24,578
Views
13
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Consumerism and well-being in early adolescence

, &
Pages 802-820 | Received 22 Sep 2011, Accepted 11 Apr 2012, Published online: 21 May 2012

Abstract

It has been suggested that consumerism is negatively related to well-being in children and adolescents, as well as adults. Few studies have explored whether certain aspects of consumerism have stronger associations with well-being than others, or between-group differences in associations. This article uses data from a sample of early adolescents to examine: levels of consumerism; relationships between different aspects of consumerism and well-being; and differences according to gender, school year group and family affluence. Data were obtained in 2010 via secondary school pupil surveys (N=2934). Consumerism measures comprised number of ‘standard’ and ‘premium’ possessions and four dimensions of consumer involvement; well-being measures comprised self-esteem, psychological distress and anger. There was evidence of high penetration of consumerist values. There were positive associations between number of possessions and anger, and between ‘dissatisfaction’ and poorer well-being, regardless of how measured. ‘Brand awareness’ was associated with positive male well-being, but negative female well-being. Many relationships between consumerism and well-being were stronger than those between family affluence and well-being. These results suggest only certain aspects of consumerism are associated with poorer adolescent well-being. Although, for some sub-groups, other aspects might be associated with better well-being, there was no evidence that modern consumer goods promote happiness.

Introduction

Consumerism or materialismFootnote1, referring to conspicuous consumption or beliefs that goods are a means to happiness generally or personally, has been highlighted as one of today's ‘social evils’ (Joseph Rowntree Foundation Citation2009) and as detrimental to health and well-being (Eckersley Citation2006, Citation2011). This study examines levels of several different dimensions of consumerism and associations between these measures of consumerism and psychological well-being in a large sample of Scottish early adolescents. We begin by describing scales which measure the concept of consumerism, their associations with well-being, and differences according to gender and socio-economic status (SES). The majority of work on consumerism has focused on adults (Bottomley et al. Citation2010), and a glance at the references for this article shows that most of the literature in this area has been located in consumerism or marketing journals rather than those relating to young people. This is despite the fact that the impact of modern consumer culture is likely to be most marked among young people (Eckersley Citation2011).

Among adults, an early study (Belk Citation1984) examined three measures, possessiveness, non-generosity and envy. Subsequently, scales were developed reflecting beliefs that possession of goods is a means to happiness and life satisfaction (Richins Citation1987, Citation2004). Much subsequent work has been based on the Belk and Richins scales. A slightly different approach is represented by an index of aspirations, allowing identification of those who value financial success above other aspirations such as self-acceptance (Kasser and Ryan Citation1993).

Among adolescents, a study of ‘consumer learning’ used a brief scale to measure an ‘orientation emphasising possessions and money for personal happiness and social progress’ (Ward and Wackman Citation1971, p. 426). Studies of American adolescents conducted since 2000 have developed a 10-item ‘Youth Materialism Scale’ for 9–14 year olds (Goldberg et al. Citation2003) and an 18-item ‘Consumer Involvement Scale’ comprising a broad range of consumer attitudes, values and activities for 10–13 year olds (Schor Citation2004). While these scales measure consumerism as a personality attribute and/or value, a more recent study of 8–18 year olds measured the concept via the number of material possessions (e.g. ‘money’, ‘new clothes’, ‘my own cell phone’) included on a collage of things which ‘make you happy’ (Chaplin and John Citation2007).

It has been suggested that a consistent finding from this stream of research is that consumerism is negatively related to overall life satisfaction (Sirgy Citation1998). In adult samples, higher levels of possessiveness, non-generosity and envy have been associated with reduced happiness and satisfaction (Belk Citation1984), general affect (La Barbera and Gurhan Citation1997) and emotional well-being (Kashdan and Breen Citation2007). Negative relationships have also been found between consumerism and both life satisfaction and self-esteem (Richins and Dawson Citation1992, Christopher et al. Citation2007). In addition, among young adults, those valuing financial success above other aspirations have been found to have poorer well-being, assessed both via self-report and structured psychiatric interview (Kasser and Ryan Citation1993).

Focusing on children and adolescents, American studies have found relationships between consumerism and psychosomatic symptoms, depression, anxiety and lower self-esteem in 9–13 year olds (Schor Citation2004), increased anxiety and reduced happiness and self-esteem in 10–18 year olds (Kasser Citation2005) and reduced self-esteem in 8–18 year olds (Chaplin and John Citation2007). In UK studies, consumerism was found to be related to increased self-reported emotional and behavioural problems in 11–19 year olds (Flouri Citation2004) and to lower self-esteem and higher parent–child conflict among 9–13 year olds (Nairn et al. Citation2007). Consumerism was also associated with parent–child conflict and disappointment in Dutch 8–12 year olds (Buijzen and Valkenburg Citation2003), while agreement with statements such as ‘I'd be happier if I could afford to buy more things’ was negatively related to life satisfaction among Hungarian 14–21 year olds (Piko Citation2006).

Most studies of children and adolescents reporting on gender differences in consumerism describe higher levels among males (Churchill and Moschis Citation1979, Goldberg et al. Citation2003, Flouri Citation2004, Kasser Citation2005), although one found no gender difference (Nairn et al. Citation2007). Results in respect of age are inconsistent. Some report no differences in levels according to either age or school grade (Ward and Wackman Citation1971, Goldberg et al. Citation2003, Kasser Citation2005). However, studies have variously found: a positive relationship between consumerism and age among 11–19 year olds (Flouri Citation2004); that consumerism increased from middle childhood to early adolescence then declined in late adolescence (Chaplin and John Citation2007); that pupils in the final two years of (UK) primary schooling reported more consumerism than those in the first two years at secondary school (Nairn et al. Citation2007); and that high status brands become particularly important as children move into secondary school (UNICEF UK/Ipsos Mori Citation2011).

Findings in respect of SES appear more consistent. Thus, in American studies, more consumer-oriented 18 year olds were found to have grown up in less advantageous socio-economic circumstances (Kasser et al. Citation1995), 9–14 year olds with the highest levels of consumerism tended to be drawn from families with lower incomes (Goldberg et al. Citation2003) and 9–13 year olds from less well-off families displayed higher levels of consumer involvement (Schor Citation2004). Similarly, in the UK, consumerism scores were higher among 14–16 year olds from working class, compared with middle class-backgrounds (Dittmar and Pepper Citation1994) and among 9–13 year olds from deprived compared with affluent areas (Nairn et al. Citation2007). It has been suggested that levels of consumerism are higher among more disadvantaged children and adolescents because they feel insecure about themselves (Isaksen and Roper Citation2012). Such ideas draw from earlier literatures describing SES differences in responses to material rather than symbolic rewards (Douvan Citation1956), an emphasis on the present rather than planning ahead (Hoggart Citation1957) and a focus on material goods rather than experience (Martineau Citation1958). An alternative explanation is that since material possessions are more readily available to advantaged groups, they are not perceived as particularly prestigious (Belk et al. Citation1984).

Despite this evidence of between-group differences in levels of consumerism, few studies have explored whether there are also between-group differences in the relationships between consumerism and well-being. The UK study of 9–13 year olds which found consumerism to be related to self-esteem and parent–child conflict found ‘some differences in strength of association’ according to gender and SES, but does not report these in detail (Nairn et al. Citation2007). There is some evidence from adults that consumerism has a stronger association with poorer well-being among those with lower incomes or less education (La Barbera and Gurhan Citation1997, Burroughs and Rindfleisch Citation2002). It has been suggested that pressures to consume may result in a damaged self-concept among low-income adolescents who cannot keep up with the latest trends (Isaksen and Roper Citation2008). In line with this, studies of UK 8–13 and 15–16 year olds have concluded that visible, high status brands are more important to those from less affluent backgrounds (UNICEF UK/Ipsos Mori Citation2011, Isaksen and Roper Citation2012).

Further, although most studies report on relationships between an overall consumerism score and well-being, certain aspects may have stronger associations than others. For example, there is evidence that while personal consumerism (‘I'd be happier if …’) is associated with reduced well-being in both adults and adolescents, more general beliefs (‘it's really true money can buy happiness’) are not (Richins Citation1987, Piko Citation2006). Although measures of possessiveness, non-generosity and envy are generally combined into a single scale, one adult study found that possessiveness had weaker relationships with unhappiness than the other two dimensions (Belk Citation1984), and another that envy had a stronger relationship with affect than did non-generosity (La Barbera and Gurhan Citation1997). Similarly, analyses based on one of the measures used in this article found that while ‘dissatisfaction’ had a strong relationship with depression and anxiety among US 9–13 year olds (Schor Citation2004) and with self-esteem among US 11–15-year-old females (Bottomley et al. Citation2010), relationships between ‘consumer orientation’ or ‘brand awareness’ and these measures of well-being were weaker (Schor Citation2004) or non-existent (Bottomley et al. Citation2010).

This article examines levels of consumerism and associations between different aspects of consumerism (ownership of material possessions and measures of consumer involvement) and well-being (self-esteem, psychological distress and anger) among a large sample of Scottish school pupils in the first three years of secondary education (mean ages 12, 13 and 14 years). Given the possibility that high status brands may be particularly important for lower SES groups (Isaksen and Roper Citation2008, UNICEF UK/Ipsos Mori Citation2011, Isaksen and Roper Citation2012), our measures of consumerism include both ‘standard’ and more desirable ‘premium’ possessions. Further, we include four dimensions of consumer involvement (‘brand awareness’, ‘dissatisfaction’, ‘consumer orientation’ and ‘consumer indifference’) since there is evidence that certain aspects of consumer involvement may have stronger associations with well-being than others. Well-being is represented by self-esteem, psychological distress and a simple measure of anger. Differences in both levels of material possessions and consumer involvement and in the relationship between these measures and well-being are examined according to gender, school year (as a proxy for both age and the influence of peers (Nairn et al. Citation2007)) and family affluence. Finally, multivariate analyses allow comparison of the independent associations which both family affluence and consumerism have with well-being.

Methods

Sample

Data are drawn from a survey of Scottish Secondary school pupils conducted in 2010. The study was approved by the relevant Glasgow University Ethics Committee, local education authorities and schools. To maximise representativeness, we selected schools with different socio-economic catchments (as indicated by the proportion in receipt of free school meals) from two urban and semi-rural areas in Scotland's central belt. Within the seven selected schools, all pupils in Secondary 1–3 (S1–S3) year groups were initially invited to participate via letters to their parents enclosing parental opt-out consent forms. Pupils separately received information about the study and consented prior to participation.

Pupils filled in a questionnaire in examination-type conditions during school-based sessions. The sample comprised 1496 males and 1438 females (92% of the eligible sample of 3189). Levels of non-consent were very low; almost all non-responders were those absent on the survey days. Mean (SD) ages were 12.0 (0.33) years in S1, 13.0 (0.29) in S2 and 14.0 (0.34) in S3.

Measures

Material possessions

Informal discussions with early adolescents during questionnaire development generated a list of 12 possessions which they considered desirable. Pupils indicated whether they owned each item on the list. For the purpose of analysis, more expensive or recently released items were categorised as ‘premium’ (Xbox 360, PS3 or Wii; PSP; iPod or iPhone; iPod dock; own laptop; LCD TV in bedroom; touch screen mobile phone – range = 0–7, mean = 4.1, standard deviation = 1.7) and the remaining items as ‘standard’ (any other games console; any other MP3 player; own PC; any other TV in bedroom; any other mobile phone – range = 0–5, mean = 2.7, standard deviation = 1.2).

Consumer involvement

Pupils completed 16 items based on the ‘Consumer Involvement’ scale administered to around 300 American 10–13 year olds by Schor (Citation2004). We included the 16 (of 18 original) items which Schor found grouped into a single factor (with three further sub-factors) (). Pupils indicated agreement with each item via a 4-point scale.

Table 1. Material possessions according to gender and family affluence – percentages with chi-square (and significance).

Table 2. Responses to consumerism scale items – percentages.

Well-being

Pupils completed a 10 item self-esteem scale (based on Rosenberg (Citation1965)), including items such as ‘I am pretty sure of myself’, with a 4-point response, summed to produce a total score (possible range 0–36; actual range 2–30). For the purposes of analysis, pupils scoring 17 or below (26.8%) were categorised as ‘lower self-esteem’. To measure psychological distress, pupils completed the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) which has been validated for use with younger adolescents (Tait et al. Citation2003). The GHQ was designed as a measure of state, focusing on inability to carry out normal functions (e.g. ‘been able to enjoy your normal day-to-day activities’) and emergence of distressing symptoms (e.g. ‘felt constantly under strain’). Each item includes four answer options. We used binary scoring (0-0-1-1) and the standard cut-off, which is a score of three or above (Goldberg and Williams Citation1988). This classified 21.3% as GHQ ‘cases’ (potentially clinically significant levels of distress). To represent anger, responses to a single item (‘I get angry when anybody tells me what to do’) were dichotomised into ‘describes me very/quite well’ (18.3%) and ‘describes me a bit/not at all’.

Family Affluence Scale (FAS)

This scale, originally developed for use by the International Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children Study (Currie et al. Citation2008) includes items in respect of: number of family cars, vans or trucks; having own (not shared) bedroom; number of family computers; and number of family holidays in the past year. It was collapsed into low (scale scores 0–3), mid (4–5) and high (6–7) categories.

Analyses

Descriptive statistics for material possessions and the Consumer Involvement Scale were examined first. Preliminary unconstrained factor analyses (principal components analysis, varimax rotation) of all 16 items in the Consumer Involvement Scale found inconsistent results for gender, school year and FAS sub-groups. However, when two items (‘I like shopping’; ‘I wish my parents earned more money’) which Schor reported as not closely connected with any of the sub-scales (Schor Citation2004) were removed, unconstrained factor analyses produced a four-factor solution which was very similar in each sub-group (detailed results available on request). Factor analysis was therefore based on 14 items, conducted on the whole sample and the resulting four-factor scores saved. Since analyses by both Schor (Citation2004) and, more recently by Bottomley et al. (Citation2010) of both US and UK samples, had indicated a three-factor structure, additional confirmatory factor analyses were conducted. These indicated that the four-factor model based on 14 items (χ 2=670.6, df = 71, CFI = 0.928) fitted our data significantly better than a three-factor model based on either 14 items (χ 2=844.6, df = 74, CFI = 0.907) or on Schor's original 16 items (χ 2=1250.6, df = 101, CFI = 0.891) (detailed results available on request).

Analyses of variance examined differences in material possessions and the consumerism factor scores by gender, school year group and FAS.

Logistic regression was used to determine the associations which number of material possessions and each of the four consumer involvement factor scores had with low self-esteem, GHQ ‘caseness’ and anger. Since there was some evidence of interactions with gender, separate analyses were conducted for males and females. We report both unadjusted and mutually adjusted relationships, the latter also controlling for year group and FAS. All logistic regression analyses were conducted on those with complete data on all relevant variables, reducing the sample sizes to 2599 (low self-esteem), 2637 (GHQ) and 2699 (anger).

Results

On average, pupils owned 6.8 (SD = 2.0) of the 12 material possessions; 1.2% had 2 or fewer, 10% 10 or more. Around 90% had an Xbox 360, PS3 or Wii, almost three quarters any other games console and over half an MP3 player, laptop, LCD TV in their bedroom, iPod dock and/or non-touch screen mobile. Even the least popular, a PC, was possessed by over a third of the sample. shows ownership of each possession according to gender and FAS. Five items were more likely to be owned by males (Xbox 360, PS3 or Wii; PSP; LCD Tv; PC; other mobile) and five by females (iPod dock; laptop; touch screen phone; other games console; other MP3 player) with no gender differences in respect of two items (iPod touch or iPhone; other TV). With the exception of two ‘standard’ items (other TV in bedroom; other mobile), all were most likely among those from high FAS households, particularly ‘premium’ items such as an iPod touch or iPhone (23% low FAS, 50% high FAS), iPod dock (23% and 67%) and own laptop (44 and 69%).

shows responses to the Consumer Involvement Scale. Although around 80% (strongly) agreed that ‘I have pretty much everything I need I terms of possessions’, around a third wished their parents gave them more money to spend and that their parents earned more, a quarter felt other kids had more ‘stuff’ and a fifth wished their family could afford to buy them more. However, over 90% claimed not to care what others had when deciding who to be friends with. Almost all (96%) wanted to make a lot of money when they grew up, and the vast majority usually had a new acquisition in mind (86%), liked shopping (78%), buying souvenirs (80%) and cared a lot about their own possessions (80%). Only a quarter claimed not to care about their clothes (28%), around half believed that being cool (47%) and brand names (59%) were important and two-thirds liked clothes with popular labels (69%). However, three quarters did not care what sort of car their family had.

Schor (Citation2004) identified three consumer involvement sub-factors which she labelled ‘dissatisfaction’ (items 1–5 in ), ‘consumer orientation’ (items 6–9) and ‘brand awareness’ (items 10–14); items 15 and 16 were not closely connected with these sub-scales. Our factor analyses suggested four factors (), three of which were the same or similar to those identified by Schor, and given the same labels. The fourth factor, representing only two items (‘When I decide who to be friends with, I don't care what toys or stuff the person has’ and ‘It doesn't matter to me what kind of car my family has’) was labelled ‘consumer indifference’. ‘Brand awareness’ explained 23% of the total variance; ‘dissatisfaction’ 15%; ‘consumer orientation’ 11%; and ‘consumer indifference’ 7% (total variance explained = 56%).

Table 3. Factor analysis (varimax) of consumer items – variance explained and rotated component matrix.

Correlations between number of possessions and each of the consumer involvement factor scores were generally modest (‘brand awareness’ with ‘standard’ items r=0.022, p=0.260, with ‘premium’ items r=0.317, p=.000; ‘dissatisfaction’ with ‘standard’ r=0.014, p=0.477, with ‘premium’ r=−0.168, p=0.000; ‘consumer orientation’ with ‘standard’ r=.108, p=.000, with ‘premium’ r=0.124, p=0.000; ‘consumer indifference’ with ‘standard’ r=0.012, p=0.546, with ‘premium’ r=−0.050, p=0.008). Thus, while number of ‘premium’ possessions was related positively to ‘brand awareness’ and negatively to ‘dissatisfaction’, number of ‘standard’ possessions was not related to either. Numbers of both types of possessions had a very weak positive relationship with ‘consumer orientation’ but no relationship with ‘consumer indifference’. The four consumer involvement scores were unrelated to each other, reflecting their derivation via factor analysis.

As shows, 13 of the 18 comparisons examining differences in material possessions and the consumerism factor scores according to gender, year group and FAS were significant. However, most effect sizes were very small, only two (FAS differences in ‘premium’ possessions and ‘dissatisfaction’) with partial eta squared >0.02. Number of ‘premium’ possessions and the consumer involvement factors ‘brand awareness’, ‘dissatisfaction’ and ‘consumer orientation’ were higher among males, while ‘consumer indifference’ was lower. The S2 year group had the highest number of both ‘standard’ and ‘premium’ possessions, while ‘brand awareness’ and ‘dissatisfaction’ were highest among the oldest year group (S3s). Finally, reflecting responses in respect of the individual possessions, FAS was unrelated to number of ‘standard’ items, but positively associated with number of ‘premium’ items. FAS was also positively related to both ‘brand awareness’ and ‘consumer orientation’, and negatively to ‘dissatisfaction’.

Table 4. Number of material possessions and consumer factor scores according to gender, year group and family affluence.

Logistic regression analyses examined associations which material possessions and consumer involvement had with the three well-being measures. First, to determine whether associations differed by gender, a series of models entered: ‘standard’ possessions, gender, ‘standard’ possessions by gender; ‘premium’ possessions, gender, ‘premium’ possessions by gender; etc, for each outcome (i.e. 18 models). Of these, four interactions with gender were significant (‘standard’ possessions by gender in respect of low self-esteem, p=0.033, and ‘brand awareness’ by gender in respect of each measure of well-being, p<0.001) (). The exercise was repeated, replacing gender with year group (no significant interactions) and FAS (one significant interaction – ‘standard’ possessions by FAS in respect of low self-esteem, p=0.022). Given evidence of some consistent gender differences, subsequent analyses were conducted separately for males and females.

Table 5. ‘Low’ self-esteem, GHQ-12 ‘caseness’ and get angry according to year group, family affluence, material possessions and consumer factors: unadjusted odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) for males and females and significance of interaction with gender; mutually adjusted OR with 95% CI for males and females.

shows the associations which year group, FAS, possessions and the consumerism factors had with the well-being measures amongst males and females; unadjusted and mutually adjusted odds ratios are presented for each. Focusing first on the relationships which year group and FAS had with well-being, the odds of low self-esteem increased with year group among females, and were also somewhat higher among pupils from low compared with high FAS backgrounds. GHQ ‘caseness’ increased with year group in both males and females and, among males only, was lower among those from more affluent households. The tendency to get angry did not show consistent or significant patterns with year group or FAS. Examination of the Wald statistic shows that relationships between FAS and well-being were weaker than many of those between the measures of consumerism and well-being. This was particularly the case among females and in respect of anger.

‘Standard’ and ‘premium’ items showed somewhat different relationships with the three well-being measures. Relationships between possessions and both low self-esteem and GHQ caseness were generally weak, although owning more ‘standard’ possessions was associated with significantly increased odds of low self-esteem among females, and of GHQ ‘caseness’ among males. Anger showed a consistent relationship with possessions; amongst both males and females (in both unadjusted and adjusted analyses) the odds of anger were significantly elevated amongst those with more ‘standard’ and ‘premium’ possessions.

There were gender differences in the relationships between ‘brand awareness’ and each of the three well-being measures. Higher ‘brand awareness’ was associated with decreases in the odds of both low self-esteem and GHQ ‘caseness’ amongst males, but increases in adjusted analyses amongst females, and with a much smaller increase in anger amongst males than females.

Higher levels of ‘dissatisfaction’ were consistently associated with increased odds of low self-esteem, GHQ ‘caseness’ and anger in both males and females (unadjusted and adjusted analyses), and the Wald statistic confirmed that for each well-being measure, this had the strongest relationship out of all the consumer involvement factors. ‘Consumer orientation’ and the ‘consumer indifference’ factor showed few or no relationships with the measures of well-being in these young people. The only statistically significant relationship in mutually adjusted analyses was increased odds of anger amongst females with greater ‘consumer orientation’.

Additional analyses (not shown), based on the mutually adjusted models, explored the ‘standard’ possessions by FAS interaction observed for low self-esteem in unadjusted analysis of the whole sample (described earlier). Among males, the ‘standard’ possessions by FAS interaction in the mutually adjusted model was non-significant (p=0.201). Among females, the interaction was significant (p=0.025) and the associations were OR = 0.62 (95% CI = 0.41–0.94) in those from low; OR = 1.32 (1.11–1.56) in those from mid; and OR = 1.21 (1.04–1.41) in those from high family affluence backgrounds. Thus, more ‘standard’ items were associated with higher self-esteem among the least affluent females, but lower self-esteem among those from more affluent backgrounds (the same pattern occurred for males, but relationships were weaker).

Discussion

Almost all this sample of contemporary western early adolescents possessed large numbers of electronic gaming, media and communication devices, yet most demonstrated a strong ‘consumer orientation’ in terms of wanting to acquire more and a minority expressed ‘dissatisfaction’ with levels of possessions or disposable income. A major focus of this article was associations between consumerism and well-being. We found the different dimensions of consumerism were associated with different aspects of adolescent well-being in different ways and to different degrees. Many of the relationships were stronger than those between family affluence and well-being. The clearest relationships between number of possessions (regardless of type) and well-being occurred in respect of anger. Associations with ‘brand awareness’ differed by gender. Consumerist ‘dissatisfaction’ showed the strongest relationships with well-being, while ‘consumer orientation’ and ‘consumer indifference’ had weaker, or no relationships with well-being. We discuss findings relating to levels of possessions and consumer involvement and their socio-demographic patterning first.

Social inequalities existed not so much in terms of whether these early adolescents owned a games console, computer, TV or phone, since almost all did, but rather in terms of items categorised for analytic purposes as ‘premium’. However, even though the more affluent possessed more items which were expensive, recently released and/or had additional features, these were also possessed by a large minority of the less affluent. Such items were associated with ‘brand awareness’, and may have enhanced their owners’ peer status (Lease et al. Citation2002, Meisinger et al. Citation2007). Although over 80% of the sample felt they had ‘pretty much everything I need in terms of possessions’, the relationship between (more) ‘premium’ possessions and (less) ‘dissatisfaction’ was relatively weak; not all with large amounts of desirable ‘stuff’ felt satisfied with what they had, and over three quarters of the sample usually had a new acquisition in mind. Interestingly, there was no association between number of ‘standard’ possessions and either ‘brand awareness’ or ‘dissatisfaction’. It may be that among western adolescents, such non-premium items now have little meaning.

Responses in respect of the ‘Consumer Involvement’ scale from this sample of Scottish 11–14 year olds in 2010 can be compared with those obtained from American 10–13 year olds in 2001–2002 (Schor Citation2004). The American sample expressed higher rates of dissatisfaction via responses to ‘other kids have more stuff’ and wishing their family could afford to buy more or that their parents gave them more money, but appeared less interested in brands, labels and ‘cool’. It remains open to speculation whether the variation represents cultural (USA vs. Scotland) or secular (2001–2002 vs. 2010) differences. Our factor analysis suggested almost identical dimensions to those obtained from previous US and UK samples (Schor Citation2004, Bottomley et al. Citation2010), with the addition of a fourth factor which we termed ‘consumer indifference’. However, it is possible this reflects something else. It included only two items, both of which were shown to have very low loadings on one- and three-factor scale solutions in another study (Bottomley et al. Citation2010). Unlike the other Consumer Involvement Scale items, neither is directly related to ‘me’/'I’, and previous authors have suggested that one (‘When I decide who to be friends with …’) might be prone to socially desirable responding (Bottomley et al. Citation2010).

Our finding of higher levels of all our consumerism measures among males is consistent with most other studies (Churchill and Moschis Citation1979, Goldberg et al. Citation2003, Flouri Citation2004, Kasser Citation2005). Males also had higher numbers of ‘premium’ possessions, but the difference was small. Although this could reflect a male bias in our list of ‘technology’, it is notable that a number of these items were actually more likely to be owned by females, with the largest male excess in respect of ‘premium’ games consoles. It would be very difficult to construct a list of age-appropriate items which would not otherwise be highly gendered (e.g. particular brands of clothing; make-up). A US study of material dimensions of social status, conducted in 2006 among 14–18 year olds noted the importance of ‘higher-priced material goods’, including technology such as iPods, particular brands of mobile phone, laptop computers and Bluetooth (Sweet Citation2010). Any school year group differences in material possessions and consumerism were small and inconsistent, again supporting other studies (Ward and Wackman Citation1971, Goldberg et al. Citation2003).

Most studies suggest greater consumerism among those from lower SES backgrounds. Our findings suggest different SES patterning for different dimensions of consumerism. Pupils from less affluent families reported greater ‘dissatisfaction’. However, the more affluent had more ‘premium’ possessions, greater ‘brand awareness’ and ‘consumer orientation’. This might be expected, given the greater resources of the more affluent. It is also in line with an earlier US study which suggested greater recognition of the symbolism of such products among 9–12 year olds from higher, compared with lower SES backgrounds (Belk et al. Citation1984). However, it contrasts with a Scottish study which found that while book ownership was more likely among adolescents from more affluent households, items representing ‘new consumer’ possessions were more likely among the less affluent (West et al. Citation2006). Similarly, a qualitative study of English 15–16 year olds found that those from low-income households placed greater importance on visible ‘status’ symbols, expensive clothing brands and money than their high-income peers (Isaksen and Roper Citation2012).

Analyses of the associations between consumerism and well-being included two ‘control’ variables, year group (age) and family affluence. In line with other studies, (e.g. Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus Citation1994, Sweeting and West Citation2003) we found evidence of greater reductions in well-being with increasing age among females than males. While there was evidence of poorer well-being among males from lower family affluence groups, in females there were no significant differences according to affluence, consistent with suggestions that when compared with childhood or adulthood, youth is a period of ‘relative equality’ in health (West and Sweeting Citation2004).

Our finding that the different dimensions of consumerism were associated with different aspects of adolescent well-being in different ways and to different degrees is important and highlights the need for studies of consumerism to clearly and consistently define the concept(s) measured. As an aspect of well-being, anger was distinguished from self-esteem and distress. Adolescents with greater numbers of both ‘standard’ and ‘premium’ possessions were more likely to report that ‘I get angry whenever anyone tells me what to do’, perhaps reflecting greater narcissistic characteristics. Previous studies have found associations between increased levels of consumerism and both poorer relationships and more aggressive tendencies towards others (Kasser Citation2003). Relationships between possessions and both self-esteem and distress were less consistent. However, ‘dissatisfaction’ had stronger associations with these two measures of well-being than with anger. Previous studies have attributed such findings to an overreliance on unfulfilling, extrinsic relations with objects rather than more rewarding relations with people (Kasser and Ryan Citation1993), a focus on the acquisition of possessions in an unsuccessful attempt to offset feelings of unhappiness or insecurity (Chaplin and John Citation2007) and the belief that there is more to life than what one currently has (Bottomley et al. Citation2010, Eckersley Citation2011).

Results were further complicated by the finding that certain aspects of consumerism were associated with well-being in different ways for different groups. ‘Brand awareness’ was related to better male, but worse female well-being. Previous studies have found that brand name clothing is particularly important for adolescent social status (Sweet Citation2010, Isaksen and Roper Citation2012). In adolescence, male ‘brand awareness’ may be particularly associated with sports-related clothes and accessories (at a life-stage when sporting prowess or affiliation are highly valued), while ‘brand aware’ females may be ‘top girls’ experiencing stress associated with maintaining peer-status (Michell and Amos Citation1997, West et al. Citation2010). Alternatively, more insecure females may seek to bolster well-being and position in the pecking order through brand choice, ‘cool’ clothes and possessions; quite literally ‘retail therapy’. Previous studies have suggested consumerism might be more detrimental for less affluent individuals (La Barbera and Gurhan Citation1997, Burroughs and Rindfleisch Citation2002, Isaksen and Roper Citation2008). We found some evidence that greater numbers of ‘standard’ possessions were associated with higher self-esteem among less affluent adolescents (among whom having some possessions might be better than having none), but lower self-esteem among the more affluent (among whom ‘premium’ possessions were common). However, this was the only instance where the associations between consumerism and well-being differed by family affluence. The balance of the evidence from this study is therefore that consumerism is neither better nor worse for early adolescents from lower SES backgrounds.

This study adds to the body of evidence suggesting that some aspects of consumerism are related to adolescent well-being. In so doing, it highlights the importance of focusing on not only socio-economic, but also cultural factors, in relation to health (Eckersley Citation2011). It is one of only a handful of such studies conducted in the UK, and based on a large sample. An important limitation is that like almost all studies in this area, it is cross-sectional, thus limiting conclusions about causation (Eckersley Citation2006). Some of the associations, particularly between ‘dissatisfaction’ and poorer well-being may result from shared method variance. Poorer well-being may also lead to greater dissatisfaction or, as suggested earlier, attempts to increase happiness via the accumulation of possessions (Belk Citation1985) or style/labels. Although structural equation modelling in one (cross-sectional) study of American pre-adolescents suggested that the direction of effect was from consumer involvement to poorer well-being, rather than the reverse (Schor Citation2004), an alternative conclusion is suggested by the findings of others that inducing high self-esteem reduces expressions of consumerism (Chaplin and John Citation2007, Park and John Citation2011). Path models in studies of UK and Dutch pre-teens have suggested that relationships between consumerism and both self-esteem and parent–child conflict, are mediated by attitude to parents (Nairn et al. Citation2007) and that consumerism is related to disappointment via purchase requests (Buijzen and Valkenburg Citation2003).

In conclusion, this study suggests it may be important to temper the notion that consumerism in general is detrimental to psychological health and well-being. Rather, certain aspects of consumerism, particularly dissatisfaction, might be associated with poorer well-being, others may be unrelated to well-being and, for some sub-groups, yet others related to greater well-being. However, in line with suggestions that material goods do not contribute directly to young people's happiness (UNICEF UK/Ipsos Mori Citation2011), there was no evidence from this sample of western early adolescents that large numbers of modern consumer goods increased well-being. This is an important message, although difficult to communicate amid the plethora of media marketing and advertising in contemporary society.

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the UK Medical Research Council as part of the Gender and Health Programme at the Social and Public Health Sciences Unit (project code MC_A540_5TK50). The authors would like to thank Robert Young, who conducted the confirmatory factor analyses and Sally Macintyre, who provided comments on an earlier version of this paper. Acknowledgements are also due to Judith Schor, whose ‘Consumer Involvement’ scale we used, and to the young people, schools, and all those from the MRC Social and Public Health Sciences Unit involved in the ‘Adolescent Lifestyles in Contemporary Europe’ (‘ALICE’) study.

Notes

1. Previous studies have used both terms and the concepts are often used interchangeably. For the purposes of simplicity and consistency, the term ‘consumerism’ is used throughout this article, with the exception of the one case where a ‘Materialism Scale’ is referred to.

References

  • Belk , R. 1984 . Three scales to measure constructs related to materialism: reliability, validity and relationships to measures of happiness . Advances in consumer research , 11 : 291 – 297 .
  • Belk , R. 1985 . Materialism: trait aspects of living in the material world . Journal of consumer research , 12 : 265 – 280 .
  • Belk , R. , Mayer , R. and Driscoll , A. 1984 . Children's recognition of consumption symbolism in children's products . Journal of consumer research , 10 : 386 – 397 .
  • Bottomley , P. , Nairn , A. , Kasser , T. , Ferguson , Y.L. and Ormrod , J. 2010 . Measuring childhood materialism: refining and validating Schor's Consumer Involvement scale . Psychology and marketing , 27 : 717 – 740 .
  • Buijzen , M. and Valkenburg , P.M. 2003 . The unintended effects of television advertising . Communication research , 30 : 483 – 503 .
  • Burroughs , J.E. and Rindfleisch , A. 2002 . Materialism and well-being: a conflicting values perspective . Journal of consumer research , 29 : 348 – 370 .
  • Chaplin , L.N. and John , D.R. 2007 . Growing up in a material world: age differences in materialism in children and adolescents . Journal of consumer research , 34 : 480 – 493 .
  • Christopher , A.N. 2007 . Materialism, defensive and assertive self-presentational tactics, and life satisfaction . Journal of social and clinical psychology , 26 : 1145 – 1162 .
  • Churchill , G.A. and Moschis , G.P. 1979 . Television and interpersonal influences on adolescent consumer learning . Journal of consumer research , 6 : 23 – 35 .
  • Currie , C. 2008 . Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) family affluence scale . Social science and medicine , 66 : 1429 – 1436 .
  • Dittmar , H. and Pepper , L. 1994 . To have is to be: materialism and person perception in working-class and middle-class British adolescents . Journal of economic psychology , 15 : 233 – 251 .
  • Douvan , E. 1956 . Social status and success strivings . Journal of abnormal and social psychology , 52 : 219 – 223 .
  • Eckersley , R. 2006 . Is modern Western culture a health hazard? . International journal of epidemiology , 35 : 252 – 258 .
  • Eckersley , R. 2011 . A new narrative of young people's health and well-being . Journal of youth studies , 14 : 627 – 638 .
  • Flouri , E. 2004 . Exploring the relationship between mothers’ and fathers’ parenting practices and children's materialist values . Journal of economic psychology , 25 : 743 – 752 .
  • Goldberg , D. and Williams , P. 1988 . A user's guide to the General Health Questionnaire , Windsor : NFER-Nelson .
  • Goldberg , M.E. 2003 . Understanding materialism among youth . Journal of consumer psychology , 13 : 278 – 288 .
  • Hoggart , R. 1957 . The uses of literacy: aspects of working class life , London : Chatto and Windus .
  • Isaksen , K.J. and Roper , S. 2008 . The impact of branding on low-income adolescents: a vicious cycle? . Psychology and marketing , 25 : 1063 – 1087 .
  • Isaksen , K.J. and Roper , S. 2012 . The commodification of self-esteem: branding and British teenagers . Psychology and marketing , 29 : 117 – 135 .
  • Joseph Rowntree Foundation , 2009 . Contemporary social evils . Bristol : Policy Press .
  • Kashdan , T.B. and Breen , W.E. 2007 . Materialism and diminished well-being: experiential avoidance as a mediating mechanism . Journal of social and clinical psychology , 26 : 521 – 539 .
  • Kasser , T. 2003 . The high price of materialism , Cambridge , MA : MIT Press .
  • Kasser , T. 2005 . “ Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents ” . In What do children need to flourish? Conceptializing and measuring indicators of positive development , Edited by: Moore , K.A. and Lippen , L.H. 357 – 373 . New York : Springer .
  • Kasser , T. and Ryan , R.M. 1993 . A dark side of the American dream: correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration . Journal of personality and social psychology , 65 : 410 – 422 .
  • Kasser , T. 1995 . The relations of maternal and social environments to late adolescents’ materialistic and prosocial values . Developmental psychology , 31 : 907 – 914 .
  • La Barbera , P.A. and Gurhan , Z. 1997 . The role of materialism, religiosity, and demographics in subjective well-being . Psychology and marketing , 14 : 71 – 97 .
  • Lease , A. , Kennedy , C. and Axelrod , J. 2002 . Children's social constructions of popularity . Social development , 11 ( 1 ) : 87 – 109 .
  • Martineau , P. 1958 . Social classes and spending behavior . The journal of marketing , 23 : 121 – 130 .
  • Meisinger , E. 2007 . Variant and invariant predictors of perceived popularity across majority-Black and majority-White classrooms . Journal of school psychology , 45 : 21 – 44 .
  • Michell , L. and Amos , A. 1997 . Girls, pecking order and smoking . Social science and medicine , 44 : 1861 – 1869 .
  • Nairn , A. , Ormrod , J. , and Bottomley , P. , 2007 . Watching, wanting and wellbeing: exploring the links . London : National Consumer Council .
  • Nolen-Hoeksema , S. and Girgus , J. 1994 . The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence . Psychological bulletin , 115 : 424 – 443 .
  • Park , J.K. and John , D.R. 2011 . More than meets the eye: the influence of implicit and explicit self-esteem on materialism . Journal of consumer psychology , 21 : 73 – 87 .
  • Piko , B. 2006 . Satisfaction with life, psychosocial health and materialism among Hungarian youth . Journal of health psychology , 11 : 827 – 831 .
  • Richins , M.L. 1987 . Media, materialism and human happiness . Advances in consumer research , 14 : 352 – 356 .
  • Richins , M.L. 2004 . The material values scale: measurement properties and development of a short form . Journal of consumer research , 31 : 209 – 219 .
  • Richins , M.L. and Dawson , S. 1992 . A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: scale development and validation . Journal of consumer research , 19 : 303 – 316 .
  • Rosenberg , M. 1965 . Society and the adolescent self-image , Princeton , NJ : Princeton University Press .
  • Schor , J. 2004 . Born to buy , New York : Scribner .
  • Sirgy , M.J. 1998 . Materialism and quality of life . Social indicators research , 43 : 227 – 260 .
  • Sweet , E. 2010 . “If your shoes are raggedy you get talked about”: symbolic and material dimensions of adolescent social status and health . Social science & medicine , 70 : 2029 – 2035 .
  • Sweeting , H. and West , P. 2003 . Sex differences in health at ages 11, 13 and 15 . Social science and medicine , 56 : 31 – 39 .
  • Tait , R. , French , D. and Hulse , G. 2003 . Validity and psychometric properties of the General Health Questionnaire-12 in young Australian adolescents . Australian and New Zealand journal of psychiatry , 37 : 374 – 381 .
  • Unicef UK/Ipsos Mori , 2011 . Child well-being in the UK, Spain and Sweden: the role of inequality and materialism . London : Unicef UK .
  • Ward , S. and Wackman , D. 1971 . Family and media influences on adolescent consumer learning . American behavioral scientist , 14 : 415 – 427 .
  • West , P. and Sweeting , H. 2004 . Evidence on equalisation in health in youth from the West of Scotland . Social science & medicine , 59 : 13 – 27 .
  • West , P. 2006 . A material paradox: socioeconomic status, young people's disposable income and consumer culture . Journal of youth studies , 9 : 437 – 462 .
  • West , P. 2010 . The relative importance of family socioeconomic status and school-based peer hierarchies for morning cortisol in youth: an exploratory study . Social science & medicine , 70 : 1246 – 1253 .