191
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLE

Get by With a Little Help From a Word: Multimodal Input Facilitates 26-Month-Olds' Ability to Map and Generalize Arbitrary Gestural Labels

Pages 250-269 | Published online: 22 Apr 2013
 

Abstract

In the early stages of word learning, children demonstrate considerable flexibility in the type of symbols they will accept as object labels. However, around the 2nd year, as children continue to gain language experience, they become focused on more conventional symbols (e.g., words) as opposed to less conventional symbols (e.g., gestures). During this period of symbolic narrowing, the degree to which children are able to learn other types of labels, such as arbitrary gestures, remains a topic of debate. Thus, the purpose of the current set of experiments was to determine whether a multimodal label (word + gesture) could facilitate 26-month-olds' ability to learn an arbitrary gestural label. We hypothesized that the multimodal label would exploit children's focus on words thereby increasing their willingness to interpret the gestural label. To test this hypothesis, we conducted two experiments. In Experiment 1, 26-month-olds were trained with a multimodal label (word + gesture) and tested on their ability to map and generalize both the arbitrary gesture and the multimodal label to familiar and novel objects. In Experiment 2, 26-month-olds were trained and tested with only the gestural label. The findings revealed that 26-month-olds are able to map and generalize an arbitrary gesture when it is presented multimodally with a word, but not when it is presented in isolation. Furthermore, children's ability to learn the gestural labels was positively related to their reported productive vocabulary, providing additional evidence that children's focus on words actually helped, not hindered, their gesture learning.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to sincerely thank Laura Kurtz and the Wilbourn Infant Lab for their assistance in the recruitment, data collection, and coding for this project. We also sincerely appreciate Becky Williams for taking the time to modify the stimuli. We would also like to thank Vrinda Kalia, Laura Kuhn, and the WILD writing group for their invaluable feedback on previous versions of the manuscript. Finally, we would like to acknowledge the parents and children who participated in the study.

Notes

1Although a nonlinguistic sound cannot be considered a “label” in the traditional sense, Woodward and Hoyne (Citation1999) use the term “labeled” to describe the target object.

2The denominator is 17 instead of 20 because there were 3 trials where children selected both objects simultaneously. Thus, we only used trials where either the target or distracter object was selected.

*For the generalization trials, one female failed to make a selection (n = 23).

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Jacqueline Prince Sims

Jacqueline Prince Sims is now at the Department of Counseling, Developmental and Educational Psychology, Boston College.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.