973
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Perspective

Alfred Wilhelm Volkmann on stereoscopic vision

ABSTRACT

Wheatstone’s demonstration of binocular single vision and depth with stimulation of non-corresponding points reverberated throughout mid-nineteenth century German visual science. It challenged the received view that single vision was a consequence of retinal correspondence otherwise objects were seen double. Wheatstone also argued that stimulation of corresponding points could yield double vision. He interpreted his experimental observations in psychological rather than physiological terms, as did Helmholtz later. Volkmann addressed both of these challenges in a long article on stereoscopic vision published in 1859. While he accepted the first of the questions Wheatstone posed Volkmann was more cautious with regard to the second. Volkmann was an experimentalist who applied psychophysical methods to determine thresholds for stereoscopic depth perception. In line with many of his colleagues in Germany, he took issue implicitly with Wheatstone’s approach: how can the detailed quantitative experiments supporting Vieth and Müller’s interpretation of binocular single vision be derailed by simple observations with a stereoscope? Unlike most of his colleagues, Volkmann was swayed in Wheatstone’s favor through his own experiments: both physiological and psychological processes are involved in stereoscopic depth perception.

Alfred Wilhelm VolkmannCitation1 (1801–1877) presented his theoretical and experimental response to Wheatstone’sCitation2 (1838) analysis of binocular vision in 1859. The impact of Wheatstone’s stereoscopic investigations on German visual science was seismic. Müller’sCitation3 elegant analysis of binocular single vision, linking the Vieth-Müller circle with identical (or corresponding) retinal points, was challenged by the perception of singleness and depth from stimulating non-corresponding retinal points and many of Müller’s countrymen sprang to its defense.Citation4 Five articles in recent issues of Strabismus present translations into English by H. J. Simonsz from Volkmann’sCitation1 examination of stereoscopic vision and his attempts to reconcile the disparate views. Volkmann is shown in together with the title page of his article.

Figure 1. Detail of a portrait of Alfred Volkmann and the title page of his article on stereoscopic vision.

Figure 1. Detail of a portrait of Alfred Volkmann and the title page of his article on stereoscopic vision.

In Part 1Citation5 of the translation (pp. 1–32 of Volkmann’sCitation1 article) Volkmann assesses the approaches of BrückeCitation6 and PanumCitation7 to accommodate stereoscopic phenomena without abandoning the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 2Citation8 (pp. 32–59) presents the results of Volkmann’s experiments determining fusional thresholds in the three dimensions of space – horizontal, vertical and torsional. Part 3Citation9 (pp. 59–72) presents Volkmann’s attempt to reconcile the seemingly incompatible approaches of German physiologists and the empiricist interpretations of Wheatstone. Volkmann’s considerations and experiments on Wheatstone’s observation that stimulation of corresponding points can lead to double vision are presented in Part 4Citation10 (pp. 72–86). The final translation (Part 5,Citation11 pp. 86–100) addresses some paradoxical aspects of stereoscopic vision.

Two years after Volkmann’s article, NagelCitation12–15 published a book on vision with two eyes parts of which have also been translated in Strabismus. The starting points for both Volkmann and Nagel were the implications of Wheatstone’s stereoscopic phenomena for Müller’s theory: “If Wheatstone were right on this point, the doctrine of vision was threatened to be overturned completely”Citation1(p2). What did Wheatstone demonstrate that stirred visual scientists, particularly those in Germany, so dramatically? First, he showed that points that are not on the Vieth-Müller circle can be seen as single and in depth; secondly, he presented a figure that stimulated identical retinal points and was seen as double. Volkmann addressed both aspects of vision with two eyes.

A portrait of Wheatstone is shown in together with the first page of his article introducing the stereoscope and stereoscopic depth perceptionCitation2 (p.371).

Figure 2. A portrait of Charles Wheatstone and the title page of his first article on stereoscopic vision.

Figure 2. A portrait of Charles Wheatstone and the title page of his first article on stereoscopic vision.

Wheatstone was a pioneer of research on stereoscopic vision. Indeed, stereoscopic vision was not appreciated until 1838, when he demonstrated the synthesis of depth from the binocular combination of two plane drawings mounted in his stereoscope. Wheatstone not only invented the stereoscope, but he also carried out many ingenious manipulations with stereograms. He also adopted philosophical empiricism to interpret how these various factors combine. That is, he conceived of perception as a mental process in which previous exposures to patterns of stimulation were related to one another. Wheatstone demonstrated that dissimilar pictures on the two retinae could be seen singly and in depth. For example, two vertical lines at different distances could be projected onto a flat surface between the eyes and the object: when each eye was presented with a single projection the impression of depth was reproduced. He also showed that paired three-dimensional outline cubes, when viewed in his stereoscope, induced the same impressions of depth as those produced by flat projections of them. Moreover, when the cubes were placed so as to project identical images to the eyes then depth was no longer seen. Similar changes in the perception of depth could be produced with paired skeleton figures as with outline drawings. Thus, representing the appropriate perspective projections of an object to each eye yielded the perception of solidity. In the set of stereograms that Wheatstone published, outlines were used for the specific purpose of reducing any reliance on monocular cues to depth. When he transposed the stereograms the direction of depth reversed. However, when the projections were of a symmetrical solid, like a cube, there were changes in apparent size attendant upon the reversal of depth. This relationship between perceived size and distance was one to which Wheatstone was to return in his second paper on binocular vision.Citation16 He adopted the empirical methods of the physicist to unravel the independent contributions of factors like accommodation, convergence, retinal size and disparity in determining perceived size.

A summary of Wheatstone’s articleCitation17 was soon translated into German and a full translation was provided laterCitation18; Volkmann cited the English version rather than the translations into German. William James shrewdly observed that Wheatstone’s paper “contains the germ of almost all the methods applied since to the study of optical perception. It seems a pity that England, leading off so brilliantly the modern epoch of this study, should so quickly have dropped out of the field. Almost all subsequent progress has been made in Germany, Holland, and, longo intervallo, America”Citation19 (pp. 226−227).

The first of the German physiologists to bridle at the attack by Wheatstone on Müller’s doctrine of identical retinal points was BrückeCitation6 (); his article was published in the Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie edited by Müller. Brücke argued that detailed vision is restricted to a small foveal region so that any object will be scanned rapidly. In the case of binocular vision the eye movements involve variations in vergence as well as version, with the ensuing perception based on a synthesis of these different retinal impressions. According to this hypothesis, stereoscopic depth could not be perceived if the eyes did not move.

Figure 3. A portrait of Ernst Brücke and the title page of his article on stereoscopic vision.

Figure 3. A portrait of Ernst Brücke and the title page of his article on stereoscopic vision.

WheatstoneCitation2 (p. 392) had already considered and rejected this hypothesis: “Another and a beautiful proof that the appearance of relief in binocular vision is an effect independent of the motion of the eyes, may be obtained by impressing on the retinæ ocular spectra [afterimages] of the component figures.” Volkmann reached the same conclusion using a novel method. In the same year as his article on stereoscopic vision, he invented the tachistoscopeCitation20 to examine precisely this question; the instrument enabled brief presentation of paired stimuli, one to each eye, to determine whether depth was still seen – it was. DoveCitation21 had reached the same conclusion using paired afterimages produced by illumination with an electrical spark. Thus, the evidence against Brücke’s eye movement hypothesis was strong.

Volkmann noted that PanumCitation7 () also rejected Brücke’s analysis of stereoscopic depth perception and adopted a position supporting both psychological and physiological factors involved in it. Panum made a more nuanced attempt to salvage Müller’s theory from the attack on it by Wheatstone: he replaced points of retinal correspondence by circle of correspondence in what came to be referred to as ‘Panum’s fusional areas’. However, the logical consistency of this hypothesis was questioned by Volkmann who reported a number of related experiments. Volkmann was principally an experimentalist. Interest in theory needed to be empirically supported and 39 experiments are described in the whole article.

Figure 4. A portrait of Peter Ludvig Panum and the title page of his book on seeing with two eyes.

Figure 4. A portrait of Peter Ludvig Panum and the title page of his book on seeing with two eyes.

Panum devoted much of his book to examining another aspect of binocular vision highlighted by Wheatstone – binocular rivalry.Citation22–24 When radically different stimuli are presented to each eye in a stereoscope, they are not combined but compete, resulting in dynamically changing patterns of visibility. WheatstoneCitation2 examined rivalry between the letters A and S each surrounded by similar circles. Letters were recognized as complex patterns and simpler stimuli were soon enlisted. One of the first systematic studies of rivalry was published by Panum.Citation7 The orthogonal grating stimuli he introduced have dominated the study of rivalry ever since. Panum drew attention to the dynamic variations and to the mixtures or composites that are seen and he sought to interpret the phenomenon in physiological terms rather than the psychological factors proposed by WheatstoneCitation2 and later by Helmholtz.Citation25

Volkmann’s mastery of experimental vision was displayed in Part 2 of the translation. He had been a colleague of Fechner in Leipzig and he determined the thresholds for the detection of stereoscopic depth using line stimuli in a variety of orientations.

It is in Part 3 of the translation that Volkmann cites Wheatstone’sCitation16 second, and much neglected, memoir in binocular vision. In it Wheatstone described and illustrated an adjustable mirror stereoscope, a prism stereoscope, and a pseudoscope for reversing disparities. The main purpose of these was to extend the range of conditions under which the two eyes could be stimulated. Wheatstone used the stereoscope with adjustable arms to vary retinal size, convergence, accommodation, and disparity. He found that “The perceived magnitude of an object, therefore, diminishes as the inclination of the axes becomes greater, while the distance remains the same; and it increases, when the inclination of the axes remains the same, while the distance diminishes. When both of these conditions vary inversely, as they do in ordinary vision when the distance of an object changes, the perceived magnitude remains the same”Citation16(p3). On the basis of these experiments, as well as his own Volkmann appreciated that stereoscopic depth involves psychological as well as physiological processes: “The psychological influences are very important in the theoretical consideration because they make any explanation of the discussed fusion process through innate relations impossible”Citation9(p189).

Wheatstone’s second line of attack was that stimulation of corresponding retinal points can result in double vision. This was examined experimentally by Volkmann in Part 4 of the translation.Citation10 The stimulus Wheatstone used was a thick tilted line and a thin vertical in one eye and a single thick vertical in the other. The reproduction of this figure by VolkmannCitation10 (Fig. 15) does not show the differences in thicknesses of Wheatstone’s stimulusCitation2 (Fig. 13), although Volkmann does describe them as thick and thin lines. When VolkmannCitation26 (Fig. 34) returned to this issue the illustration distinguished more clearly between the thick and thin lines. He was not able to replicate Wheatstone’s observations, nor were others before and after him.Citation6,Citation12,Citation25,Citation27 However, the stimulus combinations used were rarely like those employed by Wheatstone. For example, the squares surrounding the lines to aid binocular alignment were often not included, and there was little consensus in the alternative observations described. For these reasons, Ono and WadeCitation28 tried to stay as close as possible to Wheatstone’s arrangement: a replica of Wheatstone’s mirror stereoscope was used with photographic copies of his stimuli at the size and viewing distance he described. The results of three experiments were not conclusive as there were large individual differences. The Wheatstone stimulus is itself complex as different predictions would be made for the upper and lower halves of the display and Volkmann’s results could be taken as reflecting this.

VolkmannCitation1,Citation26,Citation29,Citation30 wrote about binocular vision both before and after Wheatstone announced the stereoscope to the public in 1838 and it is instructive to compare his final analysis with the earlier accounts. A brief chapter on single vision with two eyes is included in his first book.Citation29 His chapter on Seeing in Wagner’s handbookCitation30 contains a section on single and double vision illustrating the Vieth-Müller circle. Wheatstone’s stereoscopic experiments are described but Volkmann’s sympathies remained with the theory of identical retinal points. It would seem that this initial engagement with Wheatstone’s ideas was the goad to his experiments reported in 1859.Citation1 As would be expected, there is a lengthy appraisal of vision with two eyes in his second book on optics.Citation26 It is based largely on the long article that has been translated in the recent issues of Strabismus. This chapter has been overlooked largely because (as pointed out by Strasburger, Huber and RoseCitation31) it was omitted from the table of contents!

Volkmann was an experimentalist who applied the psychophysical methods of Fechner to determine thresholds for stereoscopic depth perception. In line with many of his colleagues in Germany, he took issue implicitly with Wheatstone’s approach: how can the detailed quantitative experiments supporting Vieth and Müller’s interpretation of binocular single vision be derailed by simple observations with a stereoscope? Unlike most of his colleagues, Volkmann was swayed in Wheatstone’s favor through his own experiments. While he accepted the first of the questions Wheatstone posed (that stimulation of non-corresponding points can yield singleness and depth) he was more cautious with regard to the second (that stimulation of corresponding points can lead to double vision).

An index of the significance of Volkmann’s 1859 article is that in the section on stereoscopic vision, HelmholtzCitation25 (1867) provides relatively little empirical data of his own but uses that from Volkmann.Citation1 Indeed, Volkmann is accorded more citations than Wheatstone.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Correction Statement

This article has been republished with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.

Additional information

Funding

The author(s) received funding from the University of Dundee for publishing this article.

References

  • Volkmann AW. Die stereoskopischen Erscheinungen in ihrer Beziehung zu der Lehre von den identischen Netzhautpunkten. Archiv für Ophthalmologie. 1859;5:1–100.
  • Wheatstone C. Contributions to the physiology of vision.—part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philos Trans R Soc London. 1838;128:371–394.
  • Müller J. Handbuch der Physiologie des Menschen. Vol. 2. Coblenz, Germany: Hölscher; 1838.
  • Wade NJ. Albrecht Nagel on vision with two eyes. Strabismus. 2020;28(3):115–118. doi:10.1080/09273972.2020.1802181.
  • Volkmann AW. The stereoscopic phenomena in relation to the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 1: the views by Wheatstone, Brücke and Panum. Strabismus. 2020;28(4):223–235. doi:10.1080/09273972.2020.1854513.
  • Brücke EW. Ueber die stereoskopischen Erscheinungen und Wheatstone’s Angriff auf die Lehre von den identischen Stellen der Netzhäute. Archiv für Anatomie,Physiologie und Wissenschaftliche Medicin. 1841;8:459–476.
  • Panum PL. Physiologische Untersuchungen über das Sehen mit zwei Augen. Kiel, Germany: Schwerssche Buhhandlung; 1858.
  • Volkmann AW. The stereoscopic phenomena in relation to the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 2: horizontal, vertical and torsional amplitudes of fusion. Strabismus. 2021;29(1):61–71. doi:10.1080/09273972.2021.1872916.
  • Volkmann AW. The stereoscopic phenomena in relation to the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 3: observable disparities differ in parts of the visual field. Strabismus. 2021;29(3):189–194. doi:10.1080/09273972.2021.1959718.
  • Volkmann AW. The stereoscopic phenomena in relation to the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 4: wheatstone’s assertion that identical images which fall on corresponding retinal points may appear double and in different positions. Strabismus. 2021;29. doi:10.1080/09273972.2021.200013.
  • Volkmann AW. The stereoscopic phenomena in relation to the doctrine of identical retinal points. Part 5. Strabismus. 2022;30: 86–100.
  • Nagel A. Das Sehen mit zwei Augen, die Lehre von den identischen Netzhautstellen. Leipzig: Winter; 1861.
  • Nagel A. Vision with two eyes, the doctrine of the identical retina points. Part 1: the stereoscopic phenomena. Strabismus. 2020;28(1):55–59. doi:10.1080/09273972.2020.1725298.
  • Nagel A. Vision with two eyes, the doctrine of the identical retina points. Part 2: fusion of disparate images. Strabismus. 2020;28(1):55–59. doi:10.1080/09273972.2020.1725298.
  • Nagel A. Vision with two eyes, the doctrine of the identical retina points. Part 3: stereoscopic experiment: fusion of disparate images including cyclofusion. Strabismus. 2020;28(1):55–59. doi:10.1080/09273972.2020.1725298.
  • Wheatstone C. Contributions to the physiology of vision.—part the second. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philos Trans R Soc London. 1852;142:1–17.
  • Wheatstone C. Ueber das Sehen mit zwei Augen und das Stereoskop. Annalen der Physik und Chemie. 1839;47:625–627. doi:10.1002/andp.18391230813.
  • Wheatstone C. Beiträge zur Physiologie des Gesichtssinnes. Erster Theil. Ueber eine merkwürdige und bis jetzt unbeachtete Erscheinung beim Sehen mit beiden Augen. Annalen der Physik und Chemie. 1842;1:1–48. Supplementary Volume. doi:10.1002/andp.18421310102.
  • James W. Principles of Psychology. Vol. 2. Macmillan: London; 1890.
  • Volkmann AW. Das Tachistoskop, ein Instrument, welches bei Untersuchung des momentanen Sehens den Gebrauch des elektrischen Funkens ersetzt. Berichte über die Verhandlungen der Leipziger Gesellschaft der Wissenschaften. 1859;11:90–98.
  • Dove HW. Die Combination die Eindrücke beider Ohren und beider Augen zu einem Eindruck. Monatsberichte der Berliner preussische Akademie der Wissenschaften. 1842;41:251.
  • Wade NJ. Ocular equivocation: the rivalry between Wheatstone and Brewster. Vision. 2019;3(2):1–26. doi:10.3390/vision3020026.
  • Wade NJ. On the origins of terms in binocular vision. i-Perception. 2021;12(1):1–19. doi:10.1177/2041669521992381.
  • Wade NJ. On the art of binocular rivalry. i-Perception. 2021;12(9):1–23. doi:10.1177/20416695211053877.
  • Helmholtz H. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. Voss: Leipzig; 1867.
  • Volkmann AW. Physiologische Untersuchungen im Gebiete der Optik, I. Heft. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel; 1863.
  • Hering E. Beiträge zur Physiologie. Vol. 2. Leipzig: Engelmann; 1862.
  • Ono H, Wade NJ. Resolving discrepant results of the Wheatstone experiment. Psychol Res. 1985;47(3):135–142. doi:10.1007/BF00309264.
  • Volkmann AW. Neue Beiträge zur Physiologie des Gesichtssinnes. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel; 1836.
  • Volkmann AW. Sehen. In: Wagner R, ed. Handwörterbuch der Physiologie, mit Rücksicht auf physiologische Pathologie, Dritter Band, Erste Abtheilung. Braunschweig: Vieweg; 1846:265–351.
  • Strasburger H, Huber J, Rose D. Ewald Hering’s (1899) on the limits of visual acuity: a translation and commentary – with a supplement on Alfred Volkmann’s (1863) physiological investigations in the field of optics. i-Perception. 2018;9(3):1–14. doi:10.1177/2041669518763675.