9,546
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Developmetrics

Development and initial validation of the comprehensive early childhood parenting questionnaire (CECPAQ) for parents of 1–4 year-olds

, , &
Pages 233-247 | Received 22 Jun 2015, Accepted 12 Apr 2016, Published online: 09 May 2016

Abstract

Parenting is a multifaceted task and the way in which parents fulfill this task plays an important role in children’s growth and development, especially in early childhood. Conceptualization and assessment of parenting behavior is elementary for research on child and family development and would fill a gap in clinical work, as there is a lack of questionnaires simultaneously assessing multiple parenting behaviors specific for early childhood. Therefore the Comprehensive Early Childhood Parenting Questionnaire (CECPAQ) was designed; a parent report measure of commonly occurring behaviors in five domains of parenting (i.e., support, stimulation, structure, harsh discipline, and positive discipline) central to early childhood. Data were collected from 1139 mothers and 526 fathers of 1–4 year-old children. Confirmatory factor analysis confirmed a five-factor structure (composed of 54 items). The five parenting domains were found to have good internal consistency and temporal stability. Preliminary evidence of validity of the CECPAQ was demonstrated by moderate relationships with measures of parental stress and child problem behavior. It is concluded that the CECPAQ is a promising measure of self-perceived parenting behavior for parents of 1–4 year-old children.

Parenting is a multifaceted task and the way in which parents fulfill this task plays an important role in children’s growth and development, especially in early childhood (0–5 years; e.g., Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, Citation2000). Conceptualization and measurement of parental behavior constitutes an elementary component of research on child development, and researchers and practitioners are increasingly concerned with assessing parental behavior (e.g., Duppong Hurley, Huscroft-D’Angelo, Trout, Griffith, & Epstein, 2013). Self-report measures are considered a useful approach to assess these behaviors, as they are easy to administer and provide summary reports of behavior over longer periods of time or in situations that are difficult to observe. Whereas recently a comprehensive parenting questionnaire was published concerning children aged 5–13 (Sleddens et al., Citation2014), for younger children no such questionnaire is yet available. The development and initial validation of a comprehensive parenting questionnaire assessing various parenting behaviors central to the development of toddlers and preschoolers is described in this article.

At least three main theories regarding the role of parenting in early childhood are important. First, the attachment theory (Bowlby, Citation1969) states that parents need to serve as a warm and secure base, enabling children to feel comfortable to explore their environment, manipulate objects, and interact with others; experiences that form the foundation for healthy child development (Bowlby, Citation1969; Sroufe, Citation1979). Parents form a secure base by showing affection to their child, by being sensitive to their cues, and by being responsive by satisfying the child’s needs and helping the child to regulate its behavior and emotions. In addition, parents have to provide an organized environment and external structure for children by setting clear and consistent rules (consistency) and follow-through on them (i.e., not being lax), without being overreactive by having their emotional state interfere too much with their responses to their child (Grolnick & Pomerantz, Citation2009; Lorber, Citation2012). Indeed, previous studies showed that high levels of parental support and structure are associated with positive developmental outcomes of the child (Davidov & Grusec, Citation2006; Verhoeven, Junger, Van Aken, Deković, & Van Aken, Citation2010a; Zimmer-Gembeck & Thomas, Citation2010).

A second theory, formulated by Vygotsky, states that parents stimulate their child to learn and develop (Holden, Citation2010) by engaging them in learning activities (i.e., storytelling, singing, dancing) and providing a variety of experiences (i.e., grocery shopping, watering plants, playing with other children). Indeed, such activities and experiences are known to contribute to the child’s cognitive, communicative, motor and socio-emotional development (Cates et al., Citation2012; Miquelote, Santos, Caçola, Montebelo, & Gabbard, Citation2012). In addition, the provision of age appropriate toys has been associated with better cognitive and language development and reduction of the need for early intervention to support their development (Dos Santos et al., Citation2008; Thepsuthammarat, Thinkhamrop, & Choprapawon, Citation2012; Tomopoulos et al., Citation2006).

Thirdly, social learning theory (Bandura, Citation1977) states that children learn how to behave in their social context through modeling and observational learning. Parents can stimulate both positive and negative behavior in their children by using rewards and punishment. The parenting domain of positive discipline refers to parental behavior that reinforces the child’s adaptive behavior, for example, parental induction (e.g., explaining why certain behavior is unwanted) and praising (Choe, Olson, & Sameroff, Citation2013; Kerr, Lopez, Olson, & Sameroff, Citation2004). In contrast, the domain of harsh discipline refers to disciplinary techniques, such as verbal and physical punishment and psychological control, with which parents try to reduce unwanted behavior, but also induce fear and aggression in their children. Indeed, children who are frequently exposed to harsh disciplinary techniques are more likely to show oppositional and aggressive behaviors (Gershoff, Citation2002; Lansford et al., Citation2011; Lysenko, Barker, & Jaffee, Citation2013; Stormshak, Bierman, McMahon, & Lengua, Citation2000). The use of psychological control- i.e., parents’ attempts to control their children’s behavior through psychological means, like intrusive behavior, love withdrawal and guilt induction (Barber, Citation1996)- is thought to limit the child’s opportunities to build a healthy self-image and results in more behavior problems (Aunola & Nurmi, Citation2005; Verhoeven et al., Citation2010a; Verhoeven, Junger, Van Aken, Deković, & Van Aken, Citation2010b).

Based on these three theories, we deduced five domains of parenting that we considered important for our questionnaire: support (sensitivity, responsiveness, affection), stimulation (activities, exposure, toys), structure (consistency, laxness, overreactivity), and positive or harsh discipline (physical and verbal punishment, psychological control) (see Appendix 1). Existing questionnaires often fall short of assessing this wide range of parenting behaviors simultaneously (Duppong-Hurley et al., Citation2014).

Another restriction of the existing self- report questionnaires is that they often include items that refer to parental attitudes, beliefs, and feelings of self-efficacy rather than behavior (Lovejoy, Weis, O’Hare, & Rubin, Citation1999). Although these cognitions and feelings of efficacy are important determinants of parenting behavior (Bugental & Johnston, Citation2000; Jones & Prinz, Citation2005), they represent different aspects of parenting. The inclusion of such aspects hampers the interpretation of the measured constructs.

As a consequence of these restrictions, many researchers rely on a combination of subscales of existing measurement instruments, use different items, and make their own adjustments to item content to render them age-appropriate. This hinders the comparison of research results and limits the possibilities to develop universal theories about the role of parenting in child development.

We therefore developed a new questionnaire that captures a range of parenting behaviors that are specific and potentially important for early child development: the CECPAQ. We wanted our measure to focus on behaviors that could be shown by the parent – and not include parental attitudes and beliefs- and that have a strong theoretical foundation regarding the relationship to early child development. To ensure our questionnaire is valid to assess parenting behaviors of both mothers and fathers, we included a sample of fathers in our study.

In the current study, we report on the development of the CECPAQ and its psychometric properties by (1) describing the item-selection for the questionnaire, (2) studying the factor structure and reliability of the CECPAQ, and (3) examining how the CECPAQ is associated with parental stress and child problem behavior as means of validity.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited through day care centers and preschools and consisted of 1139 families of 1–4 year-old children (M = 26.05 months, SD = 9.35; 50.8% girls). From 526 families both parents participated. As such, the sample consists of 1139 mothers and 526 fathers. The average age was M = 33.17 years (SD = 4.39) for mothers and M = 35.29 years (SD = 5.00) for fathers. The majority of the parents were Dutch/Caucasian (97.2% of the mothers, 99.8% of the fathers) and currently living together (97.4%). Most parents were college educated (62% of the mothers and 56.5% of the fathers). The average number of children living in the family was M = 1.82, SD = .82, ranging from 1 to 6.

Procedure

Families received a recruitment letter explaining the goals of the project (i.e., to examine how mothers and fathers parent their 1–4 year-old child). Following the families consent, self-report questionnaires were mailed and parents were asked to return the completed questionnaires within two weeks.

Measures

CECPAQ

Starting from our conceptualization of parenting as consisting of five main domains (support, structure, stimulation, harsh discipline, and positive discipline), four parenting experts selected a range of existing measures of parenting behavior that were available and commonly used to assess parenting behaviors in toddlers and preschoolers. After reviewing these questionnaires, the experts agreed on 60 potential items to assess the parenting behaviors described in the introduction; 15 items for each of the first 4 parenting domains and 5 items to assess positive discipline. Six of these items were dropped because of redundancy or ambiguity. An overview of the remaining 54 items and how they are clustered in (sub)domains of parenting is provided in Appendix 1.

All items were assessed on a 6-point scale, indicating how often parents showed the described behavior (1 = never to 6 = always). For nine items (item 14–23), ratings were made on 6-point scales that are anchored by one effective and one ineffective response to the presented parenting situation.

Parental stress

A subsample of 224 mothers and 214 fathers filled out the Parental Stress Index, Short Form (PSI-SF; Abidin, Citation1995), Dutch version (De Brock, Vermulst, Gerris, & Abidin, Citation1992); a self-report measure of 25 items measuring parenting stress on a 6-point Lickert Scale ranging from 1 = Not applicable to 6 = Very applicable. Cronbach’s alpha’s in the current study were αMother = .92, and αFather = .88.

Child behavior problems

A subsample of 181 mothers and 176 fathers filled out the Child Behavior Checklist 1½–5 (Achenbach & Rescorla, Citation2000) to assess their child’s internalizing and externalizing behavior. The two broad scales of externalizing (n = 24 items) and internalizing behavior (n = 36 items) were used. Cronbach’s alpha in the current study for internalizing and externalizing behavior respectively were: αMother = .85 and .89, αFather = .78 and .89.

Data analyses

To test whether hypothesized theoretical model fits the data we used, the CECPAQ is examined by means of Confirmatory Factor Analyses using MPlus version 6.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2011). The nested structure of the data (i.e., for 526 families both parents participated and reported parenting behavior regarding the same child) was accounted for by means of testing ‘complex structure’ (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2011). The goodness of fit of the models was assessed by multiple criteria: root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) value less than .08, the comparative fit index (CFI), and the Tucker Lewis index (TLI) greater than .90 are considered an acceptable fit (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999; Kline, Citation2005). Factor loadings were considered salient when they were at least .40 (Stevens, Citation2002). As the data are ordered-categorical measures and not normally distributed (skewness of item scores ranged from −4.77 to 4.76; kurtosis from −1.04 to 29.83), we used the Robust Weighted Least Squares procedure for parameter estimation (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2011). Full information maximum likelihood estimation (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2011) was used to handle missing data. Missing data regarding parenting items was limited to 1–2.5%. Parents who had missing data on 1 or more items of the CECPAQ did not differ from parents with full data on the CECPAQ regarding their age, gender, education level, family size, levels of experienced stress or levels of reported child internalizing and externalizing behavior (statistics were: −0.052 < t < 2.680; .007 < p < .682). There was, however, a small –but significant – difference of 3 months in the mean age of the child they were raising (t = 3.564, p < .001).

We tested the hypothesized factor structure of the CECPAQ representing all 54 items, concerning 12 subdomains (lower order), and 5 parenting domains: four higher order (Support, Stimulation, Structure, and Harsh Discipline) and one first order factor (Positive Discipline) (see Figure ; five-factor model). Items loaded on just one subdomain of parenting, and each subdomain of parenting loaded on only one parenting domain. The five parenting domains were allowed to correlate (Figure ). Two comparison models were tested: A statistical comparison model, in which one higher-order factor loaded on all 13 (sub)domains of parenting (one-factor model), and a comparison model discerning two higher-order factors (two-factor model), ‘Warmth/Support’ loading on the subdomains of Support (responsiveness, sensitivity, affection) and Stimulation (activity, exposure, play), and ‘Control’ loading on the subdomains of Structure (consistency, overreactivity, laxness), Harsh Discipline (verbal punishment, physical punishment, psychological control) and the domain of Positive Discipline. The five-factor model is nested in each of the two comparison models. A significant (p < .01) difference in the model’s chi-square would indicate that the five-factor model explains the data better than the comparison model. As we used robust maximum-likelihood estimation, and differences between two robust chi-square goodness-of-fit statistics do not have a chi-square distribution (Satorra, Citation2000), an adjusted chi-square difference test was used to compare model fit (Muthén & Muthén, Citation2011; Satorra & Bentler, Citation1999).

Figure 1. Factor structure of the CECPAQ and standardized parameters.

Notes: Sens = sensitivity, Resp = responsiveness, Aff = affection, Act = learning activities, Exp = exposure to learning environment, Play = provision and playing with toys, Cons = consistency, Over (R) = overreactivity, reversed coded, Lax = laxness, reversed coded, Verb = verbal punishment, Phys = physical punishment, Psyc = psychological punishment. r = correlations between parenting domains, β = factor loadings. Item numbers in the Figure refer to the questions in Appendix 1.
Figure 1. Factor structure of the CECPAQ and standardized parameters.

The CECPAQ was considered reliable when the Cronbach’s alpha is >.70 in each parenting domain (Kline, Citation2000). The validity of the CECPAQ was determined by examining its correlations with parental stress and children’s behavior problems. Based on previous studies, medium correlations are expected between parenting and parental stress (e.g., Anthony et al., Citation2005; Mackler et al., Citation2015), and small to medium correlations between parenting and child problem behavior (e.g., Lansford et al., Citation2011; Lysenko et al., Citation2013; Stormshak et al., Citation2000; Verhoeven et al., Citation2010a, Citation2010b). These correlations are expected to be negative for all parenting domains, except for Harsh Discipline.

Results

Factor structure

Fit measures are presented in Table . Model fit indices were only acceptable for the 5-factor solution (Table ). The adjusted chi-square difference test showed that the hypothesized 5-factor solution fitted the data significantly better than the 1-factor and 2-factor solutions. Estimated standardized parameters of the five-factor model are presented in Figure . All lower-order factor loadings were statistically significant (p < .001), with standardized loadings ranging from β = .43 to β = .92. In addition, the factor loadings regarding the higher order factors were all statistically significant (p < .001) and ranged from .66 to .96. The correlations between the parenting domains were all statistically significant (p < .001) and ranged from r = −.14 to r = −.71.

Table 1. Fit measures of the three tested models regarding parenting.

Reliability

The full sample of 1665 parents was used to estimate the internal consistency of the CECPAQ. Cronbach’s alpha’s for mothers/fathers were respectively: .88/.88 for Support, .82/.86 for Stimulation, .75/.77 for Structure, .79/.79 for Harsh Discipline, and .76/.77 for Positive Discipline.

Validity

Correlations between the CECPAQ and the PSI and CBCL are presented in Table . For both mothers and fathers, Support and Structure were negatively and Harsh Discipline was positively related to parental stress. For fathers, but not for mothers, Stimulation and Positive Discipline were also negatively related to parental stress. The statistically significant correlations varied from .23 to .48.

Table 2. Descriptives of the CECPAQ scores and correlations with parental reported stress (PSI-SF) and child behavior problems (CBCL).

Three of the five parenting domains were related to child problem behavior. Structure was negatively related and Harsh Discipline positively, to internalizing and externalizing behaviors, for mothers as well as fathers. For fathers Support was negatively associated with children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors, whereas for mothers this association was only statistically significant for externalizing behaviors. The statistically significant correlations varied from .15 to .35. The parenting domains of Stimulation and Positive Discipline were unrelated to children’s problem behavior.

Discussion

The CECPAQ was developed as a comprehensive self-report measure to assess five main domains of parenting behaviors central to the development of toddlers and preschoolers. The current study provides initial support for the factor structure, reliability, and validity of the CECPAQ.

Factor structure and reliability

A confirmatory factor analysis including all final 54 items of the CECPAQ, provided a good fit with the intended five factor structure and showed that all factor loadings had a greater value than. 40. The reliabilities of the five parenting domains -with the lowest Cronbach’s alpha of .75- is acceptable and relatively high for a self-report parenting questionnaire: According to a recent review by Duppong Hurley, Huscroft-D’Angelo, Trout, Griffith, and Epstein (Citation2014) the majority of existing parenting questionnaires had Cronbach’s alpha ratings between .50 and .69. The relatively high internal consistencies and the moderate correlations between the different parenting domains indicate that the CECPAQ is capable of reliably assessing five distinct domains of parenting. Some parenting domains were strongly correlated (e.g, Support with Stimulation, Structure with Harsh Discipline), but a more parsimonious factor structure in which the subdomains of these parenting domains were collapsed into a single factor did not improve the model’s fit. This confirms the idea that these parenting domains are conceptually different, and justifies our decision to keep them separated.

Validity

We also found support for the validity of the CECPAQ. The relations found between parental stress and parenting were as expected – both regarding strength as well as direction. Medium to strong correlations were found between parental stress and parenting, with negative relations for parental reports of support and structure, and positive relations for harsh discipline. For fathers, stress was also negatively related to reported stimulation and positive discipline. This is in line with previous findings (Anthony et al., Citation2005; Mackler et al., Citation2015) and Belsky’s (Citation1984) model of parenting, stating that parents who report high levels of stress may have fewer social, emotional, and tangible resources on which to draw as parents. Consequently, these parents may be more irritable and distressed when dealing with their children, as expressed in lower levels of support, stimulation, structure and positive discipline, and higher levels of harsh discipline.

Furthermore lower levels of support and structure, and more use of harsh discipline were significantly associated with more problem behavior in children, with small to medium effect sizes. This is also in accordance with previous studies (e.g., Lansford et al., Citation2011; Lysenko et al., Citation2013; Stormshak et al., Citation2000; Verhoeven et al., Citation2010a, Citation2010b). Parental stimulation was unrelated to the child’s problem behavior. This is not surprising, as parental behaviors in this domain are aimed at stimulating cognitive, motor, and language development, rather than social-emotional behaviors. Future studies should examine how the parenting domains assessed by the CECPAQ are related to children’s developmental outcomes, other than problem behavior.

No relation was found between parental positive discipline and the children’s behavior problems. This might reflect the ‘bad is stronger than good’ principle, which states that good events (e.g., positive discipline) have less impact than bad events (Baumeister, Bratlavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, Citation2001). It could also be that the majority of the children in our sample is too young to find an effect of positive discipline on behavioral problems, as children just start to internalize rules of conduct in the preschool period (Calkins & Fox, Citation2002). Perhaps no association was found between positive discipline and child problem behavior as this domain was assessed by only four items, concerning just two discipline techniques: induction and praising. Passini, Pihet, and Favez (Citation2014) recently found that mothers of toddlers also use techniques including timeout and removal of privileges. Further study of the concept of positive discipline and its relation to children’s behavior is recommended. In-depth interviews with parents could further our insight in disciplinary techniques parents of toddlers and preschoolers use.

The results of this study should be interpreted in the light of its limitations. First, the sample of this study was rather homogenous (mainly high educated, intact families), limiting the generalizability of the results. In addition, the use of a single reporter for most data might have inflated the correlations between the CECPAQ and the other parent and child measures. Also, social desirability may have caused bias, especially when assessing negative parenting behaviors. Future studies regarding the validity of the CECPAQ should use multiple methods, such as observations of parent-child interactions to examine the relation between CECPAQ and actual parenting behavior. An important next step is to examine the suitability of the CECPAQ in other target groups (e.g., low-educated parents, clinical samples).

In conclusion, the CECPAQ seems successful in assessing parental perceptions regarding five parenting domains, for mothers and fathers of 1–4 year-olds: Support, Stimulation, Structure, Harsh and Positive Discipline. Although not exhaustive, these parenting domains are focal in a range of theoretical models explaining child behavioral and developmental problems and are therefore highly relevant for researchers and clinicians working with young children.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  • Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parenting stress index (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.
  • Anthony, L. G., Anthony, B. J., Glanville, D. N., Naiman, D. Q., Waanders, C., & Shaffer, S. (2005). The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behaviour and preschoolers’ social competence and behaviour problems in the classroom. Infant and Child Development, 14, 133–154.10.1002/(ISSN)1522-7219
  • Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2005). The role of parenting styles in children’s problem behavior. Child Development, 76, 1144–1159. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00841.x
  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67, 3296–3319. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.ep9706244861
  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratlavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
  • Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83–96.10.2307/1129836
  • Boivin, M., P′erusse, D., Dionne, G., Saysset, V., Zoccolillo, M., Tarabulsy, G., … Tremblay, R. E. (2005). The genetic-environmental etiology of parents’ perceptions and selfassessed behaviors toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 612–630. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00375.x
  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic.
  • Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cognitions in the context of the family. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 315–344.10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.315
  • Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477–498. doi:10.1017/S095457940200305x
  • Cates, C. B., Dreyer, B. P., Berkule, S. B., White, L. J., Arevalo, J. A., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2012). Infant communication and subsequent language development in children from low-income families: The role of early cognitive stimulation. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33, 577–585. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e318264c10f
  • Choe, D. E., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2013). The interplay of externalizing problems and physical and inductive discipline during childhood. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2029–2039.10.1037/a0032054
  • Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218–232.10.1037/0003-066X.55.2.218
  • Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development, 77, 44–58.10.1111/cdev.2006.77.issue-1
  • De Brock, A. J. L. L., Vermulst, A. A., Gerris, J. R. M., & Abidin, R. R. (1992). Handleiding Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index. p. 1992. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Dos Santos, M. L., Dos Santos, N. D., Bastos, A. C., Assis, A. M., Prado, M. S., & Barreto, M. L. (2008). Determinants of early cognitive development: Hierarchical analysis of a longitudinal study. Cad Saude Publica, 24, 427–437.
  • Dreyer, B. P., Mendelsohn, A. L., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1996). Assessing the child’s cognitive home environment through parental report; reliability and validity. Early Development and Parenting, 5, 271–287.10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0917
  • Dreyer, B. P., Mendelsohn, A. L., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2001). StimQ-Toddler. New York: New York University School of Medicine.
  • Duppong Hurley, K., Huscroft-D’Angelo, J., Trout, A., Griffith, A., & Epstein, M. (2014). Assessing parenting skills and attitudes: A review of the psychometrics of parenting measures. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 812–823. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9733-2
  • Gerris, J. R. M., Van Boxtel, D. A. A. M., Vermulst, A. A., Janssens, J. M. A. M., Van Zutphen, R. A. H., & Felling, A. J. A. (1993). Parenting in Dutch families. Nijmegen: Institute of Family Studies, University of Nijmegen.
  • Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539–579.10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.539
  • Grolnick, W. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). Issues and challenges in studying parental control: Toward a new conceptualization. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 165–170. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00099.x
  • Holden, G. W. (2010). Parenting: A dynamic perspective. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Irvine, A. B., Biglan, A., Smolkowski, K., & Ary, D. V. (1999). The value of the parenting scale for measuring the discipline practices of parents of middle school children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 127–142.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00114-4
  • Jones, T. L., & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of parental self-efficacy in parent and child adjustment: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 341–363.10.1016/j.cpr.2004.12.004
  • Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2004). Parental discipline and externalizing behavior problems in early childhood: The roles of moral regulation and child gender. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 369–383. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000030291.72775.96
  • Kline, P. (2000). The handbook of psychological testing (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.
  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Laird, R. D., Shaw, D. S., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2011). Reciprocal relations between parents’ physical discipline and children’s externalizing behavior during middle childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 225–238. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000751
  • Lorber, M. F. (2012). The role of maternal emotion regulation in overreactive and lax discipline. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 642–647. doi:10.1037/a0029109
  • Lovejoy, M. C., Weis, R., O’Hare, E., & Rubin, E. C. (1999). Development and initial validation of the Parent Behavior Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 11, 534–545.10.1037/1040-3590.11.4.534
  • Lysenko, L. J., Barker, E. D., & Jaffee, S. R. (2013). Sex differences in the relationship between harsh discipline and conduct problems. Social Development, 22, 197–214. doi:10.1111/sode.12002
  • Mackler, J. S., Kelleher, R. T., Shanahan, L., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2015). Parenting stress, parental reactions, and externalizing behavior from ages 4 to 10. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 388–406.10.1111/jomf.12163
  • Miquelote, A. F., Santos, D. C. C., Caçola, P. M., Montebelo, M. I. L., & Gabbard, C. (2012). Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 329–334. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.002
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2011). Mplus (Version 6.11) [Computer software and manual]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
  • Passini, C. M., Pihet, S., & Favez, N. (2014). Assessing specific discipline techniques: A mixed-methods approach. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 1389–1402. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9796-0
  • Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (2001). The parenting styles and dimension questionnaire (PSDQ). In J. Touliatos & B. Perlmutter (Eds.), Handbook of family measurement techniques (2nd ed., pp. 319–321). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Rodrigues, L. P., Saraiva, L., & Gabbard, C. (2005). Development and structural validation of an inventory for assessing affordances in the home environment for motor development. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76, 140–148.10.1080/02701367.2005.10599276
  • Sanson, A. (1996). Child rearing questionnaire (Unpublished Manual). Melbourne: University of Melbourne.
  • Satorra, A. (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In R. D. H. Heijmans, D. S. G. Pollock, & A. Satorra (Eds.), Innovations in multivariate statistical analysis. A festschrift for Heinz Neudecker (pp. 233–247). London: Kluwer Academic.
  • Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis (Technical report). Los Angeles, CA: University of California.
  • Shelton, K. K., Frick, P. J., & Wootton, J. (1996). Assessment of parenting practices in families of elementary school-age children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 317–329.10.1207/s15374424jccp2503_8
  • Sleddens, E. F. C., O’Connor, T. M., Watson, K. B., Hughes, S. O., Power, T. G., Thijs, C., … Kremers, S. P. J. (2014). Development of the comprehensive general parenting questionnaire for caregivers of 5–13 year olds. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 11, 15–29.10.1186/1479-5868-11-15
  • Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. D. Osofosky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 462–516). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Stevens, J. P. (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Stormshak, E. A., Bierman, K. L., McMahon, R. J., & Lengua, L. J. (2000). Parenting practices and child disruptive behavior problems in early elementary school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 17–29.10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_3
  • Thepsuthammarat, K., Thinkhamrop, B., & Choprapawon, C. (2012). Association between types of play materials and cognitive development among 12-month-old Thai infants: The prospective cohort study of Thai children. Asian Biomedicine, 6, 703–711. doi:10.5372/1905-7415.0605.111
  • Tomopoulos, S., Dreyer, B. P., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Flynn, V., Rovira, I., Tineo, W., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2006). Books, toys, parent–child interaction, and development in young Latino children. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 6, 72–78. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2005.10.001
  • Verhoeven, M., Junger, M., Van Aken, C., Deković, M., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2010a). Parenting and children’s externalizing behavior: Bidirectionality during toddlerhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 93–105. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2009.09.002
  • Verhoeven, M., Junger, M., Van Aken, C., Deković, M., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2010). Mothering, fathering, and externalizing behavior in toddler boys. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 307–317. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00701.x
  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Thomas, R. (2010). Parents, parenting and toddler adaptation: Evidence from a national longitudinal study of Australian children. Infant Behavior & Development, 33, 518–529.