gillberg, and Miniscalco Citation2012). This theory has been supported more recently by findings from other researchers (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017; Lai et al. Citation2017). A study by Dean, Harwood, and Kasari (Citation2017) posited that the social challenges of girls appeared less obvious than those of boys, stating that autistic girls ‘looked like’ typically developing girls from the outside. It is worthwhile considering this idea with reference to the work of Lai et al. (Citation2017) and their conceptualisation of camouflaging as a discrepancy between the ‘external’ behavioural presentation and the individual's internal status. This emphasises the importance of professionals communicating with autistic girls and learning more about how they feel in relation to social situations rather than observations alone. Therefore, despite the difficulties of autistic girls being less obvious, the social domain is identified as a core area of difficulty for autistic girls (Gould Citation2017). These finding allude to the ability of autistic girls to mask their difficulties with social communication and interaction which may result in them being less likely to be identified by professionals, in turn leading to underdiagnosis in this population (Lai et al. Citation2017). Furthermore, as autistic girls are often able to hide their difficulties with social reciprocity to onlookers, the importance of teachers being aware of the difference in the way boys and girls meet this criterion differently is significant (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014).

It is widely reported that autistic girls are more socially motivated than autistic boys (Lai et al. Citation2015) and have demonstrated a greater ability to engage in reciprocal conversation (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). Girls may experience greater success in conversation as they are more likely to engage with interest sharing (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). Autistic females also appear to communicate well and can make good eye contact and engage with appropriate body language (Lawson Citation2017). This outward appearance of ‘good communication’ is grounded in neurotypical expectations and may not come naturally or automatically to autistic girls (Mandy and Lai Citation2017). However, it has been noted that autistic girls are at an advantage to boys here as they are experts in observing and copying what they think/feel they should be doing (Mandy and Lai Citation2017; Wassell and Burke Citation2022). Reflections provided by autistic female adults indicated that they demonstrated an ability to ‘learn’ the social rules of neurotypical interaction and ‘mimic’ the expected social behaviours (Milner et al. Citation2019). This ability to engage with social imitation is evidenced in the accounts of many autistic female adults as they reflect on their experience pre- and post-diagnosis (Baldwin and Costley Citation2016; Bargiela, Steward, and Mandy Citation2016; Tierney, Burns, and Kilbey Citation2016), while insights shared by autistic girls (Buckingham Citation2019; Salter Citation2019) detail the processes involved in meeting social expectations.

As with non-autistic individuals, natural personality differences also need to be considered in this domain appreciating that while some girls can be quite sociable and enjoy chatting, others may be passive and shy (Gillberg Citation2019; Wassell and Burke Citation2022). However, it is more widely reported that autistic girls often do not engage in meaningless chatting and often experience difficulties engaging with social niceties (Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). The complexity of masking, engagement in social imitation and individual preferences emphasise the importance of moving past surface-level observation and eliciting autistic girls’ inner experiences in order to understand differences in these areas. This, in turn, will help teachers to recognise and provide tailored support appropriate to the needs of autistic girls.

Developing and maintaining friendships

It has been acknowledged that autistic young people experience difficulties with developing and maintaining friendships (Carrington, Templeton, and Papinczak Citation2003). Given that gender differences in neurotypical friendships are well established with female friendships centred around emotional sharing and male friendships based on shared activities (Aukett, Ritchie, and Mill Citation1988), it is interesting to reflect on whether these gender differences play a role in how autistic individuals experience friendships. This criterion is extremely pertinent in the education context in terms of friendships within the classroom and the more unstructured period of lunchtime.

It is reported that autistic girls displayed significant difficulties in areas such as friendships, reciprocal interaction and group play but had fewer difficulties in communication compared to boys (Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013; Rivet and Matson Citation2011). It also appears that girls present with a different manifestation of friendship problems to boys (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). When taken together these findings indicate that while the ability to mask/imitate can facilitate the appearance of successful communication, the complexity of interactions involved in friendship, particularly female friendships, are significantly more challenging to navigate.

A study examining the social networks of school-aged children with and without autism, discovered that while boys were more overtly excluded, autistic girls appeared to be overlooked and not specifically accepted or rejected in social networks, raising concern in terms of the development of friendships (Dean et al. Citation2014). The above findings indicate that it may be easier to identify challenges in friendship in autistic boys compared to autistic girls, a point supported by further research by Dean, Harwood, and Kasari (Citation2017). The social landscape of female friendships could be described as a protective factor with literature highlighting that the nature of these social groups concealed autistic girls by enabling them to float on the periphery (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017; Kreiser and White Citation2014; Wassell and Burke Citation2022). Autistic girls tend to stay in close proximity to these social groups, something that may result in their difficulties going unnoticed by observers, particularly teachers in busy classrooms. This contrasts sharply with the social landscape of male social groups whereby researchers more easily identified boys with autism as they tended to wander alone separate from structured games or groups (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017). This finding is significant in the education context as the class teacher may not always be the observer on yard and therefore it requires all teachers to be attuned to the more subtle challenges experienced by autistic girls.

While autistic girls’ experiences of friendship groups may go unnoticed by adults, their differences may not be hidden from their non-autistic peers (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017). Analysis of time spent in joint engagement versus solitary, indicates that autistic girls stay near the social group and gain access to these groups through masking their social challenges. However, they do not appear to be able to maintain engagement with their typically developing peers, perhaps due to their difficulties with recognising and interpreting the social cues. This idea is reinforced by research conducted with autistic adults reflecting on their social experiences as adolescents who discussed the complexity of social rules and their implications for maintaining friendships (Baldwin and Costley Citation2016; Bargiela, Steward, and Mandy Citation2016). Despite this, research has clearly indicated that autistic girls’ desire friendship (Sedgewick et al. Citation2016; Sedgewick et al. Citation2019).

The above findings support the idea that autistic girls are more likely to be able to initiate interaction and indeed friendship than autistic boys (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). However, their difficulties appear to lie most substantially in the maintenance of these friendships (Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011; Morewood, Tomlinson, and Bond Citation2019). While difficulties may not appear to be as obvious in the earlier years of the girls’ life, they become more apparent in adolescence as the nature of female friendships change throughout the school years (Attwood and Garnett Citation2019). Difficulties with friendship maintenance has been attributed to autistic girls’ needs for control over play, a challenge that is probably more prevalent in earlier childhood friendships (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). Additionally, non-autistic girls reach more complex social and linguistic stages earlier than boys (Barbu, Cabanes, and Le Maner-Idrissi Citation2011; Bond and Hebron Citation2019), and therefore it can be expected that the complexity of their friendships increases in line with this. This can have significant implications for autistic girls as they attempt to navigate these complex interactions. Complex interactions such as gossiping, exclusion and betrayal of trust are common in adolescent girls’ friendships, however, autistic girls experience more difficulties in relation to understanding and managing these interactions (Sedgewick et al. Citation2016). Further research by Sedgewick et al. (Citation2019) revealed that autistic girls experienced more conflict than neurotypical girls and both autistic and non-autistic boys and this in turn resulted in anxiety for the girls, a finding that is echoed in first-hand accounts given by autistic females (Buckingham Citation2019; Milner et al. Citation2019). The demands of maintaining friendships are reinforced by autistic females who described having one or two close friends, acknowledging that friendships are arduous (Milner et al. Citation2019). This is consistent with observations from other researchers (Attwood and Garnett Citation2019; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011; Horlock Citation2019), and could be best explained by the Double Empathy theory (Milton Citation2012; Milton, Gurbuz, and López Citation2022).

Another consideration within the context of friendship is that of play differences and how they vary in autistic girls. It is reported that some girls demonstrate more appropriate play behaviour compared to boys, something that may be influenced by the presentation of RRBIs and/or their communicative abilities (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017; Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013). However, other research reported that some autistic girls may prefer to play with boys as their play can be simpler and more logical (Attwood and Garnett Citation2019).

The concept of friendship is inextricable with education settings, and it is therefore imperative that teachers are alert to the varied profile exhibited by autistic girls in order to recognise difficulties that may be occurring in the social realm of the classroom and provide appropriate, contextually sensitive support.

Non-verbal communication

Differences reported in nonverbal communication correlate with the premise that girls experience a more internal presentation of autism (Wassell and Burke Citation2022), compared to the more overt behavioural presentation in boys (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). Results from one study by Hiller, Young, and Weber (Citation2014) revealed that the skill of being able to integrate nonverbal and verbal gestures was predictive of being female and over one-third of girls in the study exhibited no difficulties in this area. Attwood and Garnett (Citation2019) attends to this strength in autistic girls and attributes it to their capacity to imitate non-verbal actions in order to blend in with non-autistic individuals. This need to assimilate is often most prominent in friendship networks and therefore it has been reported that autistic girls can be more animated with their peers compared to when they are with family. This is probably done in an attempt to achieve social success.

Even though girls demonstrate a better use of social gestures, they experience similar difficulties in understanding and interpreting non-verbal behaviours as their male counterparts (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). Interpreting the subtleties in facial expression, voice intonation and reading body language were identified as potential difficulties in non-verbal communication for autistic girls (Kreiser and White Citation2014). Moreover, it was posited that difficulties in the area of non-verbal communication make it harder for autistic girls to interpret more subtle interactions such as eye-rolling, shared glances and giggling all of which are inextricably linked to adolescent female friendship groups (Dean, Harwood, and Kasari Citation2017). Understandably, these challenges may lead to social confusion which can support understanding of autistic girls’ difficulties with social interaction and more specifically maintaining friendships. Knowledge and understanding of these are important for teachers to assist in the identification and referral of students for diagnosis.

Insight from autistic individuals can help conceptualise the complexity of social interaction and communication differences. Autistic author, Holliday Willey (Citation1999) juxtaposes her strengths in public speaking with her dislike of more intimate social situations, highlighting the sheer complexity of the domain of social interaction and communication and more specifically the stress associated with understanding and interpreting body language. In order to support girls, it is important that teachers are aware of the challenges autistic girls have in the area of social interaction and communication and understand the complexity of these differences for some individuals.

Restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests or activities

The second domain of difference refers to RRBIs. Specific information in relation to the criteria in this domain can be found in Table 2 in Supplementary Material. Interestingly, research has reported that the most consistent difference in presentation between genders occur in this domain (Duvekot et al. Citation2017; Knutsen et al. Citation2019; Kreiser and White Citation2014; Sipes et al. Citation2011; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. Citation2014) with females displaying quantitatively reduced RRBIs overall (Allely Citation2019; Kirkovski, Enticott, and Fitzgerald Citation2013; Lai et al. Citation2015; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. Citation2014). It is argued that RRBIs are more easily identified than difficulties in the domain of social interaction and communication (Allely Citation2019). However, the identification of characteristics in this domain is largely influenced by gender bias and the RRBIs of autistic girls are often not reflective of the traditional male-based presentation (Nowell, Jones, and Harrop Citation2019).

It should be considered that a milder presentation in this domain may in fact be mediated by the developmental quotient of the individual with those with an average developmental quotient exhibiting the least restricted, repetitive behaviours (Sipes et al. Citation2011). In light of this, an alternative perspective relating to the camouflaging hypothesis can be considered (Hull et al. Citation2017; Knutsen et al. Citation2019). Knutsen et al. (Citation2019) examined sex differences in the RRBI domain between age- and intelligence quotient-matched female and male children diagnosed with autism using the largest known sample. This study revealed more similarities rather than differences between males and females. However, decreased repetitive interests and stereotyped behaviours were observed in younger females with lower support needs. This offers support for the phenomenon that females with an average-higher IQ are more motivated/effective at camouflaging their autistic traits (Attwood Citation2007; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). This idea is further alluded to in the work of Hull et al. (Citation2017) which noted that adults with no co-occurring intellectual disability can use compensatory strategies to camouflage their restricted, repetitive behaviours. However, the complexity and sophistication of this technique was acknowledged in that it requires high levels of self-awareness for individuals to recognise their RRBIs, develop a strategy to conceal them and finally perform the compensatory behaviours in the appropriate environments. Nevertheless, this offers a unique insight into the cohort relevant to this study. The latter debate questions whether the diagnostic instruments are sensitive to the varied presentation of repetitive behaviours in girls (Gould Citation2017). These findings, when taken together, raise concerns with regards to the identification and support of autistic girls and may go some way in explaining the under recognition of this group of individuals by teachers and other professionals. In light of this, a discussion of each criterion in the RRBI domain will follow.

Repetitive behaviours

There exists a body of research indicating that females demonstrate less unusual stereotyped and repetitive behavioursFootnote6 (Kreiser and White Citation2014; Rivet and Matson Citation2011; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al. Citation2014). This is further supported by findings of a study that considered the factors influencing the probability of a diagnosis of ASD in girls versus boys and found that restricted, repetitive behaviours were less strongly associated with autism in girls than boys (Duvekot et al. Citation2017). The aforementioned raises questions in relation to the awareness of professionals working with autistic girls and their ability to recognise what constitutes repetitive behaviours in this cohort.

Research by Hiller, Young, and Weber (Citation2014) revealed that 27% of girls in their study did not meet this criterion compared to only 6% of boys. A further study surveyed clinicians involved in diagnosis and found that perceived sex-related differences were predominantly in the area of repetitive behaviours (Jamison et al. Citation2017). An emerging pattern between diagnosis age and the RRBI domain was also noted with clinicians reporting that females exhibit less ‘severe’ behaviours in relation to stereotyped or repetitive motor movements during the school-age/adolescent period (Jamison et al. Citation2017). Other research reflects on the diagnostic process proposing that it is the more passive, non-problematic behaviours observed in autistic girls compared to the overt behaviours exhibited by autistic boys that may lead to reduced rates of referral for diagnostic assessments by teachers (Kreiser and White Citation2014). This observation only further accentuates the importance of supporting teachers to recognise the varied autism profile associated with girls. These findings question whether the presentation of restricted, repetitive behaviours in girls is qualitatively different compared to boys, a hypothesis supported by observational clinician accounts (Attwood Citation2007; Bond and Hebron Citation2019; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011).

It has been noted that the repetitive behaviours of autistic girls may look very different to the stereotypical presentation associated with autistic boys i.e. lining up toys and engaging with echolalia (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014; Wassell and Burke Citation2022). Instead, some autistic girls may engage in more inconspicuous repetitive behaviours for example re-reading the same book, re-watching the same film or re-doing homework. All of these behaviours can fulfil a role of comfort and predictability, both of which are associated with engagement in repetitive behaviours (Bond and Hebron Citation2019) and are supported by the detailed autobiographical accounts of autistic girls (Salter Citation2019).

The above discussion highlights the potential varied presentation and emphasises the importance of widening teacher knowledge to enable them to look beyond the stereotyped repetitive behaviours associated with autism. It is necessary for teachers to first appreciate the function of repetitive behaviours before then recognising behaviours which may fulfil this function for autistic girls.

Inflexibility

Inflexibility is also discussed in the context of a preference for or insistence on sameness. It is reflected in this domain through a demonstration of a strong preference for environments and behaviours that are predictable for the individual and a resistance to change (Boucher Citation2017). An early study into autism alluded to the ‘aggressive’ and ‘domineering’ behaviours that may be exhibited by autistic boys in demonstrating their insistence on sameness, meeting this criterion in a more obvious manner (Kopp and Gillberg Citation1992). It was indicated that this behavioural style was not evident in the sample of autistic girls in the study and as a result it was acknowledged that the difficulties that some girls experience in this area may go unnoticed. Differences in presentation of behaviour are also discussed in the context of the externalising behaviours of boys compared to girls’ tendencies to internalise their difficulties (Bond and Hebron Citation2019). These differences have resulted in teachers reporting fewer concerns about autistic girls as they do not outwardly express their difficulties (Bond and Hebron Citation2019). However, it is important to recognise that while an outward demonstration of this criterion may not be as explicit in girls, that is not to say that autistic girls do meet this criterion.

Insight from an autistic female author Cook O’Toole (Citation2018) emphasised that taking a more holistic view of the criteria is required to appreciate that the outward presentation may differ between males and females. Regardless, the function of these behaviours may be the same. She reflected on the way order and facts provided security and predictability to her life, contrasting this with the unpredictable and changeable nature of social interactions. Inflexibility can influence the manner in which autistic girls play with their peers and can be evidenced through a tendency to be overly dominant or controlling, a nod towards the need to control the play (Attwood and Garnett Citation2019). Controlling behaviour is also referenced in relation to interactions with peers, so moving from play activities into interactions more broadly (Bond and Hebron Citation2019; Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011). In addition to this, it is reported that autistic girls may tend to be perfectionistic (Lai et al. Citation2015). When taken together and considered outside of the stereotypical connotations associated with this criterion, these traits can demonstrate a clear preference for sameness and a resistance to change. Autistic girls who exhibit a different profile of traits in relation to these criteria may still require the support of their teachers despite the lack of an outward presentation, therefore highlighting the importance of an awareness of the internal presentation of autism.

Interests

Research has acknowledged that the manifestation of restricted interests is characteristically different in girls compared to boys (Hiller, Young, and Weber Citation2014). These interest differences may be more difficult for professionals to identify and have been suggested as a reason for the under-identification of autistic girls (Attwood Citation2007). In a controlled study conducted by Solomon et al. (Citation2012), marginally significant differences were identified with boys presenting with more ‘obvious’ restricted interests than girls. The use of the word obvious refers to interests that were more typically associated with the ‘male as norm’ presentation of autism. This finding endorsed the findings of another review paper; however, it was acknowledged that the choices available on the parent report forms could have been a limitation of this study (Rivet and Matson Citation2011). A further study by Hiller, Young, and Weber (Citation2014) revealed that the interests of girls were most commonly reported as being in the ‘seemingly random’ category. It is noteworthy to reflect on the category label of ‘seemingly random’ (examples: animals, rocks) as it appears that the interests of girls were categorised under this label as they did not reflect the other categories which could be argued as being stereotypical of male based interests. These findings correlate with other research which has revealed that the restricted interests of autistic boys are often object based and technical whereas the interests of autistic girls are often people/animal oriented (Lai et al. Citation2015).

The interests of autistic girls are often viewed as more ‘typical’ and are described as similar to those of their non-autistic peers, for example interests in animals, books, celebrities (Gould and Ashtown-Smith Citation2011; Winter-Messiers Citation2007). However, the interests of autistic girls differ from their peers in terms of their intensity and the time they engage with them (Attwood Citation2007). This is a subtle difference and could go unnoticed by teachers due to lack of awareness and knowledge of the female profile of autism. Narrative descriptions from an autistic girl (Salter Citation2019) provide useful lens into a more female perspective whereby music was identified as a ‘restricted interest’ with examples provided from early years to young adulthood to support this. Insights from an autistic perspective emphasises a varied presentation where the interest was perhaps less obvious than typically associated with autism. It is apparent that although qualitatively different from autistic boys, autistic girls demonstrate equally passionate interests. Alerting teachers to the subtle differences in the interests of autistic girls and autistic boys may better equip them to focus their professional observations.

Sensory differences

The final consideration in the RRBI domain is that of sensory processing. Limited research examines the sensory differences and experiences of autistic girls and women and therefore little is known in relation to whether gender differences exist in this domain. In an investigation carried out by Lai et al. (Citation2011) of the similarities and differences between the sexes in age- and IQ-matched adults with autism, it was found that females experienced more lifetime sensory symptoms, however, it was advised that this result should be viewed as preliminary as the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), a standardised, semi-structured interview schedule based on the DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnostic concepts was not designed to be specifically sensitive to sensory symptoms and merely provides summary information. More recently, in a study by Duvekot et al. (Citation2017) examining whether behavioural characteristics influenced the probability of an autism diagnosis in a sample of children aged 2.5-10 years old, findings acknowledged that sensory symptoms were positively associated with an autism diagnosis in both boys and girls. Moreover, it was suggested that the presentation of RRBIs did not largely contribute to a diagnosis in girls (Duvekot et al. Citation2017).

Despite the inconsistent findings and limited research in this area, it is important to appreciate that sensory differences appear to be an important feature of autism for autistic individuals and are commonly reported among autistic women (Baldwin and Costley Citation2016; Bargiela, Steward, and Mandy Citation2016; Tierney, Burns, and Kilbey Citation2016). Research has acknowledged that schools can be challenging sensory environments for autistic girls (Goodall and MacKenzie Citation2019; Milner et al. Citation2019; Moyse and Porter Citation2015), therefore necessitating teachers to work with autistic girls to support their sensory needs. In addition, further research is needed to ascertain gender differences in relation to this criterion to enhance understanding and create more inclusive sensory environments.

Future directions

This narrative review has examined literature pertinent to the sex/gender differences in autism and more specifically explored the female profile of autism. The diagnostic criteria for autism as identified in the DSM-5 (APA Citation2013) was used as a framework for interrogation. The findings have indicated differences in the presentation of autism between the sexes. The research points towards a more internal presentation of autism in girls with their characteristics less obvious compared to their male counterparts. The findings of this review have significant implications for policy, practice and research in terms of recognition of and support for autistic girls and developments in autism research to improve the outcomes of autistic females.

Awareness of a varied presentation of autism is imperative for accurate identification of and support for females. In light of this, the information presented in this review requires action on behalf of policy makers in terms of the diagnostic process and post-diagnostic supports. It is accepted that the diagnostic criteria outlined in DSM-5 (APA Citation2013) are broad, however, it could be argued that their application to the varied and perhaps more nuanced female presentation could be restricted by the knowledge of professionals involved in the diagnostic process. An expert knowledge base in the presentation of autism in females along with the ability to see beyond the androcentrism of the diagnostic process and accompanying screening tools is necessary in the current context to identify of autistic girls. As a result, there is an inherent need to provide clarity with regards to how the characteristics of autistic girls meet the diagnostic criteria. While there is an emerging body of research on the female profile of autism being published, it is important this material is being disseminated and used to advance the diagnostic process. It is recommended that this research should be used to inform revisions on the current diagnostic criteria and screening tools used in the identification of autism. In addition, it is also necessary to consider the important role of the education context in identification and support of autistic girls. There is a need for guidance documents to be issued to educational contexts to ensure the needs of autistic girls are being attended to. While 2022 saw the publication of the Autism Good Practice Guideline in the Republic of Ireland, minimal information was presented in relation to the varied presentation of autism in girls. It is important that guidance appreciates the diversity of autism and its presentation in order to challenge male-biased perceptions of autism.

Teachers and school staff are critical stakeholders in terms of the identification of autism and will often be involved in the referral process. Moreover, they play a significant role in supporting the individual post-diagnosis. This review highlighted the variable knowledge of professionals in relation to autism in girls. It is apparent that there is a lack of knowledge on the presentation of autism in females among professionals with a gendered-knowledge base evident. This review revealed a varied autism profile and highlighted the subtleties of the internal presentation in girls. As a result, it is of critical importance that professionals are aware of the sex differences in autism to improve the recognition of and support for autistic girls. In summary, the findings of this review emphasise the need for training for teachers and other school staff. It is imperative that providers of Initial Teacher Education (ITE) review course content to ensure input is provided in relation to the female profile of autism. Considering the significance of this issue, it is necessary to go beyond ITE and ensure all in-service teachers are provided with the necessary knowledge to competently support autistic girls. It is important that individuals and training bodies facilitating professional development for teachers on the topic of autism update their training to ensure it is inclusive of the female presentation of autism. Furthermore, it is believed that there is a need for all school contexts to receive some input which specifically addresses the female autism profile. It is hoped that the findings presented in this paper will raise awareness of how autism may present for girls so that practitioners can be supported to identify individuals who experience a more ‘internal’ presentation of autism, facilitate appropriate support and improve outcomes for autistic individuals.

Finally, this review supports the need for further research. While there exists a growing body of research that examines sex differences in autism, there is need for ongoing research exploring how autism presents and is experienced differently in females. Furthermore, there is potential for further empirical research to examine the current diagnostic criteria and process through the lens of gender. It could be argued that much of the current research is limited due to its reliance on diagnosed samples, who, by definition meet the current criteria for autism. However, as discussed, the androcentrism of the current diagnostic criteria are perceived to be a barrier for autistic females in obtaining a diagnosis (Duvekot et al. Citation2017). Considering this, the use of population-based samples in research could prove powerful in furthering knowledge of the female profile of autism. Lastly, valuable learning could be garnered from research with female-specific samples and from larger research studies with balanced sex samples.

Conclusion

While there is a growing body of research in the area of sex/gender differences in autism, there continues to be a lack of awareness of the female presentation of autism. The lack of clarity in relation to how autistic girls may meet the current diagnostic criteria has resulted in persistent under-identification of autistic girls. It is important to acknowledge that recognition precedes diagnosis and teachers are commonly one of the first professionals involved in this process. Despite this, research suggests that teachers are not equipped with the knowledge base to recognise autism in girls, resulting in autistic girls going unrecognised and unsupported. The author concludes that it is imperative that the internal presentation of autism, commonly associated with autistic girls, is acknowledged as an alternative profile to the traditional profile of autism which is grounded in a male-dominated research base.

This paper endeavoured to bring together insights garnered from both a quantitative and qualitative research perspective in order to provide a comprehensive understanding of the presentation of autism in girls and detail how the profile of characteristics can be viewed with reference to the diagnostic criteria. It is accepted that the narrative review methodology adopted for this paper has its limitations. Broad details are provided in relation to the search strategy, however, this paper did not follow an official method for searching and retrieving the literature, a common criticism of the narrative review approach (Coughlan and Cronin Citation2021; Paré et al. Citation2015). Moreover, as with any narrative review, the sources used were selected based on perceived relevance and a systematic and comprehensive search of all literature was not carried out nor were sources given equal weight (Paré et al. Citation2015; West and Martin Citation2023). Nonetheless, it is hoped that this review will provide education practitioners and allied professionals, as well as researchers with a foundational understanding of the presentation of autism in girls.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (11.9 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by Irish Research Council [grant number GOIPG/2022/1084].

Notes on contributors

Aoife Munroe

Aoife Munroe is a PhD Candidate in the Department of Educational Psychology, Inclusion and Special Education in Mary Immaculate College (MIC), Limerick, Ireland. Aoife is an Irish Research Council Government of Ireland Scholar, and her PhD research is focused on autistic girls’ experiences of mainstream education provision at post-primary level in the Republic of Ireland. Aoife is a qualified primary school teacher and has worked as a Lecturer in Special and Inclusive Education at St. Angela’s College, Sligo and Mary Immaculate College, Limerick.

Michele Dunleavy

Michele Dunleavy is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Educational Psychology, Inclusive and Special Education, Mary Immaculate College (MIC), Limerick, Ireland. Before joining the staff in MIC Michele worked for many years as a teacher in mainstream primary, Special Education Teacher, special schools, and special classes. Additionally, Michele was seconded, as an Advisor to the Special Education Support Service where she worked with teachers from the primary and post-primary sector. Her main research area is Autism and in particular how schools support the access of autistic students to relationship and sexuality education (RSE).

Notes

1 The adoption of neurodiversity affirmative language is preferred by the author and used where possible, however, due to the nature of the review topic, less preferred terminology will be presented, particularly in relation to the diagnostic criteria, for the purpose of clarity in the discussion between how the characteristics experienced by autistic girls may meet the criteria as identified in DSM-5 [1].

2 Sex/gender is used interchangeably to refer to the inter-related concepts of sex (biological) and gender (sociocultural).

3 SENCo stands for Special Educational Needs Coordinator. SENCo's are school staff members with specific responsibilities in special education provision.

4 For the purpose of this paper, diagnostic criteria as outlined in DSM-5 [1] will be adopted, however, The International Classification of Diseases (ICD) criteria are comparable to those of DSM.

5 In order to obtain a diagnosis, it is necessary that differences exist in all three areas of the social communication and interaction domain and in at least two out of four criteria outlined in the RRBI domain (See Tables 1 & 2), have a negative impact of daily life and must be present in early childhood (although they may not become fully evident until the social demands exceed the individual's capacity).

6 Repetitive behaviours refer to any repetitive or unusual speech, movements or use of objects. Typical examples include motor stereotypies (rhythmic, fixed movements such as hand flapping), lining up of toys, organisation of objects in a fixed manner or according to pattern, use of repetitive phrases, echolalia, scripted language, noise making, repeated engagement in an activity, etc.

References

  • Allely, C. S. 2019. “Understanding and Recognising the Female Phenotype of Autism Spectrum Disorder and the “Camouflage” Hypothesis: A Systematic PRISMA Review.” Advances in Autism 5 (1): 14–37. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-09-2018-0036
  • American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Andersson, G. W., C. Gillberg, and C. Miniscalco. 2012. “Pre-school Children with Suspected Autism Spectrum Disorders: Do Girls and Boys Have the Same Profiles?” Research in Developmental Disabilities 34 (1): 413–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.025
  • Asperger, H. 1944. “Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter.” Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankenheiten 117 (1): 76–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01837709
  • Attwood, T. 2007. The Complete Guide to Asperger’s Syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Attwood, T., and M. Garnet 2019. “Adaptations to Autism.” In Autism and Girls. complied by T. Attwood, M. Garnett, C. Faherty, S. Wagner, L. Iland, M. Wrobel, T. Bolick, J. McIlwee Myers, R. Snyder, T. Grandin. 3-16. United States: Future Horizons Incorporated.
  • Aukett, R., J. Ritchie, and K. Mill. 1988. “Gender Differences in Friendship Patterns.” Sex Roles 19 (1-2): 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00292464
  • Baldwin, S., and D. Costley. 2016. “The Experiences and Needs of Female Adults with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder.” Autism 20 (4): 483–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315590805
  • Barbu, S., G. Cabanes, and G. Le Maner-Idrissi. 2011. “Boys and Girls on the Playground: Sex Differences in Social Development Are Not Stable Across Early Childhood.” PLoS ONE 6 (1): 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016407.
  • Bargiela, S., R. Steward, and W. Mandy. 2016. “The Experiences of Late-Diagnosed Women with Autism Spectrum Conditions: An Investigation of the Female Autism Phenotype.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46 (10): 3281–3294. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8
  • Begeer, S., D. Mandell, B. Wijnker-Holmes, S. Venderbosch, D. Rem, F. Stekelenburg, and H. M. Koot. 2012. “Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43: 1151–1156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z
  • Bond, C., and J. Hebron. 2019. “Introduction.” In Education & Girls on the Autism Spectrum, edited by J. Hebron, and C. Bond, 9–28. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers Ltd.
  • Boucher, J. 2017. Autism Spectrum Disorder: Characteristics, Causes and Practical Issues. London: SAGE Publications Ltd.
  • Buckingham, K. 2019. “The Advantages of Autism: A Personal Journey.” In Girls and Autism: Educational, Family and Personal Perspectives, edited by B. Carpenter, F. Happé, and J. Egerton, 19–25. Oxon, UK: Routledge.
  • Carpenter, B., F. Happé, and J. Egerton. 2019. “Where are all the Autistic Girls? An Introduction.” In Girls and Autism: Educational, Family and Personal Perspectives, edited by B. Carpenter, F. Happé, and J. Egerton, 3–9. London: Routledge.
  • Carrington, S., E. Templeton, and T. Papinczak. 2003. “Adolescents with Asperger Syndrome and Perceptions of Friendship.” Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 18 (4): 211–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576030180040201
  • Cheslack-Postava, K., and R. M. Jordan-Young. 2012. “Autism Spectrum Disorders: Toward a Gendered Embodiment Model.” Social Science & Medicine 74 (11): 1667–1674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.013
  • Cook O’Toole, J. 2018. Autism in Heels: An Untold Story of a Female Life on the Spectrum. New York: Skyhorse Publishing.
  • Coughlan, M., and P. Cronin. 2021. Doing a Literature Review in Nursing, Health and Social Care. 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Ltd.
  • Cridland, E. K., S. C. Jones, P. Caputi, and C. A. Magee. 2014. “Being a Girl in a Boys’ World: Investigating the Experiences of Girls with Autism Spectrum Disorders During Adolescence.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44 (6): 1261–1274. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1985-6
  • Dean, M., R. Harwood, and C. Kasari. 2017. “The Art of Camouflage: Gender Differences in the Social Behavior of Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorder.” Autism 21 (6): 678–689. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316671845
  • Dean, M., C. Kasari, W. Shih, F. Frankel, R. Whitney, R. Landa, C. Lord, F. Orlich, B. King, and R. Harwood. 2014. “The Peer Relationships of Girls with ASD at School: Comparison to Boys and Girls with and Without ASD.” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 55 (11): 1218–1225. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12242
  • Driver, B., and V. Chester. 2021. “The Presentation, Recognition and Diagnosis of Autism in Women and Girls.” Advances in Autism 7 (3): 194–207. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-12-2019-0050
  • Duvekot, J., J. van der Ende, F. C. Verhulst, G. Slappendel, E. van Daalen, A. Maras, and K. Greaves-Lord. 2017. “Factors Influencing the Probability of a Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Girls Versus Boys.” Autism 21 (6): 646–658. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316672178
  • Estrin, G. L., V. Milner, D. Spain, F. Happé, and E. Colvert. 2021. “Barriers to Autism Spectrum Disorder Diagnosis for Young Women and Girls: A Systematic Review.” Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 8 (4): 454–470. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-020-00225-8
  • Farmer, E. M. Z., B. J. Burns, S. D. Phillips, A. Angold, and E. J. Costello. 2003. “Pathways Into and Through Mental Health Services for Children and Adolescents.” Psychiatric Services 54 (1): 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.54.1.60
  • Ferrari, R. 2015. “Writing Narrative Style Literature Reviews.” Medical Writing 24 (4): 230–235. https://doi.org/10.1179/2047480615Z.000000000329
  • Fombonne, E. 1999. “The Epidemiology of Autism: A Review.” Psychological Medicine 29 (4): 769–786. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291799008508
  • Fombonne, E. 2003. “The Prevalence of Autism.” JAMA 289 (1): 87–89. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.289.1.87
  • Fombonne, E. 2005. “The Changing Epidemiology of Autism.” Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 18 (4): 281–294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-3148.2005.00266.x
  • Fombonne, E. 2009. “Epidemiology of Pervasive Developmental Disorders.” Pediatric Research 65 (6): 591–598. https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203
  • Giarelli, E., L. D. Wiggins, C. E. Rice, S. E. Levy, R. S. Kirby, J. Pinto-Martin, and D. Mandell. 2010. “Sex Differences in the Evaluation and Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorders among Children.” Disability and Health Journal 3 (2): 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001
  • Gillberg, C. 2019. “Psychiatric and Neurodevelopmental Aspects of Girls with Autism.” In Education & Girls on the Autism Spectrum: Developing an Integrated Approach, edited by J. Hebron, and C. Bond, 159–183. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Goodall, C., and A. MacKenzie. 2019. “Title: What About My Voice? Autistic Young Girls’ Experiences of Mainstream School.” European Journal of Special Needs Education 34 (4): 499–513. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2018.1553138
  • Gould, J. 2017. “Towards Understanding the Under-Recognition of Girls and Women on the Autism Spectrum.” Autism 21 (6): 703–705. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317706174
  • Gould, J., and J. Ashtown-Smith. 2011. “Missed Diagnosis or Misdiagnosis? Girls and Women on the Autism Spectrum.” Good Autism Practice (GAP 12 (1): 34–41.
  • Gray, L., E. Bownas, L. Hicks, E. Hutcheson-Galbraith, and S. Harrison. 2021. “Towards a Better Understanding of Girls on the Autism Spectrum: Educational Support and Parental Perspectives.” Educational Psychology in Practice 37 (1): 74–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/02667363.2020.1863188
  • Haney, J. L., and J. A. Cullen. 2017. “Learning About the Lived Experiences of Women with Autism from an Online Community.” Journal of Social Work in Disability & Rehabilitation 16 (1): 54–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/1536710X.2017.1260518
  • Happé, F. G., H. Mansour, P. Barrett, T. Brown, P. Abbott, and R. A. Charlton. 2016. “Demographic and Cognitive Profile of Individuals Seeking a Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Adulthood.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46 (11): 3469–3480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2886-2
  • Hiller, R. M., R. L. Young, and N. Weber. 2014. “Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder Based on DSM-5 Criteria: Evidence from Clinician and Teacher Reporting.” Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 42 (8): 1381–1393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9881-x
  • Holliday Willey, L. 1999. Pretending to be Normal: Living with Asperger Syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers Ltd.
  • Horlock, S. 2019. “Girls Group: Respecting the Female Identity of Girls with Autism in a School Setting.” In Girls and Autism: Educational, Family and Personal Perspectives, edited by B. Carpenter, F. Happé, and J. Egerton, 48–55. Oxon, UK: Routledge.
  • Hull, L., K. V. Petrides, C. Allison, P. Smith, S. Baron-Cohen, M. Lai, and W. Mandy. 2017. “"Putting on My Best Normal": Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 47 (8): 2519–2534. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5
  • Jamison, R., S. L. Bishop, M. Huerta, and A. K. Halladay. 2017. “The Clinician Perspective on Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Autism 21 (6): 772–784. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316681481
  • Jarman, B., and C. Rayner. 2015. “Asperger’s and Girls: What Teachers Need to Know.” Australasian Journal of Special Education 39 (2): 128–142. https://doi.org/10.1017/jse.2015.7
  • Kanner, L. 1943. “Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact.” Nervous Child 2: 217–250.
  • Kirkovski, M., P. G. Enticott, and P. B. Fitzgerald. 2013. “A Review of the Role of Female Gender in Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43 (11): 2584–2603. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1811-1
  • Knutsen, J., M. Crossman, J. Perrin, A. Shui, and K. Kuhlthau. 2019. “Sex Differences in Restricted Repetitive Behaviors and Interests in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Autism Treatment Network Study.” Autism 23 (4): 858–868. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318786490
  • Kopp, S., and C. Gillberg. 1992. “Girls with Social Deficits and Learning Problems: Autism, Atypical Asperger Syndrome or a Variant of These Conditions.” European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1 (2): 89–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02091791
  • Kreiser, N. L., and S. W. White. 2014. “ASD in Females: Are We Overstating the Gender Difference in Diagnosis?” Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 17 (1): 67–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0148-9
  • Lai, M., M. V. Lombardo, B. Auyeung, B. Chakrabarti, and S. Baron-Cohen. 2015. “Sex/Gender Differences and Autism: Setting the Scene for Future Research.” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 54 (1): 11–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003
  • Lai, M., M. V. Lombardo, G. Paso, A. N. V. Ruigrok, S. J. Wheelwright, S. A. Sadek, B. Chakrabarti, M. R. C. A. I. M. S. Consortium, and S. Baron-Cohen. 2011. “A Behavioral Comparison of Male and Female Adults with High Functioning Autism Spectrum Conditions.” PloS One 6 (6): 1–10.
  • Lai, M., M. V. Lombardo, A. N. Ruigrok, B. Chakrabarti, B. Auyeung, P. Szatmari, F. Happé, S. Baron-Cohen, and MRC AIMS Consortium. 2017. “Quantifying and Exploring Camouflaging in Men and Women with Autism.” Autism 21 (6): 690–702. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316671012
  • Lawson, W. 2017. “Women & Girls on the Autism Spectrum: A Profile.” Journal of Intellectual Disability - Diagnosis and Treatment 5: 90–95. https://doi.org/10.6000/2292-2598.2017.05.03.4
  • Loomes, R., L. Hull, and W. Mandy. 2017. “What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 56 (6): 466–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013
  • Mandy, W., and M. Lai. 2017. “Towards Sex- and Gender-Informed Autism Research.” Autism 21 (6): 643–645. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317706904
  • Milner, V., H. McIntosh, E. Colvert, and F. Happé. 2019. “A Qualitative Exploration of the Female Experience of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 49 (6): 2389–2402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03906-4
  • Milton, D. 2012. “On the Ontological Status of Autism: The ‘Double Empathy Problem’.” Disability and Society 27 (6): 883–887. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2012.710008
  • Milton, D., E. Gurbuz, and B. López. 2022. “The ‘Double Empathy Problem’: Ten Years on.” Autism 26 (8): 1901–1903. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613221129123
  • Moore, I., G. Morgan, A. Welham, and G. Russell. 2022. “The Intersection of Autism and Gender in the Negotiation of Identity: A Systematic Review and Metasynthesis.” Feminism & Psychology 32 (4): 421–442. https://doi.org/10.1177/09593535221074806
  • Morewood, G., C. Tomlinson, and C. Bond. 2019. “Meeting the Needs of Autistic Girls at Secondary School.” In Education & Girls on the Autism Spectrum: Developing an Integrated Approach, edited by J. Hebron, and C. Bond, 116–136. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Moyse, R., and J. Porter. 2015. “The Experiences of the Hidden Curriculum for Autistic Girls at Mainstream Primary Schools.” European Journal of Special Needs Education 30 (2): 187–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2014.986915
  • Nowell, S. W., D. R. Jones, and C. Harrop. 2019. “Circumscribed Interests in Autism: Are There Sex Differences?” Advances in Autism 5 (3): 187–198. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-09-2018-0032
  • O’Byrne, A. 2018. “Including Parents Right from the Start.” In Autism from the Inside Out: A Handbook for Parents, Early Childhood, Primary, Post-Primary and Special School Settings, edited by E. Ring, P. Daly, and E. Wall, 163–179. Oxford, UK: Peter Lang Ltd.
  • Paré, G., M. Trudel, M. Jaana, and S. Kitsiou. 2015. “Synthesizing Information Systems Knowledge: A Typology of Literature Reviews.” Information and Management 52 (2): 183–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2014.08.008
  • Pisula, E., M. Pudło, M. Słowińska, R. Kawa, M. Strząska, A. Banasiak, and T. Wolańczyk. 2017. “Behavioral and Emotional Problems in High-Functioning Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Parents’ Reports and Adolescents’ Self-Reports.” Autism 21 (6): 738–748. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316675119
  • Rivet, T. T., and J. L. Matson. 2011. “Review of Gender Differences in Core Symptomatology in Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (3): 957–976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.003
  • Russell, G., S. Stapley, T. Newlove-Delgado, A. Salmon, R. White, F. Warren, A. Pearson, and T. Ford. 2022. “Time Trends in Autism Diagnosis Over 20 Years: A UK Population-Based Cohort Study.” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 63 (6): 674–682. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13505
  • Salter, R. 2019. “Education as a Girl with Asperger’s Syndrome: A First-Hand Perspective.” In Education & Girls on the Autism Spectrum: Developing an Integrated Approach, edited by J. Hebron, and C. Bond, 31–46. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Saxe, A. 2017. “The Theory of Intersectionality: A New Lens for Understanding the Barriers Faced by Autistic Women.” Canadian Journal of Disability Studies 6 (4): 153–178. https://doi.org/10.15353/cjds.v6i4.386
  • Sedgewick, F., L. Crane, V. Hill, and E. Pellicano. 2019. “Friends and Lovers: The Relationships of Autistic and Neurotypical Women.” Autism in Adulthood 1 (2): 112–123. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2018.0028
  • Sedgewick, F., V. Hill, R. Yates, L. Pickering, and E. Pellicano. 2016. “Gender Differences in the Social Motivation and Friendship Experiences of Autistic and Non-Autistic Adolescents.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46 (4): 1297–1306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2669-1
  • Sipes, M., J. L. Matson, J. A. Worley, and A. M. Kozlowski. 2011. “Gender Differences in Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Toddlers.” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5 (4): 1465–1470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.02.007
  • Solomon, M., M. Miller, S. L. Taylor, S. P. Hinshaw, and C. S. Carter. 2012. “Autism Symptoms and Internalising Psychopathy in Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42 (1): 48–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1215-z
  • Sproston, K., F. Sedgewick, and L. Crane. 2017. “Autistic Girls and School Exclusion: Perspectives of Students and Their Parents.” Autism and Developmental Language Impairments 2: 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941517706172
  • Taylor, J. L., and L. S. DaWalt. 2020. “Working Toward a Better Understanding of the Life Experiences of Women on the Autism Spectrum.” Autism 24 (5): 1027–1030. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320913754
  • Tierney, S., J. Burns, and E. Kilbey. 2016. “Looking Behind the Mask: Social Coping Strategies of Girls on the Autistic Spectrum.” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 23: 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.11.013
  • Tomlinson, C., C. Bond, and J. Hebron. 2020. “The School Experiences of Autistic Girls and Adolescents: A Systematic Review.” European Journal of Special Needs Education 35 (2): 203–219. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2019.1643154
  • Tomlinson, C., C. Bond, and J. Hebron. 2021. “The Mainstream School Experiences of Adolescent Autistic Girls.” European Journal of Special Needs Education 37 (2): 323–339. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2021.1878657
  • Van Wijngaarden-Cremers, P. J. M., E. van Eeten, W. B. Groen, P. A. Van Deurzen, I. J. Oosterling, and R. J. Van der Gaag. 2014. “Gender and Age Differences in the Core Triad of Impairments in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44 (3): 627–635. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1913-9
  • Ward, T. B., C. Curtis, and S. Seehagen. 2022. “Investigating the Effects of Perceived Student Gender on Primary School Teachers’ Recognition of Autism.” Psychology in the Schools 59 (7): 1363–1376. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22667
  • Wassell, C., and E. Burke. 2022. “Autism, Girls & Keeping It All Inside: Autistic Girls Network.” Autistic Girls Network, https://autisticgirlsnetwork.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/03/Keeping-it-all-inside.pdf.
  • West, R. E., and F. Martin. 2023. “What Type of Paper are you Writing? A Taxonomy of Review and Theory Scholarship Distinguished by Their Summary and Advocacy Arguments.” Educational Technology Research and Development 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-023-10233-0
  • Whitlock, A., K. Fulton, M. Lai, E. Pellicano, and W. Mandy. 2020. “Recognition of Girls on the Autism Spectrum by Primary School Educators: An Experimental Study.” Autism Research 13 (8): 1358–1372. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2316
  • Wilkinson, L. A. 2008. “The Gender Gap in Asperger Syndrome: Where are the Girls?” TEACHING Exceptional Children Plus 4 (4): 2–9.
  • Wing, L. 1981. “Asperger’s Syndrome: A Clinical Account.” Psychological Medicine 11 (1): 115–129. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700053332
  • Winter-Messiers, M. 2007. “From Tarantulas to Toilet Brushes: Understanding the Special Interest Areas of Children and Youth with Asperger Syndrome.” Remedial and Special Education 28 (3): 140–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/07419325070280030301
  • Wohlin, C. 2014. “Guidelines for Snowballing in Systematic Literature Studies and a Replication in Software Engineering.” In Proceedings 18th International Conference on Evaluation and Assessment in Software Engineering, New York, USA. Article 38. 321–330.