3,610
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The global problem of child maltreatment: perspectives on mechanisms of influence and illness presentation

, &
Pages 90-96 | Received 18 Jun 2016, Accepted 20 Nov 2016, Published online: 26 Dec 2016

ABSTRACT

Child abuse and neglect negatively impact both neurological and psychological development. Patterns of abuse are learned and repeated in families. Adverse childhood experiences are a risk factor for psychopathology later in life, including borderline personality disorder (BPD). Borderline personality disorder is prevalent in clinical populations in the USA, but its prevalence has not been well documented in most other parts of the world. The aim of this paper is to explore the impact of culture upon the intergenerational transmission of childhood maltreatment and the clinical presentation of abused children. To facilitate this exploration, we will consider the cases of four adolescent girls in unique socioeconomic and cultural settings around the world: Liberia, El Salvador, India and a Congolese immigrant in France. Each of these girls endorsed some features of BPD, but only two met full criteria. In societies in which externalizing behaviors are not acceptable, children may internalize their distress or separate from their families. Defining BPD in terms of internal experience makes it more difficult to identify, but it would allow for the inclusion of cases in which symptoms may manifest differently while the underlying problem is similar.

Background

Child abuse and neglect negatively impact both neurological and psychological development. However, the effects are highly variable and difficult to predict. In a recent meta-analysis, the global prevalence of child maltreatment was estimated at 22.6% (Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzendoorn, & Alink, Citation2013); the prevalence varied significantly between countries. One study compared reported rates of abuse and neglect in Kenya, Zambia and the Netherlands and found that abuse rates were much higher in the African samples, but rates of neglect were similar (Mbagaya, Oburu, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Citation2013).

While child maltreatment is a global problem, most of the research aimed at understanding and preventing it has been done in high-income countries. The aim of this paper is to explore the impact of culture on the clinical presentation of abused children. To facilitate this exploration, we will consider the cases of four adolescent girls from unique global settings. The girls had all experienced significant trauma themselves, and their families were also traumatized.

Intergenerational transmission of trauma

Research has consistently shown that patterns of abuse are learned and repeated in families (Berlin, Appleyard, & Dodge, Citation2011; Dayton, Huth-Bocks, & Busuito, Citation2015), as are patterns of attachment (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, Citation2015). A cycle has been described whereby women who experienced insufficient parental care, and, in some cases, abuse, become pregnant. They have doubts about their ability to parent as a result of the parenting they received. Without a healthy model, they struggle with setting limits, afraid of being too harsh, and then becoming excessively harsh when they perceive their own children as intrusive and even abusive. They also become resentful when asked to make sacrifices that were not made on their own behalf (Buist, Citation1998). As a result, they are more likely to abuse their own children and put them in situations that make them vulnerable to abuse by others (Buist, Citation1998).

Attachment problems are associated with behavioral disturbances and mental illness (Lyons-Ruth, Citation1996; Lyons-Ruth, Zoll, Connell, & Grunebaum, Citation1986). Mothers who have experienced trauma and developed a disorganized attachment style relate to their children in a manner that is alternately intrusive, fearful and disengaged. Without another attachment figure who is more consistent, these children go on to develop a disorganized attachment pattern themselves (Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, & Parsons, Citation1999). Disorganized attachment is associated with borderline personality disorder; it has been theorized that through the transmission of disorganized attachment, borderline personality is transmitted (Apter et al., Citation2016; Macfie & Swan, Citation2009). Studies of children in orphanages further support the significance of attachment in fostering mental health. These studies have found that without access to caring and consistent attachment figures in infancy, psychosocial, emotional and cognitive development are compromised (Bick et al., Citation2015; McLaughlin, Zeanah, Fox, & Nelson, Citation2012).

Clinical presentation

Adverse childhood experiences, including parental separation, unemployment and substance use, death of a parent or child being sent away, and physical or sexual abuse or neglect, have been linked with internalizing disorders like depression, as well as externalizing disorders like substance use, antisocial behavior and borderline personality disorder (BPD), later in life (Chapman et al., Citation2004; Schilling, Aseltine, & Gore, Citation2007). According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th Edition (DSM-V) (APA, Citation2013), BPD is characterized by impairments in self-functioning and interpersonal functioning, negative affective states and behavioral disinhibition. These criteria are detailed further in Appendix 1.

While trauma can be associated with a number of different clinical presentations, we will focus on BPD in order to consider the influence of culture upon symptom presentation. In comparison with other diagnoses, the rates of personality disorders vary significantly in different parts of the world. Responsible for a significant proportion of morbidity in the clinical population in the USA, BPD is rarely diagnosed elsewhere, particularly outside North America and Europe. One theory is that non-Western societies have close knit and hierarchical family structures that tend to repress the kind of impulsive emotional expression seen in BPD (Paris, Citation1996). Also, in societies that place a high value on social cohesion, the antisocial behaviors characteristic of antisocial personality disorder, and self-harm, a cardinal symptom of BPD, are rarely brought to clinical attention. Case reports of BPD developing following immigration to the USA lend further support to this theory (Paris, Citation1996).

If BPD is a problem of mis-attunement with a primary caregiver, it may be that in these societies children do not have the same expectations of individual recognition and validation from caregivers, so they are not impacted in the same way when it is lacking. It is also possible that the core issues that underlie BPD are present in populations around the world but that the specific presentation is influenced by the cultural context. In the Gusii tribe in Kenya, mothers have shorter moments of face-to-face contact with their infants (Dixon, Tronick, Keefer, & Brazelton, Citation2014). They avert their gaze in affectively charged moments, which children learn to do themselves. This is one example of a culturally-specific norm around the communication of affect. It follows that problems with affect regulation would present differently in this context than they might in another setting.

Case examples

In order to illustrate these patterns, we will consider the cases of adolescent girls from four distinct cultural and economic settings around the world. We will describe their clinical presentations and any features of BPD that they exhibited. All minors gave their assent to be interviewed, and consent was obtained from their legal guardians.

Street child in low-income country

Between 1989 and 2003, Liberia experienced a brutal civil war characterized by ethnic killings, sexual violence and the use of child soldiers. The war displaced a huge proportion of the population, destroyed the productive capacity and physical infrastructure of the country and eroded family and community ties (UNDP, Citation2006). In 2008, five years after the war ended, the population was approximately 3.5 million people; there were an estimated 340,000 orphans in Liberia, 18% of the total child population of the country (Liberia Institute of Statistics and Geo-Information Services, Citation2009). It is estimated that nearly 6000 more children were orphaned by the Ebola epidemic from 2014–2015 (Collins, Citation2015). Many of these orphans live on the street. Some children who were not orphaned also left home after the war.

Case example 1:

L is an18-year-old girl who had her first sexual experience at 10 years old when she was raped by a group of soldiers during the war. ‘Nothing happened to [the soldiers]’, she explained, intimating that the rape resulted in no consequences for them but severe ones for her. After the war ended, L left home and joined a group of children who were living on the street, using drugs and engaging in survival sex. Her parents urged her to come home, but she was not willing to live by their rules, which had failed to keep her safe. She did not express anger toward them but rather a sense of distance and disconnection. She no longer belonged at home, but she felt stuck in the life she was living because she had become addicted to drugs. She reported emotional lability, depression, hopelessness and risky behavior.

Institutionalized children in middle-income countries

El Salvador experienced a long, brutal civil war characterized by brothers killing brothers. A children’s home on the outskirts of San Salvador took in orphans and children whose families were unable to care for them. These children had early experiences with parents who spent their childhoods in the midst of war. In addition to devastating economic consequences of war, the family ties that supported caregiving of the very young and old and disabled were disrupted. Most of the parents of these children, as well as the staff at the children’s home, witnessed violence and death, and grew up under threat of betrayal and danger. Traditional beliefs and values that gave life meaning and predictability were diminished.

Case example 2:

S is a 13-year-old girl, the third child of a single woman sex worker. When she was an infant, neighbors heard her cries and saved her from drowning in a ditch. Rescuers discovered that her 5-year-old sister was the primary caregiver for S and her 2-year-old brother. When she was 6 months old, S and her siblings were placed in a small children’s home, where she was provided with a safe environment, good nutrition and affectionate caregiving. Her mother has never visited the home and is not present in the children’s lives. Early on, S’s development flourished, and she grew to be competent and assertive, though private. At 1 year old, she demonstrated initiative and persistence in her efforts to feed herself with a spoon and in learning to walk. Years later, she was described as “unmotivated” in school. She withdrew into angry isolation when she perceived a situation to be unfair. In her relationships with adults, she was tentative, as if afraid to make a commitment that could expose her to rejection or abandonment. Her only consistent trusting adult relationship was with the woman who directed the home and who had cared for her since infancy. With the other children in the home, she was a provocateur, stirring up trouble and then disavowing her involvement.

Children in an orphanage in south India came from lower-caste families, most were untouchables. In a vast country of more than a billion people, with many different languages spoken and religions practiced, the remnants of the ancient caste system have left a legacy of poverty, lack of opportunity, low self-esteem and anger directed inward and within the family. Particularly in poor, rural settings, families are very traditional, with the father as the unquestioned authority. Violence against women and children is common. Within these families, there was a narrative that they could not manage to take care of themselves and their children and participate in society. In some cases, one or both parents were living but were unable to care for their children. Their early experiences were characterized by extreme poverty, exposure to domestic violence and in some cases physical and sexual abuse. Interviews with these children indicate that those who developed externalizing behaviors were exposed to maltreatment, but most children who were maltreated did not develop these behaviors. There is some anecdotal evidence to suggest that children who experienced a long history of inconsistent parenting were more likely to externalize than those who came to the orphanage following a single dramatic loss. For those who did externalize, a common behavior was running away.

Case example 3:

N is a 17-year-old girl who grew up with a father who was verbally and physically abusive; her mother died when she was young. He sent her to the street to beg. She often felt humiliated and continued to relive these experiences years later. When she was 11 years old, a tourist found N and brought her to the home. She continued to sponsor her but had not visited in four years because she wanted N to learn to be more ‘self-reliant’. N spoke of this woman frequently. N used to run away from the home after her visits, but she had not done this for a few years. Her father had started visiting and bringing gifts in an attempt to repair their relationship. N accepted his gifts but was more interested in contact with the tourist and with her brother, who had married and moved away. She presented with impairments in self-functioning and interpersonal functioning, multiple features of negative affectivity and hostility.

Child from a low-income country who immigrated to a high-income country

The Democratic Republic of Congo (DRC) has been in a state of civil war for the past 20 years, punctuated by brief, tenuous periods of peace. Many Congolese have fled to neighboring countries, and some have made it to France. Families have left behind their experiences of war and violence, as well as extended family, friends and a familiar cultural context, in the hope of finding a safer place that would also offer greater opportunities.

Case example 4:

C is a 16-year-old girl from the DRC. She was raped several years ago and then entered into a forced marriage. She fled the DRC by herself, and about a year ago she arrived in France, where she was picked up as an unaccompanied minor with no legal immigration status. She was put in a home where she started to receive help and schooling. In this safe and contained environment, she became increasingly delusional. She believed that she smelled like feces and was physically rotten. She was also responding to internal stimuli and wandering off alone at night. She was then psychiatrically hospitalized, and she began sharing the story of her traumatic past. The intensity of her psychotic symptoms began to dissipate. Her presentation was characterized by impairment in identity functioning, negative affectivity and risk-taking.

Discussion

All of the cases involved a separation between the generations, either initiated by the parents or the child. It may be that a wish to avoid overt hostility was what pulled for separation in these cases. In societies in which externalizing behaviors are not acceptable, children may internalize their distress or separate from their families. The separation serves the dual functions of separating from the source of trauma and avoiding reminders of what was lost, allowing hostile feelings to be more easily hidden.

Each of these girls endorsed some features of BPD. Two cases in particular demonstrated overt hostility, characterized by hypersensitivity to perceived interpersonal slights, and thus displayed a full range of BPD symptoms. S, in the Salvadoran children’s home, and N, in the Indian children’s home, were observed to be manifestly angry. S’s anger was seen occasionally in her provocative behavior with other children, though it usually stayed hidden behind her mask of self-isolation. N had also been in a safe environment for a sustained period of time, but she had ongoing contact with her father, who had been abusive. Her sociocultural context was unique in that her country had not recently experienced war. All of these factors together may explain why N had the most access to her anger and was able to express it.

The varied presentations of these girls, particularly with respect to the expression of negative affect, raise the question of how to define BPD, which has been a source of controversy. While the DSM-V has favored a definition based upon symptom description (APA, Citation2013), some experts have suggested instead that the definition be rooted in internal psychological process (Higgitt & Fonagy, Citation1992). These experts highlight the profound feeling of emptiness and lack of sense of self that these patients report (Higgitt & Fonagy, Citation1992; Kohut, Citation1971). A number of theories have been proposed as to how this problem develops. Kohut believed that BPD resulted from a deficit in developing a sense of self due to the lack of a reflective function provided in the primary caregiving relationship (Kohut, Citation1971). Kernberg and others have emphasized the intensity of aggressive feelings that the weak self struggles to manage (Bion, Citation1957; Kernberg, Citation1975; Klein, Citation1950; Segal, Citation1964). Fonagy and colleagues have further elaborated this idea, incorporating both conflict and deficit models. They suggest that the caregivers also harbor aggressive impulses toward the child, and the child’s awareness of this leads her to inhibit the development of a capacity to think about the mental state of others, in order to avoid any awareness of the caregiver’s hostility or disinterest (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, Citation2002; Higgitt & Fonagy, Citation1992).

Defining BPD in terms of internal experience makes it more difficult to identify, but it allows for the inclusion of cases in which symptoms may manifest differently while the underlying problem is similar. This is particularly important for the purpose of understanding patients who come from distinct cultural backgrounds because culture shapes behavior and thus influences symptom presentation. Further study of the ways in which borderline psychopathology is impacted by culture and the ways it remains consistent across cultures is needed. This has the potential to inform the treatment of severely traumatized patients and reduce stigma associated with BPD and other trauma-related diagnoses in settings around the world

Notes on contributors

E.J. Levey, MD is a Global Psychiatry Research Fellow in the Department of Psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital and an Instructor of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School. She is Adjunct Faculty at the University of Illinois at Chicago, where she directs the Pediatric Stress and Anxiety Disorders Clinic.

Gisele Apter, MD, PhD is the Director of Perinatal Psychiatry at Erasmus Hospital. She is Assistant Professor of Psychiatry at the University of Paris Denis Diderot.

Alexandra Harrison, MD is a Training and Supervising Analyst at the Boston Psychoanalytic Society and Institute in Adult and Child and Adolescent Psychoanalysis, an Assistant Clinical Professor of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School at the Cambridge Health Alliance, and on the Faculty of the Infant-Parent Mental Health Post Graduate Certificate Program at University of Massachusetts Boston.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [grant number 5T32MH093310-05].

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (2015). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. New York: Psychology Press.
  • APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Apter, G., Devouche, E., Garez, V. Valente, M., Genet, M. C., Gratier, M., … Tronick, E. (2016). The still-face: A greater challenge for infants of mothers with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord, 1–14. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2016_30_243
  • Berlin, L. J., Appleyard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (2011). Intergenerational continuity in child maltreatment: Mediating mechanisms and implications for prevention. Child Dev, 82(1), 162–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01547.x
  • Bick, J., Zhu, T., Stamoulis, C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr, 169(3), 211–219. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212
  • Bion, W. R. (1957). Differentiation of the psychotic from the non-psychotic personalities. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 38, 266–275.
  • Buist, A. (1998). Childhood abuse, postpartum depression and parenting difficulties: A literature review of associations. Aust N Z J Psychiatry, 32(3), 370–378. doi: 10.3109/00048679809065529
  • Chapman, D. P., Whitfield, C. L., Felitti, V. J., Dube, S. R., Edwards, V. J., & Anda, R. F. (2004). Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J Affect Disord, 82(2), 217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.013
  • Collins, P. (2015). What happened to Liberia's Ebola Orphans?. IRIN News 2016.
  • Dayton, C. J., Huth-Bocks, A. C., & Busuito, A. (2015). The influence of interpersonal aggression on maternal perceptions of infant emotions: Associations with early parenting quality. Emotion.
  • Dixon, S., Tronick, E., Keefer, C., & Brazelton, T. B. (2014). Mother-infant interaction among the Gusii of Kenya. Culture and Early Interaction. T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek, P. Vietze and P. H. Liederman. New York, Psychology Press: 149-168.
  • Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.
  • Higgitt, A., & Fonagy, P. (1992). Psychotherapy in borderline and narcissistic personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry, 161, 23–43. doi: 10.1192/bjp.161.1.23
  • Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York: Jason Aronson.
  • Klein, M. (1950). Contributions to psychoanalysis, 1921–1945. London: Hogarth Press.
  • Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
  • Liberia Institute of Statistics and Geo-Information Services. (2009). 2008 Population and housing census.
  • Lyons-Ruth, K. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. J Consult Clin Psychol, 64(1), 64–73. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.64
  • Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons, E. (1999). Atypical attachment in infancy and early childhood among children at developmental risk. IV. Maternal frightened, frightening, or atypical behavior and disorganized infant attachment patterns. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev, 64(3), 67–96; discussion 213-220. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00034
  • Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D., & Grunebaum, H. U. (1986). The depressed mother and her one-year-old infant: environment, interaction, attachment, and infant development. New Dir Child Dev, 1986(34), 61–82. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219863407
  • Macfie, J., & Swan, S. A. (2009). Representations of the caregiver-child relationship and of the self, and emotion regulation in the narrative of young children whose mothers have borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 21(3), 993–1011. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000534
  • Mbagaya, C., Oburu, P., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2013). Child physical abuse and neglect in Kenya, Zambia and the Netherlands: A cross-cultural comparison of prevalence, psychopathological sequelae and mediation by PTSS. Int J Psychol, 48(2), 95–107. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.691975
  • McLaughlin, K. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Attachment security as a mechanism linking foster care placement to improved mental health outcomes in previously institutionalized children. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines, 53(1), 46–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02437.x
  • Paris, J. (1996). Cultural factors in the emergence of borderline pathology. Psychiatry, 59(2), 185–192. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1996.11024760
  • Schilling, E. A., Aseltine, R. H. Jr., & Gore, S. (2007). Adverse childhood experiences and mental health in young adults: A longitudinal survey. BMC Public Health, 7, 164. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-30
  • Segal, H. (1964). Introduction to the work of Melaine Klein. New York: Basic Books.
  • Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Alink, L. R. (2013). Cultural-geographical differences in the occurrence of child physical abuse? A meta-analysis of global prevalence. International journal of psychology : Journal international de psychologie, 48(2), 81–94. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.697165
  • UNDP. (2006). National human development report of Liberia, United Nations Development Programme (UNDP).

Appendix 1. DSM V Criteria for Borderline Personality Disorder (APA 2013)

A pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, and affects, and marked impulsivity, beginning in early adulthood and present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by five (or more) of the following:

  1. Frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment

  2. A pattern of unstable and intense interpersonal relationships characterized by alternating between extremes of idealization and devaluation

  3. Identity disturbance characterized by markedly and persistently unstable self-image or sense of self

  4. Impulsivity in at least two areas that are potentially self-damaging (e.g., spending, sex, substance abuse, reckless driving, binge eating)

  5. Recurrent suicidal behavior, gestures, or threats, or self-mutilating behavior

  6. Affective instability due to a marked reactivity of mood (e.g., intense episodic dysphoria, irritability, or anxiety usually lasting a few hours and only rarely more than a few days)

  7. Chronic feelings of emptiness

  8. Inappropriate, intense anger or difficulty controlling anger (e.g., frequent displays of temper, constant anger, recurrent physical fights)

  9. Transient, stress-related paranoid ideation or severe dissociative symptoms