1,083
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
EDUCATIONAL ASSESSMENT & EVALUATION

Spanish adaptation of the Math and Me Survey in primary education: Measuring second and fourth graders’ attitudes toward mathematics

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Article: 2204707 | Received 29 Aug 2022, Accepted 16 Apr 2023, Published online: 25 Apr 2023

Abstract

The English version of the “Math and Me Survey” was developed to measure mathematical attitudes of elementary students. This study was used to translate, validate and examine the attitudes of Spanish students in the second and fourth grades of primary school. The translation-back translation procedure was adopted. A total of 81 students (42 boys and 39 girls), with a mean age of 8.19 years completed the Spanish survey. The psychometric properties of the adapted survey were examined by internal consistency, reliability and factorial structure. The exploratory factor analysis of the 18-item adapted questionnaire provided a two-factor structure, similar to the original questionnaire: mathematical self-concept and enjoyment of mathematics. For each factor, the Spanish survey had high internal consistency, respectively ω = .89 and ω =.91. However, the confirmatory factor analysis did not show overall good fit. The adapted survey should be interpreted with caution, suggesting a need for further investigation to be used as an effective tool for evaluating math self-concept and enjoyment in a Spanish-speaking context. In addition, the results of its application indicated that (1) second-graders had a better mathematics self-concept and enjoy mathematics more than fourth-graders, and (2) boys had a greater mathematics self-concept than girls, but girls enjoyed this subject more in second grade.

1. Introduction

The study of students’ mathematical attitudes has aroused interest in educational research due to its relevance in the teaching-learning process (Adelson & McCoach, Citation2010). For several decades, the importance of beliefs about and attitudes towards the learning of mathematics has deepened since they may affect students’ behavior and educational outcomes (A. Dowker et al., Citation2019; Adelson & McCoach, Citation2010; Cvencek et al., Citation2020; Hannula et al., Citation2016). Different studies (M. F. Del Río et al., Citation2019) have shown that it is possible to study students’ attitudes toward different subjects such as mathematics and therefore the behavior they manifest in relation to them. Notably, these attitudes can be conditioned by students’ budgets and personal experiences regarding the subjects and can affect their performance and learning (Areepattamannil, Citation2012; M. Del Río et al., Citation2016). As Bandura (Citation2006) posited learning is affected by the students’ beliefs in their abilities to perform a specific task (self-efficacy), and these determine their behavior and cognition (Bandura, Citation1997). In fact, children demonstrate different levels of emotional, social and cognitive engagement in school (Bong & Skaalvik, Citation2003), and they are in need of successful experiences to develop academic positive perceptions (self-concept). While self-efficacy represents children’s expectations of what they can successfully accomplish, self-concept represents children’s knowledge and perceptions of the self (Bong & Skaalvik, Citation2003; Holenstein et al., Citation2021). Academic self-concept is influenced by the result of social comparison and is related to how children feel about themselves toward a given domain (for an overview, see Bong & Skaalvik, Citation2003). Students who have a positive self-perception are more likely to perform better (Vasalampi et al., Citation2020), thus the importance of providing early positive experiences in areas such as mathematics.

In the first years of school, boys and girls can form an idea about different subjects and know if they like them or not. For this reason, it is foreseeable that as students progress through a course, they will adopt an attitude toward it, creating a self-concept and displaying their pleasure or enjoyment in relation to the subject (Eccles et al., Citation1993). They also begin to create their own perceptions, ideas and beliefs about mathematics, which can influence and even predict their academic performance (Mazana et al., Citation2019; Williams & Williams, Citation2010). Although students’ attitudes about mathematics develop over time (Ma & Kishor, Citation1997), some studies document the importance of the role of children’s early experiences such as parents’ attitudes towards mathematics (Aunola et al., Citation2013; Mohr-Schroeder et al., Citation2017) and how these may even improve students’ attitudes toward mathematics (Rowan-Kenyon et al., Citation2012; Sheldon & Epstein, Citation2005) which can increase student’s interest (Aunola et al., Citation2013) and achievement in mathematics (Lipnevich et al., Citation2016; Mohr-Schroeder et al., Citation2017). Therefore, parents can also become a positive influence on their children’s attitude, which is regarded as a key contributor to the performance in mathematics (Mazana et al., Citation2019).

On the whole, studies suggest that children’s mathematics beliefs have an impact on performance on cognitive and affective levels and the development of positive perceptions are related to successful learning (Marsh & O’Mara, Citation2008; OECD, Citation2013) as they are associated with academic performance (Geddes et al., Citation2010). Moreover, children’s emotions influence both the learning process and academic performance in which the importance of affective-emotional aspects and an interest in mathematics is evident (Hidalgo et al., Citation2012; Sorvo et al., Citation2022). In addition, the relevance of the effect of emotions in the teaching-learning process should be taken into account (Hidalgo et al., Citation2012) since they have the ability to determine the success and/or failure of the student as well as their academic performance in this subject (D. Dowker et al., Citation2012).

Research on the attitudes of boys and girls toward mathematics is considerable (A. Dowker et al., Citation2019; Adelson & McCoach, Citation2011; Asika, Citation2021; Cvencek et al., Citation2015, Citation2020; D. Dowker et al., Citation2012; Hidalgo et al., Citation2012; Paz-Albo et al., Citation2017; Viljaranta et al., Citation2014) and allows us to identify the elements that condition these attitudes and thus to explore the ways in which they can be modified to improve academic performance (for intervention development that boost children’s beliefs about math, see Cvencek et al., Citation2015). Mathematics attitudes are far more malleable than cognitive ability characteristics (Lipnevich et al., Citation2016), and therefore, for the educational field, the possibility of modifying students’ attitudes in the early years toward subjects is of great interest. Authors such as Hidalgo et al. (Citation2012) state that there is a real rejection by some students of mathematics and suggest that it is due to the influence of cognitive and emotional variables since students who understand and handle mathematics with some ease affirm that it is easy and fun (2012).

In line with the above, studies have shown that the development of positive emotions and beliefs in students promotes attitudes of approaching mathematics, and these perceptions of their mathematical abilities (mathematics self-concept) play an important role in academic performance (M. Del Río et al., Citation2016). Likewise, other international measures, such as the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), assess the level of self-concept as a key factor in the results of standardized tests; that is, the better a student’s self-concept is, the better their performance in mathematics is (Sáenz, Citation2007 Servicio de Ordenación Académica y Evaluación Educativa [SOAEE], Citation2019). Although the results by Sáenz (Citation2007) do not show a significant relationship between motivation and performance in mathematics, other studies (SOAEE, Citation2019) affirm that greater motivation correlates with better results on skills tests.

Different studies have focused on mathematics not only due to its compulsory nature in primary and secondary education but also because it is usually the subject that poses the most difficulties for students, although it is among the subjects that contribute the most important skills and knowledge, both for the study of other subjects and for success in life (Martínez-Artero & Nortes, Citation2013). Moreover, mathematics has an instrumental character since it is the basis for acquiring new knowledge in other subjects and its practice develops in the child an interest in research, creativity or even the ability to apply mathematical reasoning in everyday situations.

On the other hand, a key aspect of changing an attitude is to first know the assessment that the subject makes of the dimensions that compose it. In this case, it is necessary to know primary school students’ evaluation at both the cognitive level (mathematics self-concept) and the emotional level (the enjoyment of mathematics) with the aim of being able to modify this evaluation, as long as it is necessary, and thus influencing students’ behavior. Most studies have focused on mathematics anxiety, but as A. Dowker et al. (Citation2019) suggest other attitudes such as self-confidence and enjoyment are also important to study since they are usually positively related to performance. However, if there are instruments to assess these mathematics attitudes in secondary school children (see Adelson & McCoach, Citation2011 for a detailed description), instruments to assess primary school children’s attitudes to mathematics in a Spanish-speaking context are scarce since there is a lack of suitable scales (see Guzmán et al., Citation2021 for a review of scales for young children in the English-speaking context). Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011) validated the “Math and Me Survey” instrument, designed for third- to sixth-grade students, identifying two dimensions: the enjoyment of mathematics and mathematical self-perceptions. This 5-point Likert-type scale has reported a good level of reliability (see Adelson & McCoach, Citation2011) and uses a vocabulary that is understandable by second-grade children, making the scale more accessible to younger children. Therefore, the “Math and Me Survey” questionnaire originally developed by Adelson (Citation2006) was adapted to Spanish to assess primary school children’s beliefs about their abilities to perform well in mathematics (self-perceptions) and the extent to which they enjoy doing and learning mathematics (enjoyment).

Taking into account all the above, the specific objectives of this study were (a) to examine the properties of the adapted Spanish version of the “Math and Me Survey” questionnaire (Adelson, Citation2006) for use with Spanish children; (b) to describe and analyze mathematics self-concept and the enjoyment of mathematics in a sample of Spanish second- and fourth-grade children; and (c) to explore the role of gender and age in the relationship between self-concept and enjoyment. Moreover, based on previous research on the psychometric properties of the “Math and Me Survey”, it is hypothesized that the Spanish version will also have a two-factor solution as well as the measured variables influenced by the same factor proposed by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011). We also hypothesize that there will be significant differences in mathematics self-concept and enjoyment of mathematics between boys and girls. We expect the boys to identify with math more strongly (A. Dowker et al., Citation2019; Cvencek et al., Citation2011, Citation2015; D. Dowker et al., Citation2012; M. F. Del Río et al., Citation2019) and younger children to enjoy learning math more.

2. Method

The adaptation process was based on the method of translation and back translation by professionals and a pilot study. In addition, an exploratory and descriptive quantitative methodology is used to deepen understanding of the attitudes of primary school students toward mathematics.

2.1. Participants

This study included 81 students from a primary school aged between seven and ten years, (M = 8.19; SD = 1.07) and was conducted during the second semester of the 2018–2019 school year. Of the 81 participants, 36 were in second grade (44.4%), and 45 were in fourth grade (55.6%). The sample is summarized in Table .

Table 1. Sample data by class and course

The sample design was nonprobabilistic and accidental (Otzen & Manterola, Citation2017). To access the sample, first, a co-ed educational center was invited to collaborate in this study to obtain a balanced sample, nearly equally-sized subgroups. The parents of all participants provided written informed consent.

2.2. Instrument

As a measurement instrument for this research, an adaptation of the “Math and Me Survey” designed by Adelson (Citation2006) was developed to measure the attitudes toward mathematics of primary school students. The translated questionnaire “Las Mates y Yo” (Math and Me) consists of 18 items that measure mathematics self-concept (8 items) and enjoyment of mathematics (10 items) on a five-point Likert scale, in which 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = agree and 5 = strongly agree. This scale was chosen because the reliability of a questionnaire is greater than when using a four-point scale, and the scale is suitable for primary school students (Adelson & McCoach, Citation2010).

The original version was translated and culturally adapted to ensure the semantic and conceptual equivalence of the new questionnaire (Carvajal et al., Citation2011). This process included a method that is considered to be of the highest quality, namely, translation-back translation with bilingual speakers (Carvajal et al., Citation2011) in a pilot study. In addition, the questionnaire was evaluated by an interdisciplinary team of experts in translation, bilingual education, psychology and linguistics. The final version in Spanish was adapted during the administration of the pilot study, and the modifications were implemented in agreement with the members of the evaluation team.

2.3. Procedure

After obtaining approval for the research from the Research Ethics Committee of the Rey Juan Carlos University (internal registration number 2808201810018), the participating school was informed about the protocol they had to follow when administering the instrument. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents or guardians on behalf of the participant children and teachers administered the questionnaire in their classrooms to ensure that they followed the same protocol and that the variables of interest were controlled as much as possible.

2.4. Data analysis

To analyze the data, in addition to a descriptive analysis, factor analysis was used to identify latent factors. The exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to test the dimensionality and contrast the structure of the translated scale with that of the original instrument using IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh (Version 28.0.). Confirmatory analysis (CFA) was computed using AMOS to test the measurement model. McDonald’s omega was calculated to ensure the internal consistency of the instrument. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to make comparisons among the classroom groups, and the Students’ t-test was used to compare the means of the second- and fourth-grade children groups and to determine whether gender differences existed by grade level.

3. Results

3.1. Factor analysis

The factor analysis was performed to test the dimensionality of the instrument using the maximum likelihood method (ML) since it allows a wide range of fit indices (Goretzko et al., Citation2021). Inspection of the strength of the relationship among the questionnaire items in the correlation matrix revealed the presence of many coefficients of .3 and above (as recommended by Tabachnick & Fidell, Citation2011), and the factorability of the data was assessed using Bartlett’s test of sphericity, and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy. Bartlett’s test of sphericity reached statistical significance (χ2 = 976.133; gl = 153; p < .001), and the KMO value was .851, exceeding the recommended value of .6, supporting the factorability of the correlation matrix (Pallant, Citation2016; Tabachnick & Fidell, Citation2011).

To determine the numbers of factors to extract, we considered the Kaiser-Guttman rule, the interpretation of the elbow of the Scree plot and the results from the parallel analysis (PA). Using Kaiser’s criterion, the first four factors recorded eigenvalues above 1 (8.023, 2.515, 1.216, 1.048), explaining 44.57%, 13.97%, 6.76% and 5.82% of the variance respectively. An inspection of the Scree plot revealed a clear break after the second factor so, as suggested by Pallant (Citation2016), only the first two factors were retained since they also capture much more of the variance accounted for. Additionally, we also conducted a PA, using the online tool by Patil et al. (Citation2017), since it is among the most accurate methods in determining the number of factors to retain (Fabrigar & Wegener, Citation2011; Finch, Citation2020; Hayton et al., Citation2004). Considering the results of the PA, which agreed with the interpretation of the Scree plot, and the variance explained by the Kaiser-Guttman rule, we decided to extract two factors. The two-factor model explained a total of 58.54% of the variance, with Factor 1 contributing 44.57% and Factor 2 contributing 13.97%.

The pattern matrix of the ML solution with oblimin rotation, the structure matrix, and the extraction communalities for the 18 items of the “Las Mates y Yo” questionnaire are in Table . Initial communalities for all the items are above .30 (Table ) and the rotated structure sorted the measured variables into the same factors of the original “Math and Me Survey” instrument proposed by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011). Both factors show a number of strong loadings with 10 items loading above .3 on Factor 1, which seems to index “enjoyment of mathematics” and 8 items loading above .4 on Factor 2, which seems to index “mathematics self-concept”, and have a moderate correlation (.447).

Table 2. “Las Mates y Yo” questionnaire items, their standardized factor pattern and structure coefficients, and their communalities

As the EFA suggested a two-factor solution, as described above, a two-factor CFA was performed using maximum likelihood estimation to test the model with procedures of computing goodness-of-fit coefficients: the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI).

The two latent variables represent the same two factors of the originally “Math and Me Survey” proposed by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011). After estimating the model fit, goodness-of-fit statistics showed that the two-factor model, which had a χ2 of 287.48 and df of 134 (p < .001), did not exhibit good fit. With this model, the χ2/df ratio (2.145) was greater than 2.0. Both the CFI (.832) and TLI (.808) did not meet the ideal cutoff value of being above .95 (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999; Marsh et al., Citation2004). Finally, the SRMR (.088) was above the cutoff value of .08 (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999), typically used as an indicator of good model fit, and the RMSEA (.120) was noticeably greater than the ideal cutoff value of <.06 suggested by Hu and Bentler (Citation1999).

3.2. Reliability

To estimate the reliability of the scores for each of the two factors of the “Las Mates y Yo” instrument, internal consistency was calculated using McDonald’s omega since it is a more suitable measure for applied research, as suggested in the current methodological literature (Hayes & Coutts, Citation2020). We reverse scored Items 4, 7, 9, and 13 and used the recoded items in the reliability analysis. The resulting McDonald’s omega values can be interpreted as a good internal reliability since estimates were>0.8 for the globality of the questionnaire (ω = .917), and its both dimensions: enjoyment of mathematics (ω = .912) with 10 items, and mathematics self-concept (ω =.886) with 8 items. These coefficients assume that the items measure the same construct and resemble the results obtained by Hilton (Citation2018) and Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011). Likewise, it was evident that there would be no significant improvements if any items were eliminated.

3.3. Attitudes toward mathematics

The analysis of the data collected shows that second- and fourth-grade students had a positive attitude toward mathematics in two dimensions: self-concept and enjoyment. Notably, as shown in Table , the attitudes of second-grade students toward mathematics are higher in both dimensions. However, comparing the differences between the two courses shows that they are only statistically significant in the enjoyment dimension [t (66.720) = 5.77, p < .001; d = 0.808] and in the total calculation of the questionnaire [t (75.706) = 4.09, p < .001; d = 0.730]. This difference found in the total score is most likely due to the difference observed in the enjoyment scale. The effect sizes for these analyses were found to exceed Cohen’s convention for medium (d = 0.5) and large effects (d = 0.8).

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of students’ attitudes by course

When segmenting the data by gender (Table ), there were no significant differences between boys and girls in second grade in any of the dimensions. It might be inferred, therefore, that there is no statistically significant difference between boys and girls in the enjoyment dimension [t (34) = 1.56, p = .127], the self-concept dimension [t (34) = 1.94, p = .061] and total computation [t (34) = 0.60, p = .552]. However, there were only a statistically significant difference in the mean self-concept dimension between boys and girls in fourth grade [t (43) = 2.67, p = .011; d = 0.812].

Table 4. Means and standard deviations of students’ attitudes by grade level and gender

As shown in Table , boys have a higher self-concept than girls, but this result is lower in fourth grade. On the other hand, second-grade girls enjoy mathematics more than fourth-grade girls, which indicates a statistically significant difference in scores [t (37) = 4.765, p < .001; d = 1.531], which was also observed in the overall calculation of the questionnaire [t (37) = 3.695, p = .001; d = 1.187].

4. Discussion and conclusion

This study sought first to determine whether the factorial structure of the Spanish adaptation of the “Math and Me Survey” by Adelson (Citation2006) was similar to that obtained in previous studies. Second, this study aimed to determine the mathematics attitudes related to self-concept and enjoyment in primary school students. Finally, it sought to assess possible differences in attitudes related to age and gender.

Regarding our first objective, the analysis showed that the exploratory factorial structure of the Spanish adaptation was similar to that of the questionnaire by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011). The EFA seemed to mainly favor a two-factor solution, which was shown to explain over 58% of the variance and the items are grouped into the same two factors or dimensional constructs, namely, mathematics self-concept and enjoyment of mathematics, as shown in Table . The interpretation of the two factors was consistent with the mathematics structure originally proposed by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011), and these two dimensions are aligned with the original instrument and are related to each other. In addition, the EFA results affirmed that the first objective was achieved since the 18 measured variables are influenced by the same two factors, supporting the original structure of the “Math and Me Survey”.

Confirmatory factor analysis was then performed using a two-factor model although none of the goodness-of-fit statistics showed overall good fit for the adapted instrument. As Marsh et al. (Citation2011, 2014) and Morin et al. (Citation2013) suggest the assumptions of CFA could be overly restrictive for many multidimensional instruments and therefore, the CFA measurement model neither fits with empirical data nor represent substantive theory realistically. Furthermore, based on Marsh et al. (Citation2014) research almost no multidimensional scales provide a good fit and they usually fail to meet standards of good measurement. This indicates a need for looking into the underlying factors impacting the structure because fit indices such as RMSEA might also be elevated due to relatively small sample sizes (Finch, Citation2020; Taasoobshirazi & Wang, Citation2016), typical of models tested in math and science education (Taasoobshirazi & Wang, Citation2016). However, some of the studies carried out by Adelson and McCoach (Citation2011) found that a two-factor structure of the original “Math and Me Survey” was a better fit for their data, but this used a larger sample size (N = 302), and the goodness-of-fit indices performed particularly well (see Adelson & McCoach, Citation2010 for their model fit indicators).

This study also showed the specific characteristics of attitudes toward mathematics in the population analyzed with respect to these two dimensions. Significant differences were observed in relation to the age of the students, and the scores on the self-concept and enjoyment dimensions are inversely proportional, that is, the younger the age is, the higher the scores are. This statistically significant correlation in the enjoyment dimension shows that the working hypothesis was in the correct direction. The results support previous studies with primary school children in suggesting that students’ interest and motivation in relation to mathematics declines with age as they find it uninteresting and unentertaining. In addition, the study done by the Spanish Association for Digitalization (Asociación Española para la Digitalización, Citation2019) corroborates that enjoyment of mathematics is related to a good perception of mathematics in primary education. However, these assessments change in secondary education and may be of interest to researchers in education and school counseling since it appears that mathematics interest, motivation, and enjoyment decreases with age (Adelson & McCoach, Citation2011; Gottfried et al., Citation2007; Hettinger et al., Citation2022).

In terms of the gender of the participants, the data obtained show that the means of the two dimensions are high but decrease over time in both groups. Although the scores indicate that both boys and girls have a high self-concept, there are differences between students participating in the same course. Boys seem to have a higher mathematics self-concept than girls, which becomes a statistically significant difference in fourth grade. Previous research has confirmed this gender differences in terms of self-concept in primary education (see Mejía-Rodríguez et al., Citation2021; Rodríguez et al., Citation2020; Vasalampi et al., Citation2020) and suggest that girls may be losing their motivation during this educational stage. D. Dowker et al. (Citation2012) also found that primary school boys rated themselves higher than girls. Similarly, other studies (Cvencek et al., Citation2020; Paz-Albo et al., Citation2017) also point to these gender differences, echoing the results of this study. However, these differences in mathematics self-concept may be the result of differences in how the instrument functions across boys and girls since this study did not examine latent differences between genders.

The self-perception of boys and girls varies as students move through grade levels, although these differences become more visible in secondary education, where the positive self-perception of girls toward mathematics decreases more compared to that of boys (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development [OECD, Citation2015). As M. Del Río et al. (Citation2016) show, this math-gender stereotype belief can produce differences in student treatment that increase learning gaps and different learning opportunities for boys and girls (M. Del Río et al., Citation2016). Moreover, as research suggest male students have usually higher self-concept values in math domains (Rodríguez et al., Citation2020; Saß & Kampa, Citation2019), and it may influence students’ math-related beliefs, academic performance, school course selection and career interntions (Asika, Citation2021; Han, Citation2019; Passolunghi et al., Citation2014; Saß & Kampa, Citation2019). Although research is inconclusive on the role of self-concept in learning mathematics (see Holenstein et al., Citation2021), it seems to have a crucial impact on future development and career awareness of primary school students. This may be also particularly useful for school counselors and primary school educators since they can administer the survey to identify bilingual Spanish students with low mathematics self-concept and strengthen their math-related self-concepts that could result in student’s increased interest and academic achievement over time.

Regarding the dimension of pleasure or enjoyment that the student experiences, the results indicate that girls in second grade enjoy mathematics the most but that there are significant differences in this dimension between girls in second and fourth grade. In addition, it seems that fourth-grade girls begin to enjoy mathematics less, and moreover, it is precisely in this course that boys begin to display enjoyment and greater confidence in their aptitudes. Similarly, previous studies (Mata et al., Citation2012) have shown this decline in attitudes for girls as they progress in school, although as Jacobs et al. (Citation2002) point out gender differences in children beliefs with age, particularly for math, including their enjoyment of mathematics. These results can also help school counselors and educators understand how this dimension can also influence students’ academic performance throughout primary education as well as their performance and choice of mathematics (Adelson & McCoach, Citation2011) in secondary education or other subjects related to STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics). Therefore, it is essential to consider not only children’s school-related experiences but also the influences of parents, teachers and peers who influence children’s beliefs and values (Mejía-Rodríguez et al., Citation2021) in addition to looking into how mathematics teaching and learning are negotiated in the classroom since the mathematics curriculum also becomes more demanding and it may provoke a fall in school children’s math-related attitudes and interest (Mata et al., Citation2012).

These results show that self-concept and enjoyment are relevant to engage in subjects such as mathematics; however, their lack can cause girls to lose interest in these disciplines (Rodríguez et al., Citation2020; Valero-Matas & Coca, Citation2021). In addition, the results indicate how self-beliefs and mathematical enjoyment can be determinants of not only behavior and motivation but also academic performance (Asika, Citation2021; Rodríguez et al., Citation2020). In fact, research (Pekrun et al., Citation2017) has shown that students who enjoy mathematics have better long-term math achievement.

In general, the results obtained in this study show that both age and gender are important variables for understanding differences in self-concept in relation to STEM disciplines, such as mathematics, as girls tend to have a lower self-concept than boys (Mejía-Rodríguez et al., Citation2021; Rodríguez et al., Citation2020; Vasalampi et al., Citation2020). This result underscores the importance of enjoying learning, mainly in girls, to engage in these subjects that present more challenges to students (Asociación Española para la Digitalización, Citation2019) and thus encourage interest in STEM subjects from an early age. These data demonstrate the same pattern of results presented in the report The ABC of Gender Equality by the OECD (Citation2015), although in countries with economies that have better performance on PISA, girls outnumber boys in subjects such as mathematics (OECD, Citation2015). In countries such as Spain, results from PISA (OECD, Citation2019) also seems to indicate that this gap is decreasing as girls’ performance in mathematics improves, although the results remain below the EU average.

Analysis of the data reveals a correlation between age and the dimensions of mathematics self-concept and enjoyment of mathematics, and these two dimensions are key determinants of academic performance. In addition, as suggested by Asika (Citation2021), self-concept is one of the predictors of academic performance and an essential pillar in the improvement of learning (OECD, Citation2015), so it may be desirable to focus in the early years on preserving and boosting students’ confidence in their ability in mathematics (A. Dowker et al., Citation2019).

In conclusion, the results suggest that the adapted Spanish version of the “Math and Me Survey” by Adelson (Citation2006) is equivalent to the original and can be used in Spanish-speaking contexts. Likewise, the results shed light on the existing relationship between the subject, in this case the student, and the attitudinal object, mathematics, as a key skill of the primary education curriculum. In addition, the results may contribute to understanding how mathematics self-concept and enjoyment of mathematics vary with gender and age, although causal relationships cannot be established due to the transversality of the study. Finally, longitudinal studies that reinforce the results are still pending so researchers can gain a better understanding of how these two dimensions might influence later STEM achievement and choice.

5. Limitations of the study and implications for further research

The reported work has several strengths but is not without limitations. First, we acknowledge that some caution is needed in drawing conclusions based on the analysis, as the sample size was relatively small and future studies should include a larger sample from a wider variety of schools as well. However, under some circumstances a sample of 100 -or even 50- cases are sufficient since it can also yield good quality results (de Winter et al., Citation2009), but more is better. Some authors also suggest that there should be a ratio of at least five cases for each item (see Pallant, Citation2016; Tabachnick & Fidell, Citation2011). Second, it would also be of interest further research should examine latent differences between genders (i.e., measurement invariance) and explore the extent to which the factor structure is represented in the same way for girls and boys. Additionally, the different results and its interpretation awaits clarification by more research.

Correction

This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.

Acknowledgments

This study is part of the work carried out during a research stay at the UIC Bilingualism Research Laboratory of the University of Illinois at Chicago in 2018. The authors would like to thank Luis López-Carretero, Director of the UIC Bilingualism Research, for his collaboration and Professor Jill L. Adelson of the University of Louisville for providing us with the “Math and Me Survey” questionnaire. We also thank the school principal, teachers, parents and students for their generous cooperation.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Notes on contributors

Jesús Paz-Albo

Jesús Paz-Albo is an associate professor at the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos and the coordinator of the Innovation and Educational Improvement Research Group since 2020. He served as the director of the Master’s Degree in Leadership and Management of Educational Centers from 2013 to 2018. His research interests focus on child development, educational technologies, teacher education and bilingual education.

Aránzazu Hervás-Escobar

Aránzazu Hervás-Escobar is an assistant professor at the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos and the coordinator of the AVANTE Innovation Group. Her research focuses on neuropsychology, educational psychology, and teacher education.

References

  • Adelson, J. L. (2006). Math and Me Survey. Unpublished instrument.
  • Adelson, J. L., & McCoach, D. B. (2010). Measuring the mathematical attitudes of elementary students: The effects of a 4-point or 5-point Likert-type scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70(5), 796–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164410366694
  • Adelson, J. L., & McCoach, D. B. (2011). Development and psychometric properties of the math and me survey: Measuring third through sixth graders’ attitudes towards mathematics. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 44(4), 225–247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748175611418522
  • Areepattamannil, S. (2012). First- and second-generation immigrant adolescents’ multidimensional mathematics and science self-concepts and their achievement in mathematics and science. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 10(3), 695–716. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-011-9319-7
  • Asika, M. O. (2021). Self-concept, self-efficacy and self esteem as predictors of academic performance in mathematics among junior secondary school students in Edo state. Sumerianz Journal of Education, LInguisticas and Literature, 4(1), 15–22. https://doi.org/10.47752/sjell.41.15.22
  • Asociación Española para la Digitalización. (2019). El desafío de las vocaciones STEM. DigitalES. The challenge of STEM vocations.
  • Aunola, K., Viljaranta, J., Lehtinen, Nurmi, J. -E., & Lehtinen, E. (2013). The role of maternal support of competence, autonomy and relatedness in children’s interests and mastery orientation. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.002
  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company.
  • Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education (Vol. 5, pp. 307–337). Information Age Publishing.
  • Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different are they really? Educational Psychology Review, 15(1), 1–40. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021302408382
  • Carvajal, A., Centeno, C., Watson, R., Martínez, M., & Sanz, Á. (2011). ¿Cómo Validar un instrumento de medida de la salud? [How is an instrument for measuring health to be validated? Anales del sistema sanitario de Navarra, 34(1), 63–72. https://doi.org/10.4321/S1137-66272011000100007
  • Cvencek, D., Kapur, M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Math achievement, stereotypes, and math self-concepts among elementary-school students in Singapore. Learning and Instruction, 39, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.04.002
  • Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82(3), 766–779. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x
  • Cvencek, D., Paz-Albo, J., Master, A., Herranz, C. V., Hervás, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2020). Math is for me: A field intervention to strengthen math self-concepts in Spanish-speaking 3rd grade children. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. Article 593995. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.593995
  • Del Río, M. F., Strasser, K., Cvencek, D., Susperreguy, M. I., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2019). Chilean kindergarten children’s beliefs about mathematics: Family matters. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 687–702. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000658
  • Del Río, M., Strasser, K., & Susperreguy, M. (2016). ¿Son las habilidades matemáticas un asunto de Género? Los estereotipos de género acerca de las matemáticas en niños y niñas de Kínder, sus familias y educadoras [Is math ability a gender issue? Gender stereotypes about math in kindergarten children, their families and teachers]. Revista Calidad en la Educación, 45, 20–53. https://doi.org/10.31619/caledu.n45.14
  • de Winter, J. C., Dodou, D., & Wieringa, P. A.(2009). Exploratory factor analysis with small sample sizes. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 44(2), 147–181. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170902794206
  • Dowker, D., Bennett, K., & Smith, L. (2012). Attitudes to mathematics in primary school children. Child Development Research, 2012, 1–8. Article 124939. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/124939
  • Dowker, A., Cheriton, O., Horton, R., & Mark, W. (2019). Relationships between attitudes and performance in young children’s mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 100(3), 211–230. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-019-9880-5
  • Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64(3), 830–847. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131221
  • Fabrigar, L. R., & Wegener, D. T. (2011). Exploratory factor analysis. Oxford University Press.
  • Finch, W. H. (2020). Using fit statistic differences to determine the optimal number of factors to retain in an exploratory factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 80(2), 217–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164419865769
  • Geddes, J. D., Murrell, A. R., & Bauguss, J. (2010). Childhood learning: An examination of ability and attitudes toward school. Creative Education, 1(03), 170–183. https://doi.org/10.4236/ce.2010.13027
  • Goretzko, D., Pham, T. T. H., & Bühner, M. (2021). Exploratory factor analysis: Current use, methodological developments and recommendations for good practice. Current Psychology, 40, 3510–3521. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00300-2
  • Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P. H., & Guerin, G. W. (2007). Multivariate latent change modelling of developmental decline in academic intrinsic math motivation and achievement: Childhood through adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(4), 317–327. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407077752
  • Guzmán, B., Rodríguez, C., Ferreira, R. A., & Hernández-Cabrera, J. A. (2021). Psychometric properties of the Revised Child Mathematics Anxiety Questionnaire (CMAQ-R) for Spanish speaking children. Psicología Educativa, 27(2), 115–122. https://doi.org/10.5093/psed2020a17
  • Han, G. (2019). Self-concept and achievement in math among Australian primary students: Gender and culture issues. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 603. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00603
  • Hannula, M. S., Martino, P., Pantziara, M., Zhang, Q., Morselli, F., Heyd-Metzuyanim, E., Lutovac, S., Kaasila, R., Middleton, J. A., Jansen, A., & Goldin, G. A. (2016). Attitudes, beliefs, motivation, and identity in mathematics education: An overview of the field and future directions. In G. Kaiser (Ed.), Attitudes, beliefs, motivation and identity in mathematics education. ICME-13 Topical Surveys (pp. 1–35). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-32811-9_1
  • Hayes, A. F., & Coutts, J. J. (2020). Use omega rather than Cronbach’s alpha for estimating reliability. But … Communication Methods and Measures, 14(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629
  • Hayton, J. C., Allen, D. G., & Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7(2), 191–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428104263675
  • Hettinger, K., Lazarides, R., & Schiefele, U. (2022). Motivational climate in mathematics classrooms: Teacher self-efficacy for student engagement, student- and teacher-reported emotional support and student interest. ZDM Mathematics Education. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-022-01430-x
  • Hidalgo, S., Maroto, A., Ortega, T., & Palacios, A. (2012). Influencia del dominio afectivo en el aprendizaje de las matemáticas [Influence of the affective domain in the learning of mathematics. In V. Mellado, L. J. Blanco, A. B. Borrachero, & J. A. Cárdenas (Eds.), Las emociones en la enseñanza y el aprendizaje de las ciencias y las matemáticas (Vol. 1, pp. 218–243). Grupo de Investigación DEPROFE.
  • Hilton, A. (2018). Engaging primary school students in mathematics: Can iPads make a difference? International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 16(1), 145–165. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-016-9771-5
  • Holenstein, M., Bruckmaier, G., & Grob, A. (2021). How do self-efficacy and self-concept impact mathematical achievement? The case of mathematical modelling. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 155–174. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12443
  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00421
  • Lipnevich, A. A., Preckel, F., & Krumm, S. (2016). Mathematics attitudes and their unique contribution to achievement: Going over and above cognitive ability and personality. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.027
  • Ma, X., & Kishor, N. (1997). Attitude towards self, social factors, and achievement in mathematics: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 9(2), 89–121. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024785812050
  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. -T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
  • Marsh, H. W., Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Morin, A. J., & Nagengast, B. (2011). Methodological measurement fruitfulness of exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM): New approaches to key substantive issues in motivation and engagement. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(4), 322–346. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282911406657
  • Marsh, H. W., Morin, A. J., Parker, P. D., & Kaur, G. (2014). Exploratory structural equation modeling: An integration of the best features of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10(1), 85–110. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153700
  • Marsh, H. W., & O’Mara, A. J. (2008). Self-concept is as multidisciplinary as it is multidimensional: A review of theory, measurement, and practice in self-concept research. In H. W. Marsh, R. G. Craven, & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Self-processes, learning, and enabling human potential: Dynamic new approaches (pp. 87–115). Information Age Publishing.
  • Martínez-Artero, R. N., & Nortes, A. (2013). Actitud hacia las matemáticas en futuros docentes de primaria y de secundaria [Attitude towards mathematics in future primary and secondary teachers]. Edetania Estudios y Propuestas Socioeducativas, 44, 47–76.
  • Mata, M. L., Moneiro, V., & Peixoto, F. (2012). Attitudes towards mathematics: Effects of individual, motivational, and social support factors. Child Development Research, 2012, 1–10. Article 876028. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/876028
  • Mazana, M. Y., Montero, C. S., & Casmir, R. O. (2019). Investigating students’ attitude towards learning mathematics. International Electronic Journal of Mathematics Education, 14(1), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.29333/iejme/3997
  • Mejía-Rodríguez, A. M., Luyten, H., & Meelissen, M. R. M. (2021). Gender differences in mathematics self-concept across the world: An exploration of students and parent data of TIMSS 2015. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 19(6), 1229–1250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-020-10100-x
  • Mohr-Schroeder, M. J., Jackson, C., Cavalcanti, M., Jong, C., Schroeder, D. C., & Speler, L. G. (2017). Parents’ attitudes toward mathematics and the influence on their students’ attitudes toward mathematics: A quantitative study. School Science and Mathematics, 117(5), 214–222. https://doi.org/10.1111/ssm.12225
  • Morin, A. J. S., Marsh, H. W., & Nagengast, B. (2013). Exploratory structural equation modeling. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 395–436). IAP Information Age Publishing.
  • OECD. (2013). PISA 2012 results: Ready to learn: Students’ engagement, drive and self-beliefs (Volume III).PISA. OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264201170-en
  • OECD. (2015). The ABC of gender equality in education: Aptitude, behaviour, confidence. OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264229945-en
  • OECD. (2019). PISA 2018 results (Volume I): What students know and can do. PISA, OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/5f07c754-en
  • Otzen, T., & Manterola, C. (2017). Sampling techniques on a population study. International Journal of Morphology, 35(1), 227–232. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0717-95022017000100037
  • Pallant, J. (2016). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS. Open University Press/McGraw-Hill.
  • Passolunghi, M. C., Rued, T. I., & Tomasetto, C. (2014). Math–gender stereotypes and math-related beliefs in childhood and early adolescence. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 70–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.005
  • Patil, H., Singh, S. N., Mishra, S., & Donavan, D. T. (2017). Parallel analysis engine to aid in determining number of factors to retain using R [Computer software].
  • Paz-Albo, J., Cvencek, D., Herranz, C. V., Hervás, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Preschoolers’ mathematical play and colour preferences: A new window into the development of gendered beliefs about math. Early Child Development and Care, 187(8), 1273–1283. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1295234
  • Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., & Goetz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance: Longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Development, 88(5), 1653–1670. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12704
  • Rodríguez, S., Regueiro, B., Piñeiro, I., Estévez, I., & Valle, A. (2020). Gender differences in mathematics motivation: Differential effects on performance in primary education. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. Article 3050. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03050
  • Rowan-Kenyon, H. T., Swan, A. K., & Creager, M. F. (2012). Social cognitive factors, support, and engagement: Early adolescents’ math interests as precursors to choice of career. The Career Development Quarterly, 60(1), 2–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0045.2012.00001.x
  • Sáenz, C. (2007). La competencia matemática (en el sentido de PISA) de los futuros maestros [Mathematical competence (in the PISA sense) of future teachers]. Enseñanza de las Ciencias, 25(3), 355–366. https://doi.org/10.5565/rev/ensciencias.3701
  • Saß, S., & Kampa, N. (2019). Self-concept profiles in lower secondary Level – an explanation for gender differences in science course selection? Frontiers in Psychology, 10. Article 836. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00836
  • Servicio de Ordenación Académica y Evaluación Educativa. (2019). Los resultados de Asturias en PISA 2018 [The results of Asturias in PISA 2018]. Consejería de Educación del Gobierno del Principado de Asturias. Dirección General de Ordenación, Evaluación y Equidad Educativa.
  • Sheldon, S. B., & Epstein, J. L. (2005). Involvement counts: Family and community partnerships and mathematics achievement. The Journal of Educational Research, 98(4), 196–207. https://doi.org/10.3200/JOER.98.4.196-207
  • Sorvo, R., Kiuru, N., Koponen, T., Aro, T., Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., & Aro, M. (2022). Longitudinal and situational associations between math anxiety and performance among early adolescents. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1514(1), 174–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14788
  • Taasoobshirazi, G., & Wang, S. (2016). The performance of the SRMR, RMSEA, CFI, and TLI: An examination of sample size, path size and degrees of freedom. Journal of Applied Quantitative Methods, 11(3), 31–39.
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2011). Using multivariate statistics. Pearson Education.
  • Valero-Matas, J. A., & Coca, P. (2021). La percepción de las materias STEM en estudiantes de primaria y secundaria [The perception of STEM subjects in elementary and secondary students]. Sociología y Tecnociencia: Revista Digital de Sociología del Sistema Tecnocientífico, 11(1), 116–138. https://doi.org/10.24197/st.Extra_1.2021.116-138
  • Vasalampi, K., Pakarinen, E., Torppa, M., Viljaranta, J., Lerkkanen, M. -K., & Pikkeus, M. (2020). Classroom effect on primary school students’ self-concept in literacy and mathematics. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 35(3), 625–646. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-019-00439-3
  • Viljaranta, J., Tolvanen, A., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2014). The developmental dynamics between interest, self-concept of ability, and academic performance. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 58(6), 734–756. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2014.904419
  • Williams, T., & Williams, K. (2010). Self-efficacy and performance in mathematics: Reciprocal determinism in 33 nations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 453–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017271