2,471
Views
31
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLES

Does Peer Use Influence Adoption of Efficient Cookstoves? Evidence From a Randomized Controlled Trial in Uganda

, , &

Abstract

The authors examined the effect of peer usage on consumer demand for efficient cookstoves with a randomized controlled trial in rural Uganda. The authors tested whether the neighbors of buyers who ordered and received a stove are more likely to purchase an efficient cookstove than the neighbors of buyers who ordered but have not yet received a stove. The authors found that neighbors of buyers who have experience with the stove are not detectably more likely to purchase a stove than neighbors of buyers who have not yet received their stove. The authors found evidence of peer effects in opinions about efficient cookstoves. Knowing that a prominent member of the community has the efficient stove predicts 17–22 percentage points higher odds of strongly favoring the stove. However, this more favorable opinion seemingly has no effect on purchase decisions.

Half the world cooks with inefficient stoves that burn solid fuels such as wood and charcoal. Smoke from these stoves kills 4 million people a year (Lim et al., Citation2012), and their inefficiency contributes to deforestation and global climate change. Increasing the adoption of more efficient, cleaner cookstoves is a public health priority and policymakers must choose one or more strategies to achieve this goal. Potential strategies include subsidized prices, financing, marketing campaigns, and peer influence. This article examined the latter strategy by observing how one person's purchase and use of a stove affects peer attitudes and purchase behaviors. The former strategies are the subject of other articles stemming from the same research program (Levine, Beltramo, Blalock, & Cotterman, Citation2013).

The findings of this study are potentially generalizable to products other than cookstoves. Millions of lives are lost each year because households do not purchase products that protect from mosquitoes, treat drinking water, or reduce household air pollution. Understanding how peers influence an individual's adoption decision could provide insights for increasing the purchase of products that improve health and well-being. For products such as safer cookstoves or water filters, such knowledge has the potential to save many lives.

Learning from others is important because it can generate a social multiplier that speeds product adoption (Glaeser, Sacerdote, & Scheinkman, Citation2003). Estimating the role of peers or social interactions in driving adoption is made difficult by the problem of correlated unobservables between peers and, especially, between friends (Manski, Citation1993). We used randomization to eliminate the problem.

Our randomized trial delayed delivery of a new stove, the Envirofit G-3300, for some households but not others. We then compared purchasing behaviors of the neighbors of those two groups. Both groups are neighbors of someone who agreed to buy the stove, but one group witnessed delivery of the stove before the other group. We examined the strength of a demonstration effect (stove is physically present for neighbors to see, feel, observe) versus an ordering effect (only that the household has agreed to purchase a stove). We also tested the causal pathways that underlie the theory of peer effects, such as familiarity with the new stove, having seen the new stove cook a meal, and beliefs related to the new stove is effective in saving fuel, easy to use, improves health. Because we randomized the timing of when stoves are delivered to each group, we were able to distinguish the causal effects of an additional peer physically having a stove (demonstration effect) as opposed to the effect of a peer endorsing the purchase of a stove (ordering effect).

Literature Review

Individuals often learn from and imitate he behavior of others within their social network. For example, numerous studies suggest that social groups influence individual's behaviors in ways spanning from the trivial, like what movie to see, to more serious issues such as drug use, financial management, school attendance, and even criminal behavior (Bayer, Pintoff, & Pozen Citation2004; Bursztyn, Ederer, Ferman, & Yuchtman, Citation2012; Duncan, Boisjoly, Kremer, Levy, & Eccles, 2005; Mbondo, Citation2013; Moretti, Citation2011; Sacerdote, Citation2011). Evidence of learning from others is ample in rural agrarian settings (Bandiera & Rasul, Citation2006; Conley & Udry, Citation2010; Foster & Rosenzweig, Citation1995), but is not always present. Some recent studies have failed to find peer spillovers (Beltramo, Citation2010; Guryan, Kroft, & Notowidigdo, Citation2009; Luoto et al., Citation2012).

Neighbors often behave similarly because of a common environment (e.g., prices, connectedness to cities, infrastructure) and because neighbors have self-selected into a community. Even if many neighbors are born in a community, there is self-selection of who remains in a community. This makes social interaction effects difficult to distinguish from unobserved factors that are correlated across neighbors (Manski, Citation1993; Moffitt, Citation2001).

Some studies have randomly allocated people into peer groups to overcome potential endogeneity issues, for example randomized allocation of roommates (Duncan et al., Citation2005) and randomized allocation of housing vouchers (Kling, Liebman, & Katz, Citation2007). Other studies have looked at randomized allocations of interventions across naturally occurring groups (Miguel & Kremer, Citation2004; Oster & Thornton, Citation2009). We used the second strategy and randomize the timing of the delivery of cookstoves.

Theory of Peer Effects

We tested the incremental effect of having one close neighbor in a person's social network who ordered the stove but had not received it—the ordering effect—versus the incremental effect of having one close neighbor who both ordered and received the stove—the demonstration effect. The objective was to measure the incremental difference of having a neighbor who physically owns and perhaps demonstrates the new stove to his or her neighbors, above any potential endorsement that comes from just ordering the new stove. We compared neighbors of early buyers, those who ordered the stove and had taken delivery, to the neighbors of late buyers, those who had ordered the stove but not yet taken delivery. We followed Rogers’ Diffusion of Innovation theory and hypothesized that close neighbors—geographically and socially—share ideas and as a result influence the purchase of the new stove (Rogers, Citation2003).

Geographic and Social Closeness

The theory of peer effects among neighbors assumes high communication and trust, which in our setting in rural Mbarara, Uganda, implied the following:

Assumption 1: Neighbors are geographically close.

Assumption 2: Neighbors communicate frequently.

Assumption 3: Neighbors consult each other if they have problems.

Living Near an Early Buyer Increases Familiarity With the Envirofit

Because neighbors share information, we assumed that when someone gets a new product, neighbors typically know about it. Early buyers’ neighbors have lived for a month near the early buyers’ new stove. We thus hypothesize:

Hypothesis 1: Neighbors of early buyers are more likely to have heard about the Envirofit.

Hypothesis 2: Neighbors of early buyers have seen more people cook with the Envirofit.

Peer effects are likely to increase uptake only if the product is popular. Thus, the theory of peer effects requires the following:

Assumption 4: The stove is popular among those who use it.

Given high communication and favorable opinions, the theory of peer effects implies if early buyers hear more about the stove and most of what they hear is good.

Hypothesis 3: Living near an early buyer improves opinions of the Envirofit.

Living Near an Early Buyer Increases Purchase of the Envirofit

Evidence from the marketing literature (although stated and actual preferences may vary) posits that favorable opinions predict purchase (Arndt, Citation1967; Arts, Frambach, & Bijmolt Citation2011; Morrison, Citation1979).

Hypothesis 4: Living near an early buyer should increase the likelihood of purchase.

Peer Effects Are Stronger for the Geographically and Socially Close

Any theory of peer effects among neighbors suggests that the effects will be strongest among those who are geographically and socially close. We had three measures of closeness. First, we looked at geographic distance. Second, we created an Index of Social Closeness on the basis of a combination of frequency of communication, common activities, and evidence of trust measured by whether the neighbor would solicit advice from the early/late buyer neighbor. Third, we analyzed reciprocated friendships. Card and Giuliano (Citation2012) found stronger peer effects in reciprocated friendships—that is, both members of a pair name the other as a close friend.

Bandiera and Rasul (2006), in their experiment testing farmers’ decision to adopt a new crop, found that adoption decisions are uncorrelated among individuals belonging to different religion networks. Conley and Urdy (Citation2010) used data on farmers’ communication patterns to define each individual's information neighborhood. They found that farmers do adopt practices of successful neighbor farmers but conditional on a set of common agricultural and sociocultural conditions including growing conditions, clan membership, and religion. Following this finding, we also tested separately whether neighbors who attend the same church affects the decision to purchase the nontraditional cookstove.

Experimental Design

The study spans 14 parishes where the randomization is within parish.Footnote1 The Centre for Integrated Research and Community Development, a nongovernmental organization based in Kampala that specializes in market research related to household energy, acted as the in-country data collection and sales team partner.

This study builds on two previous experiments that distributed the same efficient cookstove, the Envirofit G-3300, in these parishes. The first, a study from March to June, 2012, sold stoves in 24 parishes (Levine et al., Citation2013). We recruited a focal point person in each parish who we paid a small fee to spread the word about the upcoming sales meeting and to gather roughly 60 people to each meeting. This study takes place in 14 of the same parishes and a total of 720 participants attended sales meetings in the sales study. Of those who attended in the previous study, 57% (n = 410) purchased a stove (Levine et al., Citation2013) when a free trial and time payments were offered.

A second study took place in the same 14 parishes used for this study. Among the 410 buyers from the first study, an impact evaluation occurred in the second and third quarters of 2012 to measure the effect of an efficient cookstove on health, fuel use, and stove adoption (Beltramo, Blalock, Levine, & Simons, Citation2014). Households were eligible to participate in the impact evaluation if they mainly used wood as a fuel source, regularly cooked for eight or fewer persons (the Envirofit is able to cook portions for at most eight people), were generally home every day, and cooked in an enclosed kitchen (Harrell et al., Citation2013). The sample was randomized across eligible buyers within each parish. Half the buyers were randomly selected to receive their stove early, while half the group received their stoves late (Harrell et al., Citation2013). Of the eligible 410 buyers, we randomly chose 12 households (6 early buyers and 6 late buyers) per parish to participate in the impact evaluation study, resulting in a total of 168 randomized participants across 14 parishes. We used an intention-to-treat framework to analyze these households based on their assignment.

This experiment used the same 168 early and late buyers of the impact evaluation and interviews 763 of their neighbors. To identify neighbors, we visited the 168 early and late buyers and asked them to identify up to five neighbors who lived within a 5-minute walk with whom they talk frequently. We then measured whether these neighbors’ decisions to purchase an efficient cookstove depend on whether they live next to early buyers (who had ordered and received a new stove) or late buyers (who had ordered the new stove but not yet received it). We measured the extra effect of a neighbor owning and perhaps demonstrating the use of the new stove, above any effect that comes from a neighbor endorsing the stove by ordering it.

Sample Selection and Measurement Definition

To identify neighbors who were socially close, we first visited our early or late buyers and asked them “Who among your neighbors within a five minute walk do you speak to frequently?” We recorded up to five neighbors of each early or late buyer. The data collection team then made several attempts on multiple days to visit and interview the neighbors listed by the early and late buyers. When visiting these neighbors, the enumerators asked (without prompt about the early or late buyer), “Who among your neighbors within a five minute walk do you speak to frequently?” If the neighbor listed the neighboring early/late buyer household, the enumerator then silently recorded the early/late buyer. This identification strategy was deliberate so as to ensure unbiased measures of “reciprocated neighbors.” When they had time, enumerators also visited households near the early or late buyer, but not listed by the early or late buyer as ones they “speak to frequently.”

Favoring a Stove Measurement

We asked respondents to rank the main attributes of the Envirofit including improves health, reduces fuel use, and ease of use compared with the traditional three stone fire. For each dimension, neighbors were shown a visual 10-point scale and asked to place a coin on the scale, where a 1 (on the left) indicated the Envirofit is better and a 10 indicated the three-stone fire is better. As an example to solicit opinions about the Envirofit improves health, neighbors were asked to mark to the left on the 10-point scale if they believe the Envirofit is better for your health or to the right if the three-stone fire is better for your health. To ensure accurate responses, respondents first played a trial game ranking preferences between two common local meals. Enumerators through a series of survey questions then made sure that each participant understood the game before ranking preferences between the Envirofit and the three-stone fire. We classified either a 1 or 2 in favor of the Envirofit as selecting the Envirofit over the three-stone fire (Table ).

Geographic and Social Closeness Measurement

To measure geographic closeness using GPS readings we measured the kilometers apart between early and late buyers and the individual neighbor. To measure social closeness, we created an index of self-reported frequency and timeliness of communication between the respondent and the experimental neighbor, number of shared activities with the experimental neighbor, and whether the respondent would solicit advice from the experimental neighbor (Table ). For each of these four items, we created a standardized score:

  • “Frequency of contact between neighbors” each month (daily = 6, three times a week = 5, once a week = 4, twice a month = 3, monthly = 2, and less than a month = 1).

  • “Last reported contact between neighbors” (today = 5, two to six days ago = 4, a week ago = 3, two weeks ago = 2, a month or more prior = 1).

  • The count of “type of activities reported by neighbors” the neighbor reports sharing with the experimental neighbor (1–4).

  • Could you go to this neighbor if you had a problem and needed advice? (yes = 1; no = 0).

The four measures of social closeness summed ranges from 0 to 14, with 0 being the least close. To ensure cross-comparability of the four reported measures of social closeness, we created an Index of Social Closeness using a standardized score. Each of the four variables were standardized (known as z scores) such that the mean of each variable is zero and the standard deviation is 1 (Table ). The standardized score is appropriate for this index because it normalizes the four inputs to one normal distribution.

Asset Index Measurement

On the basis of field testing in two test villages during the feasibility stage and analysis of the Demographic and Health Survey (Uganda Bureau of Statistics and ICF International Inc., Citation2012), we selected a household's ownership of a television, radio, cell phone, and cows to proxy for wealth. We generated an asset index equal to the number of these four assets they owned.

Estimation Specification

Our general specification for outcome y at household i who is neighbor of an early or late buyer j in parish p is as follows:

(1)
where Xijpk is a vector of control variables, FEp is a fixed effect for each parish, ujp is a random effect for each early or late buyer, and Earlyjp is an indicator variable equal to one if the neighbor is an early buyer and zero for a late buyer. β is the coefficient of interest. We ran a series of regressions changing the outcome variable Yijp each time. The outcome variable Yijp alternates between purchase decision, number of people the neighbor knows who own an Envirofit, number of people the neighbor has seen cook with the Envirofit, and the ranking of the three beliefs between the Envirofit with the three-stone fire including the following: the Envirofit is better for your health, is easier to use, and the Envirofit uses less fuel. We clustered standard errors at the level of the early or late buyer and used the Huber-White heteroscedasticity correction. For the interaction of geographic and social closeness, we ran the following:

Here, the estimate of ϕ captures the interaction of geographic or of social closeness with an early buyer.

Because of the experimental design, no control variables are needed with a sufficiently large sample size to randomize household variation. Because sample size is limited, we included a few control variables in our main specification including asset count and household never cooks.Footnote2

Other information on the stoves, besides the 168 early and late buyers, is in the community. In the previous sales experiment, 242 other households purchased an Envirofit. Among the 763 neighbors sampled, 95 of the neighbors had purchased a stove in the previous study (Table ). As a result, the 95 households were dropped from analysis. It is possible that the 95 neighbors who previously purchased the stove also exert social or peer influence over the neighbors in addition to the early or late buyer. Thus, we included the count of other neighbors who previously purchased the stove as an explanatory variable.

In addition, the focal point person in each of the 14 parishes owns an Envirofit. Because the focal point person was actively involved in organizing the meetings and raising awareness, we controlled for whether the respondent knows the focal point person.

Sample Descriptive Statistics

The average age of neighbors (excluding those who already purchased a stove) is 25 for both early and late buyer neighbors and the average number of people who ate lunch yesterday is constant across both groups of neighbors—13 (Table ).

A higher percentage of neighbors of early buyers report cooking with wood than neighbors of late buyers—97% and 92%, respectively, p < .01, Table ). Both groups reported that dinner is the largest meal—approximately 66%. A sizeable proportion of both neighbors of early buyers (24%) and neighbors of late buyers (18%) reported that they never cook at their house (Table ). We included a control variable for household does not cook in subsequent regression analysis.

A higher percentage of neighbors of early buyers report earning income than neighbors of late buyers—91% and 89%, respectively, Table , p < .01. For neighbors who earn income, the majority report earning income in cash and in-kind, 60% of neighbors of early buyers and 55% of neighbors of late buyers. Forty percent of neighbors of late buyers reported earning income in cash in comparison with 32% of neighbors of early buyers. The modality of how neighbors report earning income—in cash, in kind, or both—is balanced across the two groups and t tests do not show a significant difference between the two means.

On the basis of analysis of the most recent Demographic and Health Survey, four relevant assets were selected based on a simple rule of thumb that one asset (TV) would be owned by upper quartile of wealth, two assets (cows and cell phone) would be owned by the median level of wealth, and one asset (radio) would be owned by the upper three quartiles of wealth (Uganda Bureau of Statistics and ICF International Inc., Citation2012). We generated an asset index by counting ownership of the four assets and find 7% of households have none, 25% own one, 48% own two, 17% own three, and 4% own all four assets (Table ).

Among the neighbors who have not purchased a stove 9% of both early and late buyers’ neighbors attended the original sales meeting where the early and late buyers purchased their stove.

Geographic and Social Closeness of the Early or Late Buyers and Their Neighbors

Of the 617 neighbors the early or late buyers listed as ones they “speak to frequently,” half (n = 312) independently mentioned the early or late buyer as someone they speak to frequently and are thus classified as “reciprocated friendships” (Table ).Footnote3 Among the other neighbors who were not recommended by the experimental sample, 28% identified the early or late buyer as someone they speak to frequently (Table ).

On average, neighbors of both early and late buyers are geographically close—0.2 kilometers (median = 0.13, SD = 0.32), or about a 4-minute walk (Table ). This is consistent with the study design and regression results show no effect of distance on our outcome variables.4

Neighbors sampled communicate frequently—63% report communicating daily, and 42% had spoken to their neighbor on the day of the survey. Furthermore, 89% of early and late buyers’ neighbors report consulting with their nearby early or late buyer if they had a problem (Table ).

We worked with our implementing partner Centre for Integrated Research and Community Development to measure the most common social activities in our setting. Field testing identified activities participants were likely to share including: farming, family events, savings groups, church, and kwesika (a community burial service group). In total, 84% of neighbors report sharing at least one of these activities with their neighboring early or late buyer (Table ). A third of neighbors report attending church with the experimental early or late buyer, 22% are part of the same kwesika group, 23% farm together, 20% attend family events together, and 18% are part of the same Rotating Savings and Credit Association.

To test whether attending the same church mattered, we ran a separate regression on all our outcome variables but found no evidence to support this effect. To test the wider effect of shared activities, frequency of contact between neighbors and trust we included the Index of Social Closeness in our main regression specification as a control variable.

Results

Pipeline and Sample Attrition

We interviewed 159 of the 168 early or late buyers (80 early buyers and 79 late buyers). Despite four visits to each household, nine early or late buyers were not home at the time of any of these visits. The enumerator's surveyed 617 neighbors the early or late buyers listed as a neighbor they “spoke to frequently.” Enumerators also had time to collect data from 146 other neighbors not listed by the buyers. Of the neighbors surveyed, 95 had already purchased the stove in the earlier sales study. We dropped these participants from the analysis of uptake.

Balance Tests

We ran t tests on all explanatory variables to compare the means of neighbors of both early and late buyers (Table ). Results are in embedded in Tables . The samples are mostly balanced. One exception is early buyers’ neighbors are slightly closer than late buyers’ neighbors (Table , p < .01), although when we tested whether distance predicts purchase (Specification 2), distance had no effect on neighbors’ decision to purchase the Envirofit.

Table 1a. Summary statistics of matched early and late buyers and their neighbors

Table 1b. Summary statistics related to communication and shared activities between neighbors

Table 1c. Summary Statistics of neighbors’ knowledge and experience with Envirofit cookstove

Table 1d. Summary statistics of sociodemographics for neighbors related to cooking

Living Near an Early Buyer Increases Familiarity With the Envirofit

We asked neighbors whether they had heard about the Envirofit and 26% of all neighbors—22% of early (29% of late) buyers’ neighbors reported having heard about the stove (Table , p < .01). This is inconsistent with Hypothesis 1 that more neighbors of early buyers will have heard about the Envirofit. However, consistent with Hypothesis 1, of those who reported knowing about the Envirofit, more neighbors of early buyers (68%) than neighbors of late buyers (50%) reported hearing about the Envirofit from the experimental sample (early or late buyer, Table , p < .01). Given that a larger portion of neighbors of late buyers have heard about the stove, it is possible they are more exposed to other peer influences related to the Envirofit in the community. Subsequently, neighbors were asked how many people they know who own an Envirofit stove.

Consistent with our theory of behavior change and Hypothesis 2, more neighbors of early buyers reported knowing at least one person who owns the Envirofit. 62% (48%) of neighbors of early (late) buyers reported knowing at least one neighbor who owns the Envirofit (Table , p < .01). Almost twice as many neighbors of early buyers (19%) reported knowing more than one person who own an Envirofit than did neighbors of late buyers (10%; Table , p < .01). Because “knowing about the stove” implies some knowledge of the stove's characteristics, the number of respondents indicating yes is lower than the number indicating that they “know someone who owns the stove.”

Table , column A (Specification 2) tests whether the number of people that the neighbor knows owns the Envirofit is correlated with being an early buyers’ neighbor (β = 0.20, p < .01), with knowing the focal point person (β = 0.55, p < .01), or with the presence of other neighbors who are also an early or late buyer (β = 0.13, p < .01). The results show that the number of people is only partially associated with being an early buyer neighbor; a larger effect is knowing the focal point person, and to a lesser extent other neighbors who are also early or late buyers.

Table 2. Summary statistics for neighbors who have not purchased an Envirofit

Table 3. The effect of a neighbor's ownership of an Envirofit on familiarity and opinions about the Envirofit

Consistent with Hypothesis 2, neighbors of early buyers were much more likely (31%) to have seen someone cook with an Enviroft than were neighbors of late buyers (20%, Table , p < .01). A third more neighbors of early buyers had seen more than one person cook with the Envirofit—9% versus 6% of neighbors of late buyers (Table , p < .01).

Table , column B (Specification 4) estimates whether the number of people the neighbor saw using the Envirofit is explained by being an early buyers’ neighbor (β = 0.13; p < .01), by knowing the focal point person (β = 0.42, p < .01), or by the number of other neighbors who are also an early or late buyer (β = 0.13, p < .01). Seeing someone cook with the Envirofit is associated with being the neighbor of an early buyer neighbor, though, is more correlated with knowing the focal point person and to a lesser extent with having other neighbors who are also early or late buyers.

Living Near an Early Buyer Does Not Lead to a Better Opinion of the Envirofit

We found support for Assumption 4 that buyers in our community like the Envirofit—57% of the 866 total attendees in the previous sales meeting purchased and own the stove (Levine et al., Citation2013). An additional piece of evidence is the previous sales offer included a free trial and only 0.2% returned the stove after the free trial (Levine et al., Citation2013).

Nevertheless, living near an early buyer does not lead to better opinion of the Envirofit (Table ). Neighbors of early buyers are slightly less likely to strongly favor the Envirofit for all three dimensions: health (20% of neighbors of early buyers vs. 26% of neighbors late buyers, p < .05), ease of use (18% vs. 23%, p < .10), and fuel savings (20% vs. 25%, p < .10).

Table , column A (Specification 1), estimates if the opinion that the Envirofit is better for your health than the three stone fire is predicted by being an early buyers’ neighbor (β = −0.06; p < .10), by the neighbor knowing the focal point person (β = 0.22, p < .01), or by the number of neighbors who are also an early or late buyer (β = 0.02, not statistically significant). The main predictor for the Envirofit improves health is knowing the focal point person and to a lesser degree being a neighbor of a late buyer. Table , column B (Specification 2), which estimates if the opinion the Envirofit is better for your health, finds the only predictor is knowing the focal point person (β = 0.17; p < .01). Similarly, Table , column C (Specification 3), which estimates whether the opinion the Envirofit uses less fuel, finds the only predictor is knowing the focal point person (β = 0.19; p < .01). The regression results, consistent with the summary statistics, reject Hypothesis 3: Living near an early buyer improves opinions of the Envirofit.

Table 4. The effect of a neighbor's ownership of an Envirofit on familiarity and opinions about the Envirofit.

Neighbors of late buyers report hearing both more good and more bad things about the Envirofit than the neighbors of early buyers. Twenty-four percent of neighbors of late buyers report hearing good things about the Envirofit versus 19% of neighbors of early buyers (Table , p < .10). At the same time, 5% of neighbors of late buyers report hearing bad things about the Envirofit versus 2% of neighbors of early buyers (Table , p < .05).

We conclude that there is little evidence that having an early buyer as a neighbor raises opinions of the stove. In contrast, knowing the focal point person has the new stove predicts 17–22 percentage points higher odds of strongly favoring the new stove (Table all specifications, p < .01). Thus, peer effects on favorable opinions may operate but not primarily through neighbors. We cannot determine whether the focal point people (compared to neighbors) have more favorable opinions, are more influential, and/or are more likely to report favorable opinions. Because of the focal point person's role in the study, including collecting payments and organizing the sales meetings, they are familiar with the Envirofit's merits.

Living Near an Early Buyer Does Not Detectably Increase Purchase of the Envirofit

Uptake of both early and late buyers’ neighbors of improved cookstoves is 9% for all neighbors (61 of 617 neighbors, Table ) offered the opportunity to purchase the stove. The main results of the experiment are summarized in Table , where we observe no effect of being an early buyer neighbor on purchase of the Envirofit stove (Specification 1), though wealth (proxied by number of assets owned) has a positive effect on a neighbor's decision to purchase an Envirofit (Table , specification 1, β = 0.04, p < .01). This suggests that households purchase decision may be limited by liquidity.

Table 5. Predicting purchase of the Envirofit

Peer Effects Are Not Detectably Stronger for the Geographically and Socially Close

It is possible that peer effects occur, but only for those who are socially close to the early buyer. On the basis of findings from Card and Giuliano (Citation2012) that peer effects are stronger in reciprocated friendships (i.e., when both members of a pair name the other as a close friend) and Bandiera and Rasul (2006), we repeated the analyses on the subset of reciprocated friendships. Table columns 3 and 4 represent the sample of reciprocated friendships and show there is no change from the wider sample and no effect of being an early buyer neighbor on purchase of the Envirofit stove. It is important to note that unlike related studies that have found a large effect of social closeness in estimating peer effects (Bandiera & Rasul, 2006; Card & Giuliano, Citation2012), when we estimated the effect of social closeness and the interaction of early buyer with our index of social closeness, the coefficient was tiny (−0.01, SE = 0.01) and not statistically significant (Table ). We separately tested whether purchase is predicted by attending the same church and found no effect.

Focal Point People Have No Mean Detectable Effect on Purchase Rates

Despite focal point people's large effect on driving opinions of the Envirofit, knowing the focal point person has no effect on purchase rates. This evidence could suggest that liquidity is the most important factor for households in our sample in predicting purchase decisions.

Discussion

We do find evidence of peer demonstration effects in opinions about the Envirofit. In particular, knowing the focal point person has the new stove predicts 17–22 percentage points higher odds of strongly favoring the new stove. Despite evidence of peer effects changing opinions positively about the Envirofit in our community, there is no evidence of the effect on purchase decision among neighbors. Thus, our results do not suggest a large social multiplier for efficient cookstoves similar to the Envirofit. A lack of positive peer effects is understandable for unpopular products (e.g., chlorine for water treatment, as in Luoto et al., Citation2012). However, in our setting, the Envirofit seemed popular.

One possibility for the absence of detectable peer effects on purchase is that households have other information sources. For example, our late buyers all had ordered the new stove and focal point people all owned stoves. More generally, knowledge of stoves does not appear to be a determining factor in adoption. In another experiment we led in the same region but in different communities, we conducted a randomized controlled trial testing whether marketing messages related to the Envirofit improves health or saves times and money had an effect on willingness to pay. Neither marketing message consistently increased willingness to pay (Beltramo, Blalock, Levine, and Simons, Citation2014).

Another possibility is that efficient stoves are widely desired already—most people want one with or without seeing the product in use—but lack the cash. We found that each additional asset increased purchase by 4%. This possibility is consistent with a previous study that found that a free trial and time payments increase the likelihood of purchase from 5% to 57%. Furthermore, each additional household asset owned increased willingness to pay by 10% (Beltramo et al., Citation2014). This result implies concerns about product durability and liquidity are far more important limiting factors in health and welfare improving products. This is consistent with other experimental evidence from Kenya, Guatemala, India, and Uganda, which found no effect of providing information about health preventative products, although genuine learning about the products occur, nor do they find evidence for peer effects though subjects discussed the product purchase decision extensively. Alternatively, they found large effects of liquidity constraints on consumer's purchase of health improving products (Meredith et al., Citation2012).

More research may be needed to understand which, if any, of the aforementioned explanations is responsible for our findings. Knowing the responsible explanation has important implications for health communication and behavioral change more broadly. If our results are generalizable to other products and other locations, then public health officials should prioritize other strategies above word-of-mouth approaches. However, it is difficult to say how generalizable results from the particular stove and community with which we worked are representative. If for example, as previously suggested, our stoves had immediate appeal to our study sample, then word-of-mouth would have had little effect. People wanted the stove even without peer endorsement. Had the same stove, however, been introduced into a more skeptical community, then peer effects might have played a greater role in tempting people to buy one.

A feature of our study that we highlight is its ease of replication in different contexts. Simple randomization of delivery time allows us to identify the impact of peer demonstration. Such randomization can and, in our opinion, should be introduced into future health interventions to build a broader understanding of the role of peer effects in health communications.

In sum, our results do not find that observing peer use of cookstoves influences adoption. A stove promotion strategy of inciting word of mouth by seeding communities with a few demonstration stoves shows little promise. To the extent that our results are generalizable, public health officials should instead direct resources from word-of-mouth strategies to others such as financing for efficient stoves.

Notes

1A parish is an administrative unit that in typically includes three to five villages and has about 5,000–6,300 residents.

2To select the relevant control variables, we first predicted stove uptake using control variables including age, asset index, primary fuel used for cooking is wood, dummy if neighbor receives income in cash only or in cash and in-kind or in-kind only, average household size at a typical meal and household never cooks without Earlyjp. Only the asset index and lack of currently cooking at home predicted purchase (p < .10).

3One late buyer and one early buyer are missing a survey. Despite these two surveys being missing, we have survey data from their neighbors. This accounts for the discrepancy between 312 total recommended neighbors who list early or late buyer in general statistics and 308 with a matched survey in later analysis.

References

  • Abadie, A. & Gay, S. (2004). The impact of presumed consent legislation on cadaveric organ donation: A cross-country study. Journal of Health Economics, 25, 599–620.
  • Arndt, J. (1967). Role of product-related conversations in the diffusion of a new product. Journal of Marketing Research, 4, 291–95.
  • Arts, J. W. C., Frambach, R. T. & Bijmolt, T. H. A. (2011). Generalizations on consumer innovation adoption: A meta-analysis on drivers of intention and behavior. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 28, 134–144.
  • Bandiera, O. & Rasul, I. (2006). Social networks and technology adoption in northern Mozambique. The Economic Journal, 116, 869–902.
  • Bayer, P. J., Pintoff, R. & Pozen, D. (2004). Building criminal capital behind bars: Peer effects in juvenile corrections. SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 441882. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=441882
  • Beltramo, T. (2010). Peer effects and usage of the solar oven: Evidence from rural Senegal (Doctoral dissertation). Università Ca’ Foscari Venezia, Italy. Retrieved from http://dspace.unive.it/handle/10579/1033
  • Beltramo, T., Blalock, G., Levine, D. I. & Simons, A. M. (2014). The effect of marketing messages, liquidity constraints, and household bargaining on willingness to pay for a nontraditional cookstove (Working Paper U.C. Berkeley CEGA #35, February). Retrieved from http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/4vj3w941
  • Beshears, J., Choi, J., Laibson, D. & Madrian, B. (2006). The importance of default options for retirement savings outcomes: Evidence from the United States. In J. Brown J. Liebman & D. A. Wise (Eds.), Social Security policy in a changing environment (pp. 167–195). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/chapters/c4539.pdf.
  • Bursztyn, L., Ederer, F., Ferman, B. & Yuchtman, N. (2012). Understanding peer effects in financial decisions: Evidence from a field experiment. NBER Working Paper 18241. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.
  • Card, D. & Giuliano, L. (2012). Peer effects and multiple equilibria in the risky behavior of friends. Review of Economics and Statistics, 95, 1130–1149.
  • Conley, T. G. & Udry, C. R. (2010). Learning about a new technology: Pineapple in Ghana. American Economic Review, 100, 35–69.
  • Duncan, G., Boisjoly, J., Kremer, M., Levy, D. M. & Eccles, J. (2005). Peer effects in drug use and sex among college students. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 375–385.
  • Foster, A. D. & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1995). Learning by doing and learning from others: Human capital and technical change in agriculture. Journal of Political Economy, 103, 1176–1209.
  • Glaeser, E. L., Sacerdote, B. I. & Scheinkman, J. A. (2003). The social multiplier. Journal of the European Economic Association, 1, 345–353.
  • Guryan, J., Kroft, K. & Notowidigdo, M. J. (2009). Peer effects in the workplace: Evidence from random groupings in professional golf tournaments. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1, 34–68.
  • Harrell, S., Beltramo, T., Levine, D. I., Blalock, G. & Simons, A. M. (2013). What is a “meal? Comparing methods to determine cooking events. Working Paper WP 2013–20. Cornell, NY: Cornell University. Retrieved from http://dyson.cornell.edu/research/researchpdf/wp/2013/Cornell-Dyson-wp1320.pdf
  • Johnson, E. & Goldstein, D. (2003, November). Do defaults save lives? Science, 302, 1338–1339.
  • Kling, J. R., Liebman, J. B. & Katz, L. F. (2007). Experimental analysis of neighborhood effects. Econometrica, 75, 83–119.
  • Levine, D. I., Beltramo, T., Blalock, G. & Cotterman, C. (2013). What impedes efficient adoption of products? Evidence from randomized variation in sales offers for improved cookstoves in Uganda. Working Paper, Berkeley Center for Effective Global Action #14, June. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/86v4x8nn#page-1
  • Lim, S. S., Vos, T., Flaxman, A. D., Danaei, G., Shibuya, K., Adair-Rohani, H., Amann, M., … Ezzati, M. (2012). A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet, 380, 2224–2260.
  • Luoto, J., Mahmud, M., Albert, J., Luby, S., Najnin, N., Unicomb, L. & Levine, D. I. (2012). Learning to dislike safe water products: Results from a randomized controlled trial of the effects of direct and peer experience on willingness to pay. Environmental Science & Technology, 46, 6244–6251.
  • Manski, C. (1993). Identification of endogenous social effects: The reflection problem. Review of Economic Studies, 60, 531–542.
  • Mbondo, G. D. (2013). Social learning through social networks and technological appropriation: The role of peer effects in the adoption and use of the Internet in Cameroonian Tontines. International Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1, 39–42.
  • Meredith, J., Robinson, J., Walker, S. & Wydick, B. (2012, October). Keeping the doctor away: Experimental evidence on investment in preventative health products. Berkeley, CA: Center for Effective Global Action. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/24715228#page-1
  • Miguel, E. & Kremer, M. (2004). Worms: Identifying impacts on education and health in the presence of treatment externalities. Econometrica, 72, 159–217.
  • Moffitt, R. (2001). Policy interventions, low-level equilibria, and social interactions. In S. Durlauf & P. Young (Eds.), Social dynamics (pp. 45–82). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Moretti, E. (2011). Social learning and peer effects in consumption: Evidence from movie sales. Review of Economic Studies, 78, 356–393.
  • Morrison, D. G. (1979). Purchase intentions and purchase behavior. Journal of Marketing, 43, 65–74.
  • Oster, E. & Thornton, R. (2009). Determinants of technology adoption: Private value and peer effects in menstrual cup take-up. NBER Working Paper 114828. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.
  • Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of Innovations. New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Sacerdote, B. (2011). Peer effects in education: How might they work, how big are they and how much do we know thus far? In E. A. Hanushek S. Machin & L. Woessmann (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (vol. 3, pp. 249–277). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/h/eee/educhp/3-04.html
  • Uganda Bureau of Statistics and ICF International Inc. (2012). Uganda Demographic and Health Survey 2011. Kampala, Uganda: UBOS; Calverton, MD: ICF International Inc.
  • Verplanken, B. & Wood, W. (2006). Interventions to break and create consumer habits. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25, 90–103.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.