2,597
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The relation between physical and mental load, and the course of physiological functions and cognitive performance

&
Pages 38-59 | Received 18 Nov 2020, Accepted 01 Apr 2021, Published online: 13 Jul 2021

Abstract

The objective of our study was to investigate differences in cognitive performance connected with physical load of varying intensities. One half of 88 examined persons sat on office chairs, and the other half sat on chairs with the added modification of the gymnastic (Swiss) ball called the dynamic directional seat pad (pad). The first rest phase was followed by the load phase, in which the subjects were administered a 20-minute sustained attention test. The number of correct answers and errors was evaluated. A BIOPAC apparatus continually recorded thoracic respiration, electrodermal activity, finger temperature, heart rate, and heart rate variability. Females on pads made 58% fewer errors than females on chairs; the number of errors was closely related to the depth of their breathing (tidal volume). It was found out that the use of the pad, in addition to the already known health benefits, also brings an increase in the precision of cognitive performance.

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

Relevance to human factors

It was found out that the use of the pad (slightly modified iteration of the Swiss ball), in addition to the already known health benefits, also brings an increase in the precision of cognitive performance (the number of errors was closely related to the depth of breathing). This fact could be helpful wherever the increased frequency of cognitive errors complicates the normal functioning of people in areas of their daily life. The fact that physical load dominates over mental stress in the behavior of physiological functions and that the number of errors is another criterion of cognitive performance besides the speed of work, could be also useful in the field of human factors.

Introduction

Understanding the most elementary components of mental and physical makeup and their relations is a necessary condition for the optimum adjustment of physiological functions that contribute to cognitive performance. However, research in this field still faces various methodological limitations (Boucsein Citation2012a). The fundamental fact of an inverted U relationship between physical activation (arousal) and cognitive performance was for example identified while walking on a treadmill (Levitt and Gutin Citation1971) or riding a bicycle ergometer (Salmela and Ndoye Citation1986; Sjöberg Citation1975; Basahel Citation2012). Although such research studies allow the measurement of heart rate, respiration or reaction time tasks during a physical load, the measurement of electrodermal activity, body temperature, heart rate variability or more complex cognitive processes (Gutin Citation1973) requires another methodological background (Brookhuis, et al. Citation2005; Mulder Citation1992). This methodology makes it possible to better detect the cause of changes in the course of physiological functions, e.g. to detect the influence of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) on the course of cognitive load and vice versa (Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007; de Waard Citation1996; Boucsein Citation2005). Another limitation of such research studies is the separate measurement of physical or mental load (Antunes et al. Citation2006), or the measurement of mental load and cognitive performance before or after the end of physical load (Salmela and Ndoye Citation1986; Coles and Tomporowski Citation2008).

The aim of our study was therefore to investigate and further specify differences in cognitive performance (mental load) and the course of physiological functions connected with simultaneously occurring physical loads of varying intensities.

The findings of our study could be particularly helpful in teaching children with ADHD or ADD, in reducing the number of car accidents by eliminating momentary drowsiness, in curing anxiety and depression (Courtney Citation2009) or in the prevention of neurodegenerative diseases (Kramer and Colcombe Citation2018). In general, it could be helpful wherever the increased frequency of cognitive errors complicates the normal functioning of people in areas of their daily life (Boyer et al. Citation2015; Takeuchi Citation2000).

As a tool for increasing the physical load (Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017), we chose the dynamic directional seat pad (hereinafter “pad”) for our research. It is a smaller, and in shape, slightly modified iteration of the Swiss ball (Carriere Citation1998). Thanks to these modifications, it can be used without complications on a common office chair during everyday sedentary work. The effect of Swiss (gymnastic) balls in the exercise therapy of muscle strength and posture development or muscle relaxation and flexibility has been documented many times (Balakrishnan, Yazid, and Mahat Citation2016; Lee, Bang, and Ko Citation2003; Mills Citation1994; Marshall and Murphy Citation2005; Cosio-Lima et al. Citation2003). Similar effects on muscle strength and posture development were also confirmed for the pad (Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017).

Physiological functions associated with or affecting cognitive functions often mentioned in the literature (Brookhuis, et al. Citation2005; Mulder Citation1992) include respiration (R), electrodermal activity (EDA), heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), and finger temperature (FT).

It seems that there are quantitative and qualitative differences in the two R parameters (and in R patterns in general) depending on whether they are provoked mentally (through cognitive processes, volitional effort, emotions) or physically (physical work, exercise; Ganong Citation2005; Švancara Citation2003; Wientjes Citation1993; de Waard Citation1996). At light to moderate physical loads, higher minute ventilation (MV) is primarily induced by increased tidal volume (VT), while respiratory rate (RR) increases considerably only with a heavy physical load. In contrast, with increasing cognitive load – generally with a slight amount of mental arousal – RR accelerates but VT is shallower (Davies, Haldane, and Priestley Citation1919; Filo Citation2014; Veltman and Gaillard Citation1998; Wientjes Citation1993; Wilson, Fullenkamp, and Davis Citation1994). MV, which is a product of RR and VT over the course of one minute, increases as well. Hence, with mental effort RR accelerates greater than VT decreases. In summary, VT and MV in general are usually distinctly higher with physical load than with cognitive load; however, there is only a minimum difference in RR with the two types of loads (Wientjes Citation1993).

EDA can be considered a psycho-physiological indicator of arousal (Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007; Boucsein et al., 2012b; Boucsein Citation2005; Kramer Citation1990; Rokyta Citation2000; de Waard Citation1996) in the subject, which must not be “contaminated” by thermoregulation processes (Rokyta Citation2000), stress hormones (Weiner Citation1982), and so forth (Boucsein Citation2012a; Kramer Citation1990; Schmidt et al. Citation2017). When the greater cognitive functioning associated with increased attention leads to higher arousal, EDA increases (Fröberg Citation1977; Boucsein Citation2005; Boucsein, Haarmann, and Schaefer Citation2007; Mulder et al. Citation2000; Reimer and Mehler Citation2011; Shimomura et al. Citation2008; Son et al. Citation2011). EDA is also affected by physical load and by irregularities in respiration (EDA grows with deeper breathing; see Boucsein Citation2012a).

There is no question about the effects of cognitive processes on HR and HRV (Brookhuis, et al. Citation2005; Kramer Citation1990; Schapkin et al. Citation2007; de Waard Citation1996; Weiner Citation1982). In the case of short-term tasks requiring sophisticated psychological (executive) operations relying on working memory or selective attention, however, the general cardiovascular pattern involves an increase in HR and blood pressure (increased blood pressure resulting in its reduced variability), a decrease in HRV (Kramer Citation1990; Mehler, Reimer, and Wang Citation2011; Miyake et al. Citation2007; Veltman and Gaillard Citation1998; de Waard Citation1996; Wilson, Fullenkamp, and Davis Citation1994), and a decrease in baroreflex sensitivity (Mulder et al. Citation2000). Although a loss in power occurs in all frequency bands (Brookhuis, et al. Citation2005), the loss in the mid-frequency band (0.10 Hz) seems to be most often demonstrated when there is an increased load of “resource-limited” (Kahneman Citation1973) cognitive processes (Kramer Citation1990; Mulder et al. Citation2000; Mulder et al. Citation2005; de Waard Citation1996). It also occurs when there is a constant mild physical load (de Waard Citation1996), which is the subject of our research. During physical activity HR increases and HRV decreases; nevertheless, intentionally slowing down HR results in ambiguous HRV behavior (Billman Citation2013; Billman et al. Citation2015). Apart from ANS, HR and HRV can be considerably influenced by breathing (Billman Citation2013; Ganong Citation2005; Kramer Citation1990; Mulder et al. Citation2000; Porges Citation1995; de Waard Citation1996) as well as by other mechanical effects leading to atrium compression and thoracic pressure change (Ganong Citation2005; Rokyta Citation2000; Van Roon et al. Citation2004), and by increased mental effort (increased cognitive activity). HRV decreases with increasing RR and increases with increasing VT. These phenomena have also been experimentally demonstrated to act in the opposite manner: HRV decreases with decreasing VT and increases with decreasing RR.

Body temperature values and their effects on the human body differ considerably (Boucsein Citation2012a; Ekman, Levenson, and Friesen Citation1983; Jirák et al. Citation1997; Kreibig et al. Citation2007; Pigeau et al. Citation1995; Švancara Citation2003; Takeyama et al. Citation2005; Vinkers et al. Citation2010; Weiner Citation1982; Wever Citation1979). It seems that mental-processing rates (including processing visual cues and other forms of cognitive processing causing mental load; see Calvin and Duffy Citation2007) depend on the degree of metabolic activity (or the arousal) of the brain’s cortex cells (Tůma Citation1989). Increased metabolic activity is related to increased core temperature (e.g., rectal, tympanic, etc.; see Folkard Citation1990; García-García and Drucker-Colín Citation1999; Kleitman Citation1963; Whang et al. Citation2007), while finger-skin-surface temperatures (i.e. peripheral body temperatures) generally decrease in such cases (Fröberg Citation1977; Vinkers et al. Citation2010).

We expect our research to confirm the inverted U relationship between activation and cognitive performance. Specifically, that the combination of a medium physical load (arousal) with a cognitive load will lead to higher cognitive performance (Levitt and Gutin Citation1971; Sjöberg Citation1975; Basahel Citation2012), and to higher values of physiological functions (Antunes et al. Citation2006; Fredericks et al. Citation2005) than when there is a cognitive load with a low physical load (arousal). Load intensity was determined by whether the examined person (EP) was sitting directly on a chair (hereinafter “chair”) or on a chair with a pad. Sitting on a chair can be considered a low physical load, while sitting on the pad can be considered a medium physical load (Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017; Wientjes Citation1993).

We shall further focus our attention on the course of cognitive performance. It is known that with a new task, performance improves at the beginning through conscious training and becomes stable at its optimum (the negative acceleration curve, Sternberg and Sternberg Citation2012). Nevertheless, it fluctuates according to the degree of automation (habituation) or according to physiologically (biologically or physically) given oscillations of attention (Senka, Kuruc, and Čečer Citation1992; Chmelař and Osecký Citation1974).

We also have to reckon with the assumption that females will have perceptual sensitivity lower than males in the visual test for sustained attention (Dittmar et al. Citation1993; Prinzel and Freeman Citation1997), which may reflect in greater changes of their cognitive functions (Kakizaki Citation1987).

Materials and methods

Research design and research sample

All measurements were carried out in an air-conditioned laboratory at a constant temperature of 23˚C (Boucsein Citation2012a; Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007; Wever Citation1979), a humidity of 40–50%, a noise level lower than 45 dB (as measured by a calibrated noise meter), and illuminance of 1500–4000 lx (lux meter) in order to prevent possible influence on physiological functions (Kramer Citation1990; Wever Citation1979). Measurements were taken from October 2016 to January 2017. Altogether 88 EPs (29 female and 59 male aged 19–27) took part in our study. We tried to assemble a relatively homogeneous sample of healthy EPs without baseline differences between our study groups. The EPs were second-year forestry students with the same academic specialization. They were randomly divided by lot into two groups, a chair and a pad group, with an equal ratio of male and female EPs in each group. All subjects were strictly forbidden to drink alcohol for a minimum of two days before the measurements were taken (Mulder and Mulder Citation1987). Measurements began between 08:30 and 08:45 to ensure similar fatigue rates among EPs (for the effects of biorhythms and cognitive loads see Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007; Wever Citation1979). None of the EPs had experienced sitting on the pad before. Short-term illnesses and long-term health problems (allergies, anemia, asthma, diabetes, etc.) were noted (Rokyta Citation2000; Billman Citation2013). EPs in the control group were seated on wooden, height-adjustable non-upholstered chairs with an unshaped seat so that they sat with their knees bent at a right angle. EPs seated on pads were forbidden to lean against the backrests of their chairs. Researchers monitored the movement of EPs on the chairs, which was supposed to be as limited as possible (Mulder Citation1992). The EPs were not informed of the duration or purpose of the experiment. They were forbidden to talk during the measurements because talking can considerably affect HR, HRV, and R behavior (Mulder and Mulder Citation1987; Mulder Citation1992; Wientjes Citation1993).

The measurement process involved the following steps:

  1. EPs were seated on wooden chairs and received oral instructions about the sequence of individual measurement phases, including an explanation of the test of sustained, selected attention (Eysenck and Keane Citation2000; Bates and Lemay Citation2004; Brickenkamp and Zillmer Citation2000; Gabrhel Citation2014; Chmelař Citation1970a; Chmelař and Osecký Citation1970b, Citation1974; Mulder et al. Citation2000; Sternberg and Sternberg Citation2012). We used a version of the Bourdon test referred to as T-78, or BoPr (hereinafter, the Bo test), a type of cancelation test (Senka, Kuruc, and Čečer Citation1992). Test performance is evaluated by counting correctly cancelled symbols (BoC) and incorrectly cancelled symbols (errors; BoE). Practice was not permitted because it could influence the performance of EPs differently.

The test can ascertain the accuracy and rate of scanning, as well as the learning strategy and its development (Bates and Lemay Citation2004). Because of its duration and complexity, the capacity for shifting and holding attention (elements of permanent attention) and the capability to focus on selected stimuli (elements of selective attention) can be detected and discriminated between (Senka, Kuruc, and Čečer Citation1992). These “prototypical” capabilities (Mulder et al. Citation2000) are often assessed using this test (or by using a similar test named d2), e.g. in professional drivers (Gabrhel Citation2014), but also in testing performance in sport or in schools (Brickenkamp and Zillmer Citation2000). The Bo test was standardized on a sample of 1020 persons of different professions aged 17 − 36 years (Senka, Kuruc, and Čečer Citation1992). Results exhibited a normal distribution. Internal consistency (split-half) in BoE was r = 0.787 and in BoC: r = 0.989. Other validity and reliability indicators can be found in Senka, Kuruc, and Čečer (Citation1992).

  1. The control phase of the measurement lasted 2 minutes. In this phase, all EPs sat on chairs with no pads. Its purpose was to analyze the initial values of the EPs’ physiological functions.

  2. The following resting phase of the measurement lasted 20 minutes. Control-group EPs continued sitting on chairs without pads, while EPs in the other group were seated on pads that had been added to their chairs. They would then sit on these pads until the end of the fourth phase. During the second phase, EPs were not to show any deliberate cognitive activity. Activities involving inadvertent attention, however, were allowed, for example, looking forward at the table or out of the window.

  3. The load phase lasted 20 minutes, during which the EPs continuously filled out the Bo test.

  4. The verification phase lasted 2 minutes. Following the end of the cognitive load, this phase also served to compare the development of physiological functions in the two groups of EPs.

A graphical visual illustration of our experiment setup is provided in .

Figure 1. Graphical illustration of experiment setup.

Figure 1. Graphical illustration of experiment setup.

Acquisition of biosignals

All four biosignals were measured simultaneously at the same sampling frequency of 1000 Hz and the same quantifying resolution of 24 bits. The biosignals were recorded using a Biopac MP35 (Biopac, Inc.) acquisition unit. All data were visually inspected once again, normal values were evaluated (Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007), and deviations from standard oscillations (Göbel, et al. Citation2005) were examined.

Inspiration maxima (VT) were detected automatically by the PhysioCrate toolbox plug-in (Nejedly and Virgala Citation2016) and by SignalPlant software (Plesinger et al. Citation2016), and visually inspected at every step. Inspiration depth was measured as a relative chest volume change (VTR) with units in mV. To explore differences in mean VT values related to the use of the pad, it was necessary to transform the data. Absolute chest volume change (VTA) was subsequently calculated and expressed in liters from relative chest volume change (VTR), see Wientjes and Grossman Citation2005. We based this on the value of one inspiration in both males and females, which amounts to 0.5 l (Ganong Citation2005; Rokyta Citation2000) and on the minute ventilation (MV) at rest of healthy females aged 18–25 years, which is 10.5 liters. To ensure accuracy, we selected data from two time ranges during the control phase of the experiment (Bland and Altman Citation2010). The EDA curve carries information on skin conductance level (SCL) and skin conductance reflex (SCR). For the purposes of assessing the activity of sympathetic nerves, we used only skin conductance level (SCL) as a robust indicator of the sympathetic activity rate. We evaluated HR (1/1000 seconds), which was acquired from an electrocardiography (ECG) as the reverse value of the difference between consequent R-peaks detected by the QRS seeker program (Vítek and Kozumplík Citation2011). HRV parameters were analyzed on an R-R interval series (Barbieri et al. Citation2005). The heart rate variability analysis software (HRVAS) plug-in for Matlab software was used to compute HRV parameters (Ramshur Citation2010; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Citation1996).

Time domain parameters were evaluated, namely the mean of R-R interval (R-R mean) and its reciprocal value mean of heart rate (HR mean), standard deviation of R-R intervals (SDNN), root mean square of the successive differences (RMSSD), the number of successive intervals differing more than 50 ms (NN50) and its corresponding relative amount (pNN50). Geometric domain parameters included HRV triangular index (TI) and the baseline width of the R-R histogram evaluated through triangular interpolation (TINN). Frequency domain parameters were evaluated utilizing three different methods for spectra estimation: Fourier transform, Welch periodogram with window width of 256 samples and window overlap of 128 samples, and Burg autoregressive model with 16th order. All periodograms were divided into three standard bands: VLF 0 − 0.04 Hz, LF 0.04 − 0.15 Hz, HF 0.15 − 0.4 Hz. Frequency domain parameters were evaluated for results of each method: VLF, LF and HF band peak frequencies (VLF peak, LF peak, HF peak), absolute powers of VLF, LF, and HF bands (aVLF, aLF, aHF), relative power of VLF, LF, and HF bands (pVLF, pLF, pHF), powers of LF and HF bands in normalized units (nLF, nHF) and ratio between LF and HF band powers (LFHF). Non-linear HRV parameters assessed in this work included sample entropy (HRV Entropy), short term fluctuation slope (alpha 1) and long term fluctuation slope (alpha 2) as a result of detrended fluctuation analysis with a range from 4 to 64 samples and break point equal to 11, and Poincaré diagram descriptors SD1 and SD2 representing the standard deviation of diagram perpendicular to (SD1) and along (SD2) the line-of-identity (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Citation1996).

Standards for the evaluation of HRV unify the length of the assessed tachograph section to 5 minutes, so that both short and long-term HR changes can be optimally captured for a correct quantification of which a time section of at least 2 minutes is needed (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Citation1996).

Results

Along with the description we are presenting an analysis of the dependent variable number of BoE in relation to the independent variables of gender and pad/chair use (ANOVA). We will also present the analysis of the dependent variables, namely physiological functions of females, in relation to the independent variable – pad/chair (t-test, the Mann-Whitney test). Finally, we will provide a description and statistical analysis of the contribution of physiological functions (predictors) to differences in average BoC and BoE values (dependent variables) in females seated on the pad or chair (standardized coefficients Beta and coefficient of multiple determination R2).

Cognitive functions

Firstly, mean values for BoC and BoE scores were calculated for each minute of the test separately. Then a total mean BoC and BoE score was calculated from the twenty partial minute average values.

In the set of males, no significant differences were found in connection with the use of the pad (see ). As for the mean scores, differences were found only in the set of females and only in their BoE scores (). The number of BoE made by females sitting on the pad was statistically significantly lower than the number of BoE made by females sitting directly on the office chair (). The difference was approximately two errors, which was (with the mean number of errors being 1.77/min) a 58% difference in the number of errors in connection with the use of the pad. This overall contrast in BoE scores is primarily the result of significant differences between the beginning of the test and its last third (see ). The findings are exacerbated by the fact that higher (non-significant) BoE scores in females seated on the pads were found only in one minute (specifically in the sixth minute) of the 20 minutes measured. The power of independent samples in the t-test equals 0.79 (SD = 2.81; N = 15, 13; Es = 1.09). In the physiological functions, erroneous records of 5 EPs (1 female and 1 male seated on the chair, and 3 females seated on the pad) and 1 unfinished Bo test from a female seated on the chair were discarded. Thus, to compare the Bo test performance with the development of physiological functions, we worked with a sample of 24 females (chair = 12, pad = 12) and 58 males (chair = 28, pad = 30). Besides the subgroup of females seated on the pads (Kolmogorov-Smirnov = p < 0.008; Shapiro-Wilk = p < 0.001), the acceptable normality of distribution in subgroups was achieved by dividing gender by chair/pad (males on the chairs: Kolmogorov-Smirnov = p < 0.200; Shapiro-Wilk = p < 0.284; males on the pads: Kolmogorov-Smirnov = p < 0.114; Shapiro-Wilk = p < 0.033; females on the chairs: Kolmogorov-Smirnov = p < 0.200; Shapiro-Wilk = p < 0.980). The group of females on the pads exhibited right-skewed BoE data due to a relatively low median value. Due to this and the relatively small sample of females, both parametric and non-parametric statistical methods were used.

Figure 2. The number of BoE over the course of 20 minutes in males (A) and females (B) seated on chairs/pads (means, error bars of SD, simple moving average - SMA).

Figure 2. The number of BoE over the course of 20 minutes in males (A) and females (B) seated on chairs/pads (means, error bars of SD, simple moving average - SMA).

Table 1. Descriptive data on the number of BoE in all EPs and separately in males and females without outliers.

Table 2. Significant differences in the number of BoE in females (without outliers) ascertained by t-test for independent samples, the Mann-Whitney test for two independent samples and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test.

The significant BoE differences among females were identified by both the t-test for independent samples and its nonparametric analogues, the Mann-Whitney test for two independent samples and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test.

Based on a univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA), we further explored differences in average BoE values in all EPs, in connection with both use of the pad and gender. Their potential interactions were also investigated. shows the main effect of the pad, with the effect of differences between males and females being significant only in the interaction with the pad (in the number of errors, the gender effect was significant only in the fourth minute of the Bo test; T-test: P < 0.038; Mann-Whitney: P < 0.045). To sum up, BoE numbers were affected most strongly by the mutual interaction of the two factors; the more important role was undoubtedly played by the factor of the pad.

Table 3. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) of the number of BoE in relation to gender and pad use.

Differences among the mean values of physiological functions in relation to the pad

Dissimilarities were found in VTR and RR and their derivatives VTA and MV, and also in FT, SCL, HR and HRV “sample entropy” (hereinafter HRV Entropy), which was calculated from the series of R-R intervals. A description of all physiological functions is presented in .

Table 4. Descriptive data on RR, VTR, VTA, MV, FT, SCL, HR and HRV Entropy of females. The table presents descriptive data on selected physiological functions capturing differences between females seated on chairs/pads during the load and rest phase.

We have presented the results in , which were produced using categorized data, that is, from the averaged values of physiological functions for each minute of the 20-minute measurements. There are many reasons why we approached the criterion of significance with caution. One of the major ones is that means do not capture dynamic changes (oscillations) of functions in the course of measurements, which considerably impairs their informative value, especially when the function oscillations are distinctive and frequent. Apart from this criterion, we also assessed the individual findings according to the practical value of differences. For example, if a difference of 0.6 °C (between the examined groups) is actually important for real life situations or not.

Table 5. Significant differences (Sig. 2-tailed) between the physiological characteristics of females (without outliers) seated on the chairs/pads ascertained by means of t-test for independent samples and power calculation (power, standardized effect – Es) for N = 12, 12 and during the load (3) and rest (2) phase ascertained by means of paired samples t-test and its nonparametric analogues, the Wilcoxon signed ranks test for paired samples.

No significant differences were found in RR function in connection with sitting on the pad in the analysis using the Mann-Whitney test and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, which demonstrate its quite parametric distribution.

Differences in the development of physiological functions and cognitive performance in connection with the use of the pad

shows at least two basic trends in the oscillations (slight and conspicuous) and behaviors (ascending and descending tendencies) of the variables examined. Another assessment criterion could certainly be the degree of influence of “breaking points,” that is, the minutes in which distinctive changes occurred in the variables. Such breaking points are minutes 26 and 27, minutes 33 and 34, and to a lesser extent minutes 23 and 24, and minute 39. VTR, FT, SCL, and also BoE and HRV Entropy exhibited rather small oscillations with either ascending or descending tendencies, hinting at the possibility of introducing a linear function despite the partial influence of breaking points. The physiological functions mentioned increased in EPs sitting on pads during the test, while the number of BoE was continually decreasing. In contrast, the variables of MV and BoC exhibited generally strongly variable “behavior” that was more affected by the “breaking points.” Here too we observed increasing BoC performance in connection with sitting on the pad, although a noticeable slump occurred between minutes 27 and 33. The conspicuous inverted character of the behavior of both MV and BoC variables, particularly apparent in females seated only on the chairs, was also important. It must also not be neglected that the average values of the physiological functions of females were always higher when sitting on the pad during the whole measurement period (20 minutes) of the load phase than the values recorded when sitting just on the chair.

Figure 3. (C – H). Comparison of selected physiological functions with BoC (BoE; see ) in the two groups of females (means, error bars of SD, SMA). provides valuable data about frequent instances of the completely inverted course of the data mentioned (e.g., between MV and BoC).

Figure 3. (C – H). Comparison of selected physiological functions with BoC (BoE; see Figure 2) in the two groups of females (means, error bars of SD, SMA). Figure 3 provides valuable data about frequent instances of the completely inverted course of the data mentioned (e.g., between MV and BoC).

A thorough investigation of the contribution of physiological functions to differences in average BoC and BoE values (standardized coefficients Beta) in females and the extent to which their share can be explained by means of these functions (coefficient of multiple determination R2) will be discussed in another paper. Current results show that the number of BoE was related most closely to VTR in both female groups. EPs seated on the chairs: R = 0.36; R2 = 0.13; ANOVA: F = 35.96; P < 0.001; stand. Beta = −0.36; t = −6.00; P < 0.001. EPs seated on the pads: R = 0.31; R2 = 0.09; ANOVA: F = 24.81; P < 0.001; stand. Beta = −0.31; t = −4.98; P < 0.001.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to search for differences in cognitive performance related to various intensities of physical load. Sitting on the chair can be considered a low physical load, while sitting on the pad can be considered a medium physical load (Levitt and Gutin Citation1971; Sjöberg Citation1975; Basahel Citation2012; Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017; Wientjes Citation1993).

Differences in cognitive performance were observed only in females. The assumption that females respond more sensitively to cognitive load than males appears to be valid (Kakizaki Citation1987). The explanation may lie in the lower perceptual sensitivity (Dittmar et al. Citation1993; Prinzel and Freeman Citation1997) which appears in females as compared with males in the tests of sustained attention to visual stimuli. It should be added that the EPs measured had no previous experience with the pad whereas the effects on muscle strength and posture development are at the strongest only after 3 months of everyday use of the pad (Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017). The question is, therefore, whether the results would not also be the same in males after that time, because of dissimilarly shaped pelvic floors, dissimilar attachment of muscles in this region, or dissimilar physical strength and amount of striated muscles present in males and females (Åstrand et al. Citation2003; Šedivý Citation2004; Šedivý Citation2006).

Females sitting on the pad made a significantly lower number of errors than those sitting on the chair. The basic assumption was therefore confirmed. When a medium physical load (20%–50% Wmax, cycling on a stationary bicycle fitted with an ergometer) and cognitive load are combined, cognitive performance increases as do the values of physiological functions (Basahel Citation2012; Antunes et al. Citation2006; Fredericks et al. Citation2005;) compared with when a low physical load (chair) and cognitive load are combined (Levitt and Gutin Citation1971; Sjöberg Citation1975). The findings can be interpreted by using the known function of inverted U in line with the Yerkes-Dodson law (Colquhoun Citation1971; Folkard Citation1975; Folkard Citation1990; Kahneman Citation1973). Nonetheless, Salmela and Ndoye (Citation1986) or Basahel (Citation2012) did not tackle the question of whether the number of errors is another criterion of cognitive performance besides the speed of work.

Surveys of Bo test validity indicate that errors are a measure of performance, which differs from the rate of processing that manifests itself in the number of correct answers (Břicháček, Brichcín, and Malotínová Citation1969; Senka and Daniel Citation1970). According to the factor analysis, BoC was part of the factor of “rate of operations” (Břicháček, Brichcín, and Malotínová Citation1969) or factors of perception, attention, imagination, quick-wittedness and memory (Senka and Daniel Citation1970). BoE then fell either in the category of “erroneous operations” (Břicháček, Brichcín, and Malotínová Citation1969), or – as claimed by Senka and Daniel (Citation1970) – BoE created a special factor which is common with the errors of short-term numeric memory.

Our Bo test is similar to the well known d2 test of attention. According to Brickenkamp and Zillmer (Citation2000), the rate of performance in the d2 test (in our case, BoC) reflects – in addition to the accuracy of visual perception – the degree of arousal (motivation side), while the number of errors (BoE) reflects the degree of self-control by means of conscious attention (Eysenck and Keane Citation2000; Gabrhel Citation2014; Chmelař Citation1970a; Chmelař and Osecký Citation1970b, Citation1974; Mulder et al. Citation2000; Sternberg and Sternberg Citation2012).

Bates and Lemay (Citation2004) have further developed these findings, confirming the rather low correlation between the number of correct answers and the number of errors in the d2 test (Wientjes (Citation1993) also arrived at similar results). It follows that the two outputs, search speed and accuracy, are in a way two independent cognitive dimensions. These authors have developed two concepts to evaluate these two independent factors: selective scanning accuracy, which is saturated by varying scores in the number of errors, and selective scanning deterioration or acceleration, which is saturated by scores in the distribution of the number of correct answers or errors in time during the test. The authors explain two possible mechanisms behind errors. Mistakes may be the result of incorrect motor reactions which may manifest themselves during a short-term drop in vigilance (in accordance with Brickenkamp and Zillmer Citation2000), or, from the viewpoint of the theory of signal detection, a result in a drop in the tendency to subordinate to the strict criterion of cognitive (mental) performance, which leads to interference with the implementation of the established goal.

In the following part of the discussion, we shall try to detect causes of the changes in cognitive and physiological functions according to literature results focused particularly on cognitive load (Boucsein Citation2012a). Compared with the results of research focused on physical load, these allow for the exploration of the effect of ANS on the course of cognitive and physiological functions directly (Dawson, Schell, and Filion Citation2007; de Waard Citation1996; Boucsein Citation2005) and offer a more precise explanation of their partial relationships (Antunes et al. Citation2006). Unfortunately, similar research for a rigorous comparison with our findings is lacking.

Supposing that the state of the EPs’ attention was continually monitored, the degree of the BoE can be derived from the degree of ANS activity. Since the degree of cognitive load was constant and hence the cognitive load can be categorized as mental effort devoted to the processing of information in the controlled mode (Mulder and Mulder Citation1987), it seems that changes in the output efficiency of EPs (number of errors) were actually related to the level of vigilance given by physiological arousal (Boucsein, Haarmann, and Schaefer Citation2007). According to the results of linear regression, we see a key function for maintaining vigilance (lucidity) in VTR (VTA) values. These are also reflected in RR values and in MV level. In females seated on the chairs, VTR decreased with increasing RR and HR, and the number of BoC increased with increased HR. Thus, we confirmed the frequently quoted relationship between physiological-function patterns and cognitive load (Kramer Citation1990; Mehler, Reimer, and Wang Citation2011; Miyake et al. Citation2007; Veltman and Gaillard Citation1998; de Waard Citation1996; Wilson, Fullenkamp, and Davis Citation1994). In this group of females, RR was so high during the load phase that the values of gross MV (l/min) increased despite the decreasing VTR values. Wientjes (Citation1993) reported the same findings. Faster RR and shallower VT do not seem to provide the R pattern most appropriate for optimal cognitive activity. These findings were not observed in females seated on the pads. Their RR was not fast enough to contribute significantly to the resulting MV value. When RR eventually accelerated sporadically, it was reflected only in shallow breathing (VTA). Wientjes (Citation1993) also found that under light to moderate physical load, higher R (MV) is primarily induced by increased VT. Deeper breathing can save more energy for cognitive performance than faster breathing because a further increase of RR apparently becomes inefficient due to the excessive use of respiratory muscles. Davies, Haldane, and Priestley (Citation1919) explain this phenomenon as “secondary anoxemia” that arises during the “ordinary fatigue” of the respiratory center, which cannot even be suppressed by artificially adding O2 into the inspired air (Davies, Haldane, and Priestley Citation1919). It is possible that increased muscular activity (deep stabilization of muscles of the back, abdomen, and buttocks; see Holinka, Gallo, and Zapletalová Citation2016, Holinka et al. Citation2017) impairs natural inspiration. In such cases, VT becomes deeper and RR slower (a decrease from approximately 14 to 7 inspirations per minute). Moreover, compared with the other group of EPs, females seated on the pads exhibited lower VTR values in the initial section of the load phase, which points to the same findings of Davies, Haldane, and Priestley (Citation1919), when exploring the initial phase of respiration under conditions of increased resistance.

We further discovered that in females seated on the chairs, SCL increased with decreasing FT. This phenomenon was documented several times by Boucsein (Citation2012a). By contrast, in females seated on the pads, SCL increased with increasing FT. It seems that due to physical load the dilatation of blood vessels and an increase in the number of open capillaries (Ganong Citation2005) are so prevalent in active muscles, and consequently in the entire body of females seated on the pads, that physical load trumps the effects of mental load. Thus, FT values determined in our study could also be the result of the ratio of vasoconstriction (mental load) and vasodilatation (physical load) of blood vessels (Boucsein Citation2012a).

The dominant influence of physical load over mental load in the behavior of physiological functions was also corroborated by Basahel (Citation2012). He observed increased HRV values when the two types of load were combined in his research, despite the fact that HRV decreases if mental load alone increases (Kramer Citation1990; Mehler, Reimer, and Wang Citation2011; Miyake et al. Citation2007; Veltman and Gaillard Citation1998; de Waard Citation1996; Wilson, Fullenkamp, and Davis Citation1994). In females seated on the pads HRV Entropy increased with rising SCL. This phenomenon could be the result of respiration patterns (Veltman and Gaillard Citation1998) because HRV (in our case HRV Entropy) increases with rising VT. We do not dare present an unambiguous interpretation of this partial HRV indicator. HR and HRV can be distinctly influenced by breathing (Billman Citation2013), which is more than likely in our pilot research study.

Conclusion

We investigated differences in cognitive performance connected with physical loads of varying intensities. A combination of mild physical load (the use of the modified gymnastic – Swiss ball) and cognitive load leads to the increased precision of cognitive performance. Females exhibited greater changes in their cognitive functions than males. Females seated on pads (mild physical load) made 58% fewer errors than females on chairs; the number of errors was closely related to the depth of their breathing (tidal volume). The number of correct answers remained unchanged. Females on the pads also showed higher skin conductance levels and HRV Entropy values.

While breathing depth, skin conductance level, finger temperature, HRV Entropy, and the number of errors exhibited no great oscillations, variance in respiration frequency (minute ventilation), heart rate, and the number of correct answers of females was considerable in the load phase.

The fact that physical load dominates over mental load in the course of physiological functions particularizes the patterns for how one group of functions facilitates or inhibits the other one.

The finding that the number of errors is another criterion of sustained, selected attention besides the speed of work, can be applied for example in the area of traffic safety (air and road etc.).

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study, filed as Project no. EK 04/2018, was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Biomedical Engineering of the Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Communication at Brno University of Technology. Informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Availability of data and material

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available in the [https://www.mendeley.com] repository, [doi: 10.17632/7djsbz6prv.1].

ANS=

autonomic nervous system

Bo test=

Bourdon test T-78

BoC=

correctly cancelled symbols of Bo test

BoE=

incorrectly cancelled symbols (errors) of Bo test

FT=

finger temperature

ECG=

electrocardiography

EDA=

electrodermal activity

HRV Entropy=

sample entropy of HRV

EP=

examined person

Es=

standardized effect

HR=

heart rate

HRV=

heart rate variability

HRVAS=

heart rate variability analysis software

MV=

minute ventilation

pad=

dynamic directional seat pad

R=

respiration

RR=

respiratory rate

SCL=

skin conductance level

SCR=

skin conductance reflex

SMA=

simple moving average

VT=

tidal volume

VTA=

absolute tidal volume change

VTR=

relative tidal volume change

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Antunes, H. K. M., R. F. Santos, R. Cassilhas, R. V. T. Santos, O. F. A. Bueno, and M. T. de Mello. 2006. “Reviewing on Physical Exercise and the Cognitive Function.” Revista Brasileira de Medicina do Esporte 12 (2): 97–103.
  • Åstrand, P. O., K. Rodahl, H. A. Dahl, and S. B. Strømme. 2003. Textbook of Work Physiology: Physiological Bases of Exercise. 4th ed, pp. 656. Champaign: Human Kinetics. ISBN-13: 978–0736001403.
  • Balakrishnan, R., E. Yazid, and M. F. B. Mahat. 2016. “Effectiveness of the Core Stabilisation Exercise on Floor and Swiss Ball on Individual with non-Specific Low Back Pain.” International Journal of Physical Education, Sports and Health 3: 347–356.
  • Barbieri, R., E. C. Matten, A. R. A. Alabi, and E. N. Brown. 2005. “A Point-Process Model of Human Heartbeat Intervals: new Definitions of Heart Rate and Heart Rate Variability.” American Journal of Physiology. Heart and Circulatory Physiology 288 (1): H424–H435. doi:https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpheart.00482.2003.
  • Basahel, A. 2012. “Effect of Physical and Mental Workload Interactions on Human Attentional Resources and Performance.” Dissertation., Brunel University. https://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/6614
  • Bates, M. E., and E. P. Lemay. 2004. “The d2 Test of Attention: construct Validity and Extensions in Scoring Techniques.” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS 10 (3): 392–400. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561770410307X.
  • Billman, G. E. 2013. “The LF/HF Ratio Does Not Accurately Measure Cardiac Sympatho-Vagal Balance.” Frontiers in Physiology 4 (26): 26–25. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2013.00026.
  • Billman, G. E., H. V. Huikuri, J. Sacha, and K. Trimmel. 2015. “An Introduction to Heart Rate Variability: methodological Considerations and Clinical Applications.” Frontiers in Physiology 6 (55): 55–53. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2015.00055.
  • Bland, J. M., and D. G. Altman. 2010. “Statistical Methods for Assessing Agreement between Two Methods of Clinical Measurement.” International Journal of Nursing Studies 47 (8): 931–936. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.10.001.
  • Boucsein, W. 2005. “Electrodermal Measurement.” In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics Methods, edited by N. Stanton, et al., pp. 18–1–18-8. Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington, DC: CRC Press. ISBN 0-415-28700-6
  • Boucsein, W. 2012a. Electrodermal Activity. 2nd ed, pp. 618. New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London: Springer. ISBN 978-1-4614-1125-3
  • Boucsein, W., D. C. Fowles, S. Grimnes, G. Ben-Shakhar, W. T. Roth, M. E. Dawson, D. L. Filion, Society for Psychophysiological Research Ad Hoc Committee on Electrodermal Measures, et al. 2012b. “Publication Recommendations for Electrodermal Measurements.” Psychophysiology 49 (8): 1017–1034. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x.
  • Boucsein, W., A. Haarmann, and F. Schaefer. 2007. “Combining Skin Conductance and Heart Rate Variability for Adaptive Automation during Simulated IFR Flight.” In Engin. Psychol. and Cog. Ergonomics, HCII 2007, LNAI 4562, edited by D. Harris, 639–647. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. ISBN-13 978-3-540-73330-0
  • Boyer, M., M. L. Cummings, L. B. Spence, and E. T. Solovey. 2015. “Investigating Mental Workload Changes in a Long Duration Supervisory Control Task.” Interacting with Computers 27 (5): 512–520. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/iwc/iwv012.
  • Břicháček, V., M. Brichcín, and M. Malotínová. 1969. “Rozbor Vztahů Mezi Parametry Vybraných Výkonových Zkoušek.” Čsl. psychologie, XIII 4: 357–366.
  • Brickenkamp, R., and E. Zillmer. 2000. Test Pozornosti d2. Testcentrum. Praha: Hogrefe Verlag Gottingen. ISBN 80-86471–00-4.
  • Brookhuis, K. A., et al. 2005. “Psychophysiological Methods.” In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics Methods, edited by N. Stanton, pp. 17–1–17-5. Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington, DC: CRC Press. ISBN 0-415-28700-6
  • Calvin, K. L., and V. G. Duffy. 2007. “Development of a Facial Skin Temperature-Based Methodology for Non-Intrusive Mental Workload Measurement.” Occupational Ergonomics 7 (2): 83–94.
  • Carriere, B. 1998. “The Swiss Ball.” Theory, Basic Exercises and Clinical Application. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer-Verlag. ISBN 3-540-61144-4
  • Chmelař, V. 1970a. “Method of Experimental Research into the Continuous Active Optical and Acoustical Attention and Its Duration Periods.” Čs. psychologie, XIV 4: 360–365.
  • Chmelař, V., and P. Osecký. 1970b. “The Course of Duration Periods of Active Visual and Acoustic Attention.” Psychologie v Ekonomické Praxi 3 (70): 132–143.
  • Chmelař, V., and P. Osecký. 1974. “Mathematical Models of the Active Attention Course.” Sborník Prací Filosofické Fakulty Brněnské University (Studia Minora Facultatis Philosophicae Universitatis Brunensis) I9: 17–51.
  • Coles, K., and P. D. Tomporowski. 2008. “Effects of Acute Exercise on Executive Processing, Short-Term and Long-Term Memory.” Journal of Sports Sciences 26 (3): 333–344. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410701591417.
  • Colquhoun, W. P. (ed.). 1971. Biological Rhythms and Human Performance, pp. 283. London and New York: Academic Press. ISBN: 0-12-182050-5
  • Cosio-Lima, L. M., K. L. Reynolds, Ch Winter, V. Paolone, and M. T. Jones. 2003. “Effects of Physioball and Conventional Floor Exercises on Early Phase Adaptations in Back and Abdominal Core Stability and Balance in Woman.” Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research 17 (4): 721–725.
  • Courtney, R. 2009. “The Functions of Breathing and Its Dysfunctions and Their Relationship to Breathing Therapy.” International Journal of Osteopathic Medicine 12 (3): 78–85. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijosm.2009.04.002.
  • Davies, H. W., J. S. Haldane, and J. G. Priestley. 1919. “The Response to Respiratory Resistance.” The Journal of Physiology 53 (1–2): 60–69. doi:https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1919.sp001859.
  • Dawson, M. E., A. M. Schell, and D. L. Filion. 2007. “The Electrodermal System.” In Handbook of Psychophysiology. 3rd ed, edited by J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, G. G. Berntson, pp. 159–181. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • de Waard, D. 1996. The Measurement of Drivers’ Mental Workload. Haren, The Netherlands: Traffic Research Centre VSC, University of Groningen.
  • Dittmar, M., L. J. S. Warm, W. N. Dember, and D. F. Ricks. 1993. “Sex Differences in Vigilance Performance and Perceived Workload.” The Journal of General Psychology 120 (3): 309–322. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1993.9711150.
  • Ekman, P., R. W. Levenson, and W. V. Friesen. 1983. “Autonomic Nervous System Activity Distinguishes among Emotions.” Science (New York, NY) 221 (4616): 1208–1210. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338.
  • Eysenck, M. W., and M. T. Keane. 2000. “Cognitive Psychology.” A Student’s Handbook, pp. 690. Hove and New York: Psychology Press (Taylor & Francis Group). ISBN 0-203-62630-3
  • Filo, P. 2014. “Comparison of the Ergonomic Characteristics of Forwarders with Stationary and Rotating Cabins.” Journal of Agricultural Science and Technology B 4: 476–484.
  • Folkard, S. 1975. “Diurnal Variation in Logical Reasoning.” British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953) 66 (1): 1–8. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01433.x.
  • Folkard, S. 1990. “Circadian Performance Rhythms: some Practical and Theoretical Implications.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B 327: 543–553.
  • Fredericks, T. K., S. D. Choi, J. Hart, S. E. Butt, and A. Mital. 2005. “An Investigation of Myocardial Aerobic Capacity as a Measure of Both Physical and Cognitive Workloads.” International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 35 (12): 1097–1107. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2005.06.002.
  • Fröberg, J. E. 1977. “Twenty-Four-Hour Patterns in Human Performance, Subjective and Physiological Variables and Differences between Morning and Evening Active Subjects.” Biological Psychology 5 (2): 119–134. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(77)90008-4.
  • Gabrhel, V. 2014. “Recenze Testu Pozornosti d2.” TESTFÓRUM 3 (4): 31–36. doi:https://doi.org/10.5817/TF2014-4-26.
  • Ganong, W. F. 2005. Přehled Lékařské Fyziologie. 20th ed, pp. 890. Praha: Galén. ISBN 80-7262–311-7.
  • García-García, F., and R. Drucker-Colín. 1999. “Endogenous and Exogenous Factors on Sleep-Wake Cycle Regulation.” Progress in Neurobiology 58 (4): 297–314. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00086-0.
  • Göbel, M., et al. 2005. Electromyography (EMG)”, in Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics Methods, edited by N. Stanton, pp. 19–1–19-8. Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington, DC: CRC Press. ISBN 0-415-28700-6
  • Gutin, B. 1973. “Exercise-Induced Activation and Human Performance: A Review.” Research Quarterly. American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation 44 (3): 256–268. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10671188.1973.10615204.
  • Holinka, M., J. Gallo, I. Tozzi, M. Zvonař, M. Filip, J. Kristiníková, and R. Pavličný. 2017. “Porovnání Vybraných Metod k Posílení Stabilizačních Svalů Bederní Páteře u Vertebrogenních Pacientů.” Rehabil. Fyz. Lék 24: 84–98.
  • Holinka, M., J. Gallo, and J. Zapletalová. 2016. “Sonografické Posouzení Stabilizačních Svalů Bederní Páteře u Vertebrogenních Pacientů.” Rehabil. Fyz. Lék 23 (2): 64–73.
  • Jirák, Z., M. V. Jokl, H. Jiráková, and P. Bajgar. 1997. “The Assessment Proposal for Long-Term and Short-Term Tolerable Hygrothermal Microclimatic Conditions.” Physiological Research 46: 307–317.
  • Kahneman, D. 1973. Attention and Effort, pp. 246. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. ISBN: 0-13-050518-8
  • Kakizaki, T. 1987. “Sex Differences in Mental Workload during Performance of Mental Tasks.” Industrial Health 25 (4): 183–194. doi:https://doi.org/10.2486/indhealth.25.183.
  • Kleitman, N. 1963. Sleep and Wakefullness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Kramer, A. F. 1990. Physiological Metrics of Mental Workload: A Review of Recent Progress. San Diego, CA: Navy Personnel Research and Development Center.
  • Kramer, A. F., and S. Colcombe. 2018. “Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study-Revisited.” Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 13 (2): 213–217. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617707316.
  • Kreibig, S. D., F. H. Wilhelm, W. T. Roth, and J. J. Gross. 2007. “Cardiovascular, Electrodermal, and Respiratory Response Patterns to Fear- and Sadness-Inducing Films.” Psychophysiology 44 (5): 787–806. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00550.x.
  • Lee, E. Y., Y. O. Bang, and J. K. Ko. 2003. “Effect of Therapeutic Gymnastic Ball Exercise in Patients with Chronic Low Back Pain.” Physical Therapy Korea 10 (3): 109–126.
  • Levitt, S., and B. Gutin. 1971. “Multiple Choice Reaction Time and Movement Time during Physical Exertion.” Research Quarterly. American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation 42 (4): 405–410. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10671188.1971.10615088.
  • Marshall, P. W., and B. A. Murphy. 2005. “Core Stability Exercises on and off a Swiss Ball.” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 86 (2): 242–249. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2004.05.004.
  • Mehler, B., B. Reimer, and Y. Wang. 2011. “A Comparison of Heart Rate and Heart Rate Variability Indices in Distinguishing Single-Task Driving and Driving under Secondary Cognitive Workload.” Proceedings of the Sixth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design. California, USA. 590–597. doi:https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1451.
  • Mills, E. M. 1994. “The Effects of Low-Intensity Aerobic Exercise on Muscle Strength, Flexibility and Balance among Sedentary Elderly Persons.” Nursing Research 43: 207–211.
  • Miyake, S., S. Yamada, T. Shoji, Y. Takae, N. Kuge, and T. Yamamura. 2007. “Multidimensional Evaluation of Human Responses to the Workload.” In Engin. Psychol. and Cog. Ergonomics, HCII 2007, LNAI 4562, pp. 379–387, edited by D. Harris. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. ISBN-13 978-3-540-73330-0
  • Mulder, L. J. M. 1992. “Measurement and Analysis Methods of Heart Rate and Respiration for Use in Applied Environments.” Biological Psychology 34 (2–3): 205–236. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(92)90016-N.
  • Mulder, L. J. M., D. de Waard, K. A. Brookhuis, et al. 2005. “Estimating Mental Effort Using Heart Rate and Heart Rate Variability.” In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics Methods, 20–1–20-8, edited by N. Stanton. Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington, DC: CRC Press. ISBN 0-415-28700-6
  • Mulder, L. J. M., and G. Mulder. 1987. “Cardiovascular Reactivity and Mental Workload.” In The Beat-by-Beat Investigation of Cardiovascular Function, pp. 216–253, edited by O. Rompelman and R. I. Kitney. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Mulder, G., L. J. M. Mulder, T. F. Meijman, J. B. P. Veldman, and A. M. Van Roon. 2000. “A Psychophysiological Approach to Working Conditions.” In Engineering Psychophysiology: Issues and Applications, edited by R. W. Backs and W. Boucsein. pp. 139–159. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Nejedly, P., and J. Virgala. 2016. “Physiocrate: A Signal Plant Toolbox for Respiratory Blood Pressure and EMG Signal Analysis.” In Proceedings of the 22nd Conference STUDENT EEICT, pp. 53–55. Brno: Vysoké učení technické v Brně, Fakulta elektrotechniky a komunikačních technologií. ISBN: 978-80-214-5350-0
  • Pigeau, R., P. Naitoh, A. Buguet, C. McCann, J. Baranski, M. Taylor, M. Thompson, and I. Mack. 1995. “Modafinil, d-Amphetamine and Placebo during 64 Hours of Sustained Mental Work. I. Effects on Mood, Fatigue, Cognitive Performance and Body Temperature.” Journal of Sleep Research 4 (4): 212–228. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00172.x.
  • Plesinger, F., J. Jurco, J. Halamek, and P. Jurak. 2016. “SignalPlant: An Open Signal Processing Software Platform.” Physiological Measurement 37 (7): N38–N48. doi:https://doi.org/10.1088/0967-3334/37/7/N38.
  • Porges, S. W. 1995. “Orienting in a Defensive World: mammalian Modifications of Our Evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal Theory.” Psychophysiology 32 (4): 301–318. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01213.x.
  • Prinzel, L. J.III, and F. G. Freeman. 1997. “Task-Specific Sex Differences in Vigilance Performance: Subjective Workload and Boredom.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 85 (3 Pt 2): 1195–1202. doi:https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.85.3f.1195.
  • Ramshur, J. 2010. Design Evaluation and Application of Heart Rate Variability Analysis Software (HRVAS). Dissertation, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN. https://github.com/jramshur/HRVAS
  • Reimer, B., and B. Mehler. 2011. “The Impact of Cognitive Workload on Physiological Arousal in Young Adult Drivers: A Field Study and Simulation Validation.” Ergonomics 54 (10): 932–942. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2011.604431.
  • Rokyta, R. 2000. Fyziologie: pro Bakalářská Studia v Medicíně, Přírodovědných a Tělovýchovných Oborech. Praha: ISV Nakladatelství. ISBN: 45-5: 80–85866.
  • Salmela, J. H., and O. D. Ndoye. 1986. “Cognitive Distortions during Progressive Exercise.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 63 (3): 1067–1072. doi:https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.63.3.1067.
  • Schapkin, S. A., G. Freude, U. Erdmann, and H. Ruediger. 2007. “Stress and Managers Performance: Age-Related Changes in Psychophysiological Reactions to Cognitive Load.” In Engin. Psychol. and Cog. Ergonomics, HCII 2007, LNAI 4562, edited by D. Harris, pp. 417–425. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. ISBN-13 978-3-540-73330-0
  • Schmidt, E., R. Decke, R. Rasshofer, and A. C. Bullinger. 2017. “Psychophysiological Responses to Short-Term Cooling during a Simulated Monotonous Driving Task.” Applied Ergonomics 62: 9–18. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2017.01.017.
  • Šedivý, V. 2004. Ergonomie: cvičení. Brno: Mendelova Zemědělská a Lesnická Univerzita v Brně. ISBN 80-7157-763-4
  • Šedivý, V. 2006. Ergonomie a BOZP. Brno: Mendelova zemědělská a lesnická univerzita v Brně.
  • Senka, J., and J. Daniel. 1970. Vypracovanie a overovanie diagnostických pomôcok pre psychologický výber v niektorých strojárskych profesiách. Záverečná správa. ČSVUP, Bratislava.
  • Senka, J., J. Kuruc, and M. Čečer. 1992. Bourdonov Test. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostika.
  • Shimomura, Y., T. Yoda, K. Sugiura, A. Horiguchi, K. Iwanaga, and T. Katsuura. 2008. “Use of Frequency Domain Analysis of Skin Conductance for Evaluation of Mental Workload.” Journal of Physiological Anthropology 27 (4): 173–177. doi:https://doi.org/10.2114/jpa2.27.173.
  • Sjöberg, H. 1975. “Relations between Heart Rate, Reaction Speed, and Subjective Effort at Different Work Loads on a Bicycle Ergometer.” Journal of Human Stress 1 (4): 21–27. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/0097840X.1975.9939549.
  • Son, J., B. Mehler, T. Lee, Y. Park, J. Coughlin, and B. Reimer. 2011. “Impact of Cognitive Workload on Physiological Arousal and Performance in Younger and Older Drivers.” Proceedings of the Sixth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design. California, USA. pp. 87–94.
  • Sternberg, R. J., and K. Sternberg. 2012. Cognitive Psychology, pp. 609. 6th ed. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning. ISBN-13978–971. 111-34476-4
  • Švancara, J. 2003. Emoce, Motivace, Volní Procesy. Brno: Psychologický Ústav FF MU v Brně. ISBN: 80-86633-11-X
  • Takeuchi, Y. 2000. “Change in Blood Volume in the Brain during a Simulated Aircraft Landing Task.” Journal of Occupational Health 42 (2): 60–65. doi:https://doi.org/10.1539/joh.42.60.
  • Takeyama, H., T. Itani, N. Tachi, O. Sakamura, K. Murata, T. Inoue, T. Takanishi, et al. 2005. “Effects of Shift Schedules on Fatigue and Physiological Functions among Firefighters during Night Duty.” Ergonomics 48 (1): 1–11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00140130412331303920.
  • Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. 1996. Heart Rate Variability: standards of Measurement, Physiological Interpretation and Clinical Use. European Heart Journal 17: 354–381.
  • Tůma, V. 1989. “Surface Temperature of the Human Body as Indicators of Regulatory and Dispositional Abilities of the Human Organism.” Čs. psychologie, XXXIII 4: 345–360.
  • Van Roon, A. M., L. J. M. Mulder, M. Althaus, and G. Mulder. 2004. “Introducing a Baroreflex Model for Studying Cardiovascular Effects of Mental Workload.” Psychophysiology 41 (6): 961–981. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00251.x.
  • Veltman, J. A., and W. K. Gaillard. 1998. “Physiological Workload Reactions to Increasing Levels of Task Difficulty.” Ergonomics 41 (5): 656–669. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/001401398186829.
  • Vinkers, C. H., R. Penning, M. M. Ebbens, J. Hellhammer, J. C. Verste, C. J. Kalkman, and B. Olivier. 2010. “Stress-Induced Hyperthermia in Translational Stress Research.” The Open Pharmacology Journal 4 (1): 30–35. doi:https://doi.org/10.2174/1874143601004010030.
  • Vítek, M., and J. Kozumplík. 2011. QRS SEEKER; Software pro detekci komplexů QRS. Ústav biomedicínského inženýrství Vysoké učení technické v Brně, Kolejní 2906/4 612 00 Brno Česká republika. http://www.ubmi.feec.vutbr.cz/vyzkum-a-vyvoj/produkty.
  • Weiner, J. S. 1982. “The Ergonomics Society. The Society’s Lecture 1982. The Measurement of Human Workload.” Ergonomics 25 (11): 953–965. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00140138208925057.
  • Wever, R. A. 1979. The Circadian System of Man. New York, Heidelberg, Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Whang, M. Ch., J. S. Lim, K. R. Park, Y. Cho, and W. Boucsein. 2007. “Are Computers Capable of Understanding Our Emotional States?” In Engin. Psychol. and Cog. Ergonomics, HCII 2007, LNAI 4562, edited by D. Harris. pp. 204–211. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. ISBN-13 978-3-540-73330-0
  • Wientjes, C. J. E. 1993. Psychological Influences upon Breathing: situational and Dispositional Aspects. Soesterberg: TNO Institute for Perception. ISBN 90-6743–235-0.
  • Wientjes, C. J. E., and P. Grossman. 2005. “Measurement of Respiration in Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics Research.” In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics Methods, edited by N. Stanton, et al. pp. 26–1–26-9. Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington, DC: CRC Press. ISBN 0-415-28700-6
  • Wilson, G., F. P. Fullenkamp, and I. Davis. 1994. “Evoked Potential, Cardiac, Blink, and Respiration Measures of Pilot Workload in Air-to-Ground Missions.” Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine 65 (2): 100–105.