7,510
Views
99
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Moving forward: how depression heterogeneity hinders progress in treatment and research

Pages 423-425 | Received 17 Jan 2017, Accepted 14 Mar 2017, Published online: 22 Mar 2017

1. Introduction

Few would argue that depression research in the last two decades has led to major improvements of clinical care for patients. The tools and insights we have at our disposal today have not changed much over the years. Three examples for slow progress are antidepressants, depression assessment, and research on biological markers. Drugs most commonly prescribed for major depression disorder (MDD) were developed in the 1980s and 1990s, only slightly outperform placebos, and their exact mechanisms remain unknown [Citation1]. Hamilton’s rating scale for depression – still the gold standard in clinical trials – is nearly 60 years old [Citation2] and differs considerably from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth Edition) (DSM-5) MDD criteria [Citation3]. And the study of MDD biology has largely resulted in null-findings or small effects that are impracticable for clinical purposes, despite ever-growing samples [Citation4,Citation5].

A growing chorus of voices – including prominent members of the DSM-5 task force and the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) [Citation6,Citation7] – has raised concerns about the reliability and validity of MDD, concluding that the clinical phenotype may hold little usefulness for research [Citation8,Citation9]. What exactly are the problems, and what can we do better?

2. Depression sum-scores don’t add up

Nearly all depression research comprises several steps. A specific rating scale or clinical interview is selected among a large number of instruments [Citation10], and symptoms such as sad mood, worthlessness, suicidal ideation, weight gain or weight loss, insomnia or hypersomnia, psychomotor problems, anxiety, genital problems, paralysis, crying, hypochondriasis, and lack of insight are assessed [Citation3]. These diverse symptoms are added up to one sum-score that represents depression severity, and a threshold on the sum-score is used to distinguish individuals with MDD from healthy controls. Researchers then investigate whether variables of interest such as biological markers or treatment response are statistically related to the sum-score or compare patients and controls regarding such variables. Three lines of research demonstrate that this approach may obfuscate insights and hinder progress.

First, MDD is a highly heterogeneous diagnosis, and two patients with a DSM-5 diagnosis may share no single symptom. Alice comes into the practice as 10-am appointment reporting sadness, weight loss, psychomotor retardation, concentration problems, and suicidal ideation. Bob – our 11-am appointment – suffers from anhedonia, insomnia, self-blame, fatigue, and weight gain. A recent paper identified 1030 unique symptom profiles in 3703 depressed patients [Citation11], and it seems questionable to investigate risk factors, biological markers, or other variables of interest in a depressed population when patients differ so considerably in their problems.

Second, it is remarkable how fuzzy the definition of MDD is. The most common rating scales feature over 50 disparate depressive symptoms [Citation3], and many scales show little content overlap with the DSM-5 MDD criteria. Such a situation is difficult to envision for medical conditions like cancer or measles. The lack of a clear definition is also apparent in the fact that many different scales exist that aim to measure the same disorder – Santor et al. counted 280 different depression scales [Citation10]. Moreover, dozens of MDD subtypes have been proposed and disregarded over the last century.

Third, the approach of summing disparate symptoms presupposes that all symptoms measure the same underlying construct, the same way that 10 math questions measure the same underlying construct ‘mathematical intelligence’ or 10 symptoms of cancer indicate the same underlying condition cancer. For depression, the notion that one sum-score of symptoms is a good proxy for severity is inconsistent with half a century of psychometric literature: depression rating scales are not unidimensional, a psychometric fact that cannot properly be reflected in one sum-score [Citation12]. Nearly 60 years ago, Hamilton insisted in his seminal 1960 paper that the sum-score be regarded only as ‘total crude score.’ His case report of seven patients provides detailed information on four depression factors such as retarded depression and anxiety reaction, omitting information on the total crude score of patients that carries little information [Citation2].

In sum, lumping disparate symptoms to a sum-score – and lumping patients with very different symptoms into one category – results in loss of important information. These common research practices have contributed to the lack of progress in depression research.

3. Moving forward: Depression Symptomics

The novel research framework Depression Symptomics promises a way forward. Depression Symptomics aims to tackle widely acknowledged shortcomings of sum-scores and depression diagnosis by focusing on three objectives: studying (1) individual symptoms, (2) the causal relations among symptoms, and (3) personalized processes of patients.

3.1. Studying individual symptoms

Recent work has shown that specific MDD symptoms differ from each other in crucial aspects: They differ in their impact on impairment of functioning, their response to specific life events, their relations with biological markers, and their risk factors [Citation13,Citation14]. There is also evidence that symptoms differ in their response to antidepressant treatment [Citation15,Citation16] – which makes sense when we consider that the most common side effects of antidepressants are the very symptoms used to measure depression (e.g. fatigue, insomnia or hypersomnia, weight and appetite changes, and sexual dysfunction). When investigating changes of symptom sum-scores over time in clinical trials, patients’ improvement on some symptoms is likely concealed by increases in other symptoms.

This also suggests that symptom-based investigations may offer potential for genetic and neuroimaging studies that have largely resulted in null-findings. The same holds for clinical trials: We should study what specific symptoms improve and worsen in response to specific antidepressants. This may enable us to predict based on baseline symptom profiles which participants will likely respond positively to what kind of drug. Many prior studies have investigated the efficacy of antidepressants, and while it is not always easy to obtain such data [Citation17], we should try to utilize the already existing information for symptom-based investigations before we collect new data (cf. [Citation15]).

3.2. Studying associations among symptoms

Symptoms of depression can interact with each other in complex dynamical systems, such as sadness → insomnia → fatigue → concentration problems. It is widely acknowledged that depressed patients are often trapped in situations of reinforcing problems. Such causal influences among symptoms are ignored in the study of sum-scores and have recently become a topic of detailed study in an emerging field termed ‘network approach to psychopathology’ [Citation18,Citation19].

Specifically, studies have shown that certain depression symptoms seem to be more relevant than others in symptom networks of patients, that symptom networks of depressed people show different characteristics than those of controls, and that symptom networks may emit so-called ‘early warning signals’ before healthy people transition into depression (review [Citation18]). This network approach opens up new possibilities of timely prevention and intervention.

3.3. Personalized medicine

Recent technological developments such as smart-watches and other wearable devices allow for daily monitoring of depressed patients [Citation20], and new statistical models enable us to estimate idiographic and nomothetic processes at the same time [Citation21]. Personalized medicine – paying closer attention to differences among patients – may offer an important inroad to overcome limitations of the highly heterogeneous depression phenotype. Different fields of application come to mind: Improved prediction of depression onset in samples at risk of developing depression, improved prediction of depression relapse, and improved prediction of treatment [Citation22].

3.4. The clinical utility of Depression Symptomics

Depression Symptomics is a novel research framework that has led to important scientific insights in different domains of research – but does it offer therapeutic gain for patients? There are several studies in support of this notion. Regarding (1) studying individual symptoms, MDD symptoms differentially predict relapse [Citation22] and differ in their responsiveness to treatment both within and across different antidepressant medications [Citation15,Citation16]. Regarding (2) studying associations among symptoms, symptoms that drive depressive processes seem to be especially strong predictors for future depression [Citation23] and so are depression networks where symptoms are strongly interconnected [Citation24]. For (3) personalized medicine, a randomized clinical trial has shown that providing patients with idiographic feedback about their dynamic processes leads to a reduction of depressive symptomatology [Citation25], and a study with one participant was able to predict the relapse into depression before it occurred based on a complex dynamical systems model [Citation26].

4. Future outlook

MDD diagnosis provides little clinical utility: It lacks treatment specificity, a clear clinical presentation, and precise diagnostic boundaries and has high comorbidity rates and a very low inter-rater reliability [Citation8,Citation9]. Head of the DSM-5 task force David Kupfer concluded that ‘the relatively low reliability of major depressive disorder […] is a concern for clinical decision-making’ [Citation6], and prior NIMH director Thomas Insel stated that the DSM’s main weakness is its ‘lack of validity’ [Citation7].

Depression Symptomics conceptualizes individual symptoms as fundamental building blocks of mental disorders that provide an untapped source of important and clinically relevant information. This is consistent with the NIMH strategic plan for mood disorder research and the NIMH Research Domain Criteria that aim to study and identify more homogeneous and reliable endophenotypes [Citation7,Citation27]. Embracing complexity was a key requirement to recent insights in biology, economics, and environmental sciences, and Depression Symptomics may change our perspective of MDD from an unwieldy and cumbersome disorder to a phenotype where attention to specific symptoms and their interactions – and attention to differences between patients – are crucial for scientific and clinical progress.

Declaration of interest

The author has no relevant affiliations or financial involvement with any organization or entity with a financial interest in or financial conflict with the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript. This includes employment, consultancies, honoraria, stock ownership or options, expert testimony, grants or patents received or pending, or royalties.

Additional information

Funding

This paper was funded by European Research Council Consolidator Grant no. 647209.

References

  • Kirsch I, Deacon BJ, Huedo-Medina TB, et al. Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. Plos Med. 2008;5:e45.
  • Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62.
  • Fried EI. The 52 symptoms of major depression. J Affect Disord. 2017;208:191–197.
  • Cai N, Bigdeli TB, Kretzschmar W, et al. Sparse whole-genome sequencing identifies two loci for major depressive disorder. Nature. 2015;523:588–591.
  • Fried EI, Kievit RA. The volumes of subcortical regions in depressed and healthy individuals are strikingly similar: a reinterpretation of the results by Schmaal et al. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:724–725.
  • Kupfer DJ, Kraemer HC Field trial results guide DSM recommendations. Huffington Post. (2013). [cited 2017 Mar1]. Available from: http://www.huffingtonpost.com/david-j-kupfer-md/dsm-5_b_2083092.html
  • Insel TR Transforming diagnosis. National Institute of Mental Health. 2013. [cited 2017 Mar 1] Available from: http://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/director/2013/transforming-diagnosis.shtml
  • Fried EI. Problematic assumptions have slowed down depression research: why symptoms, not syndromes are the way forward. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1–11.
  • Parker G. Beyond major depression. Psychol Med. 2005;35(4):467–474.
  • Santor DA, Gregus M, Welch A. Eight decades of measurement in depression. Measurement. 2006;4:135–155.
  • Fried EI, Nesse RM. Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study. J Affect Disord. 2015;172:96–102.
  • Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Epskamp S, et al. Measuring depression over time or not? Lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychol Assess. 2016;28:1354–1367.
  • Fried EI, Nesse RM. Depression sum-scores don’t add up: why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Med. 2015;13:1–11.
  • Jokela M, Virtanen M, Batty GD. Inflammation and specific symptoms of depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:1–6.
  • Hieronymus F, Emilsson JF, Nilsson S, et al. Consistent superiority of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors over placebo in reducing depressed mood in patients with major depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:523–530.
  • Chekroud AM, Gueorguieva R, Krumholz HM, et al. Reevaluating the efficacy and predictability of antidepressant treatments: a symptom clustering approach. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;6511:1-9.
  • Le Noury J, Nardo JM, Healy D, et al. Restoring study 329: efficacy and harms of paroxetine and imipramine in treatment of major depression in adolescence. The BMJ. 2015;101006:1-16.
  • Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Cramer AOJ, et al. Mental disorders as networks of problems: a review of recent insights. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;1:1–32.
  • Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. 2017;16:5–13.
  • Bos FM, Schoevers RA, Aan Het Rot M. Experience sampling and ecological momentary assessment studies in psychopharmacology: a systematic review. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25:1853–1864.
  • Schuurman NK, Ferrer E, de Boer-Sonnenschein M, et al. How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychol Methods. 2016;21:206–221.
  • Chekroud AM, Zotti RJ, Shehzad Z, et al. Cross-trial prediction of treatment outcome in depression: a machine learning approach. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;366:1–8. [cited 2017 MAR 1]. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S221503661500471X
  • Boschloo L, van Borkulo CD, Borsboom D, et al. A prospective study on how symptoms in a network predict the onset of depression. Psychother Psychosom. 2016;85(3):183–184.
  • van Borkulo CD, Boschloo L, Borsboom D, et al. Association of symptom network structure with the course of longitudinal depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:1219.
  • Kramer I, Simons C, Hartmann JA, et al. A therapeutic application of the experience sampling method in the treatment of depression: a randomized controlled trial. World Psychiatry. 2014;13:68–77.
  • Wichers M, Groot PC, Psychosystems, et al. Critical slowing down as a personalized early warning signal for depression. Psychother Psychosom. 2016;85:114–116.
  • National Institute of Mental Health. Breaking ground, breaking through: the strategic plan for mood disorders research (NIH Publication No. 03–5121). Washington, DC: National Institutes of Health. 2003.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.