2,589
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
PERSONALITY & INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES

Revision and validation of the Japanese-version cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: psychometric properties and measurement invariance across gender

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon | (Reviewing editor)
Article: 2064790 | Received 03 Dec 2021, Accepted 07 Apr 2022, Published online: 24 Apr 2022

Abstract

The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) is a self-report scale measuring the use of nine types of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The present study aimed to develop a revised version of the Japanese translated CERQ (CERQ-RJ). Specifically, we revised translations of the original Japanese version CERQ (CERQ-J), and examined its psychometric properties and measurement invariance across gender. We conducted two surveys 8 weeks apart; participants were recruited from a single Internet survey company in Japan. Results indicated that the CERQ-RJ showed better fit for the predicted nine-factor model than the CERQ-J, and acceptable reliability and validity; it also showed acceptable fit for both gender groups. Results also revealed scalar invariance of the CERQ-RJ across gender groups. Lastly, most strategies showed predicted associations with mental health outcomes in concurrent analyses. The newly translated scale may prove useful in future examinations of emotion regulation among Japanese participants.

PUBLIC INTEREST STATEMENT

Emotion regulation plays a vital role in maintaining our mental health. Prior studies on emotion regulation have mainly focused on cognitive strategies that people use to manage emotions. The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) is a self-report scale measuring the use of nine types of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The present study aimed to develop a revised version of the Japanese translated CERQ (CERQ-RJ). Specifically, we revised translations of the original Japanese version scale, and examined its psychometric properties. Results indicated that the CERQ-RJ showed acceptable reliability and validity; results also revealed that the new scale can examine gender differences. Lastly, most strategies showed significant associations with mental health outcomes in the predicted directions. The newly translated scale may prove useful in future examinations of emotion regulation among Japanese participants.

Emotion regulation can be defined as sets of processes that determine the offset of an emotional response, either deliberate and effortful or automatic and effortless (Koole, Citation2009). Prior studies have examined the former process, portraying emotion regulation as a process that occurs mostly inside people’s minds (Koole & Veenstra, Citation2015). Studies have investigated associations between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and mental health outcomes, indicating that there are “putatively” adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Aldao et al., Citation2010).

The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)—developed by Garnefski et al. (Citation2001)—is widely used to measure trait use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The scale consists of 36 items, 4 items each assessing the use of nine different cognitive strategies: self-blame, blaming others, rumination, catastrophizing, acceptance, positive reappraisal, positive refocusing, refocusing on planning, and putting into perspective. This scale was developed to measure the cognitive components of emotion regulation that have not been studied separately from other dimensions (Garnefski et al., Citation2001). In developing the scale, Garnefski et al. (Citation2001) took a theory-based approach, defining and clarifying dimensions of cognitive emotion regulation based on existing coping measures. They assumed that the first four strategies were maladaptive, and the latter five strategies adaptive; studies that investigated the associations between CERQ and mental health outcomes have generally confirmed these theoretical assumptions (Garnefski et al., Citation2001; Sakakibara & Kitahara, Citation2016). CERQ has been translated into various languages in different cultures, such as European cultures (e.g., Jermann et al., Citation2006), Middle East cultures (e.g., Megreya et al., Citation2016), and Asian cultures (e.g., Zhu et al., Citation2008); the nine-factor structure has been confirmed across cultures, generally showing adequate psychometric properties.

Sakakibara (Citation2015) developed the Japanese version of the CERQ (CERQ-J). CERQ-J is widely used among emotion regulation researchers in Japan (e.g., Kobayashi et al., Citation2020). However, there are several issues in the psychometric properties of the scale. First, results of confirmatory factor analysis in Sakakibara (Citation2015) showed poor fit indices; one item from acceptance was extracted to improve overall fit. Poor fit of data may be due to several reasons. One possibility is that translations of items in the CERQ-J may have been inadequate; hence, revision of translations may help to improve model fit. Another possibility is that poor fit may be due to cultural differences in emotion regulation between Eastern and Western cultures (e.g., Nozaki, Citation2018). For instance, a study suggested that habitual use of suppression may not be associated with negative outcomes depending on culture (Soto et al., Citation2011); thus, even if revisions are made in translations of the CERQ-J, overall fit may not improve. Next, to the best of our knowledge, no study has examined the measurement invariance of the Japanese translated CERQ across socio-demographic variables. In developing a new scale, it is crucial to examine the comparability of scores among different socio-demographic groups; therefore, there is a need for evaluation of measurement invariances of the Japanese version CERQ.

To overcome issues regarding the CERQ-J (Sakakibara, Citation2015), the present study revised translations and examined its reliability and validity. Specifically, we examined the predictive validity of the revised CERQ-J for mental health. Furthermore, we examined measurement invariance across gender among Japanese adults. We especially focused on gender, because previous studies have shown sex differences in emotion regulation (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation2012).

1. Methods

1.1. Participants and procedure

Before data collection, we preregistered all aspects of our study, except for longitudinal data collection and analysis, on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/2db3t).Footnote1 First, we revised translations of the CERQ-J (Sakakibara, Citation2015) and developed the revised Japanese-version scale (CERQ-RJ).Footnote2 All authors re-examined the Japanese translated items, and modified translations when needed. For example, the tense and auxiliary verbs were improperly translated in some items (e.g., items 2 and 12); several items were not necessarily capturing the nuance of the original items (e.g., items 12, 13, 27).Footnote3 Revised translations were then back-translated into English by an English and Japanese bilingual speaker with a PhD degree in clinical psychology. We then compared the original English version scale and back-translated items, and corrected translations when needed. Two surveys were conducted 8 weeks apart; participants were recruited from survey panels of a major Internet survey company in Japan. We aimed to collect data from approximately 700 participants at Time 1. This sample size was based on past studies on psychometric properties of CERQ (e.g., Garnefski & Kraaij, Citation2007; Garnefski et al., Citation2001) with approximately 600 participants. In accordance with our contract with the company, recruitment stopped when the expected number of participants answered the survey. A total of 5317 survey panels had access to the survey; data were collected from 722 participants. Data from one participant was excluded due to poor quality responses; we used data from 721 participants for concurrent analysis (all: Mage = 46.27, SD = 11.71; male: n = 360, Mage = 49.92, SD = 11.69; female: n = 361, Mage = 42.64, SD = 11.28). At Time 2, we aimed to collect data from approximately 300 participants; as did in Time 1, recruitment stopped when the expected number of participants answered the survey. Three hundred and ten people also participated at Time 2, although two participants were excluded due to poor quality responses; data from 308 participants were used for prospective analysis (all: Mage = 47.45, SD = 11.49; male: n = 154, Mage = 51.37, SD = 9.86; female: n = 154, Mage = 43.54, SD = 11.69). All participants responded to self-report measures over the Internet; informed consent was obtained online. This study was approved by the Ethics Review Committee of the Faculty of Economics, Law and Humanities, Kagoshima University (reference number: 20).

2. Measures

2.1. Revised version of the CERQ-J (CERQ-RJ)

The revised version of the CERQ-J (Garnefski et al., Citation2001; Sakakibara, Citation2015; CERQ-RJ) was used to measure cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Participants rated each item on a 5-point likert scale (1–5). We calculated item averages for each subscale. Higher scores indicate more strategy use.

2.2. Personality traits

Big five personality traits were measured using the Japanese version of the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI-J; Gosling et al., Citation2003; Oshio et al., Citation2012). The TIPI-J consists of 10 items which measures the big five personality traits: namely, neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Participants rated each item on a 7-point scale (1–7); higher scores indicate that the individual shows higher levels of each trait. Big five personality traits were accounted for as covariates.

2.3. Mental health measures

Psychological distress was measured using the Japanese version of K6 (Furukawa et al., Citation2008; Kessler et al., Citation2002). Participants rated how much psychological distress they experienced in the last 30 days on a 5-point scale (1–5). Higher scores indicate greater distress. Next, well-being was measured using the Japanese adaptation of WHO Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5-J; Awata et al., Citation2007). Each participant rated their well-being in the past two weeks on a 6-point scale (1–6). Higher scores indicate higher levels of well-being. In reference to results of a meta-analysis (Sakakibara & Kitahara, Citation2016), we hypothesized that rumination, self-blame, blaming others, catastrophizing, and acceptance would show positive associations, and positive reappraisal, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, putting into perspective would show negative associations with psychological distress. Conversely, we hypothesized that rumination, self-blame, blaming others, catastrophizing, and acceptance would show negative associations, and positive reappraisal, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, putting into perspective would show positive associations with well-being. We predicted that overall, strategies and mental health outcomes would show similar levels of effect size as Sakakibara (Citation2015). Although acceptance was originally conceptualized as an adaptive strategy (Garnefski et al., Citation2001), studies robustly indicate positive associations between psychopathological outcomes (e.g., Garnefski & Kraaij, Citation2018; Sakakibara & Kitahara, Citation2016); in reference to these studies, we predicted that acceptance would show positive associations with distress, and negative associations with well-being.

3. Statistical analysis

We conducted all analyses using R.4.0.0 (R Core Team, Citation2020). First, the “psych” package (Revelle, Citation2020) was used to calculate descriptive statistics. To evaluate the internal consistency of the CERQ-RJ, we computed Cronbach’s alpha coefficients and omega coefficients. To examine test-retest reliability, we computed intra-class correlation coefficients using the “irr” package (Gamer et al., Citation2012). Next, we examined the fit indices for the nine-factor model using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). In interpreting fit indices, we referred to cut-off values proposed by Hu and Bentler (Citation1999): cut-off value close to .95 for TLI and CFI, .08 for SRMR, and .06 for RMSEA. We also tested measurement invariance of scale items across gender groups. Packages “lavaan” (Rosseel, Citation2012) and “semtools” (Jorgensen et al., Citation2021) were used to conduct CFAs. Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated to determine associations among strategies and other measures. Lastly, we conducted linear regression analyses to confirm associations among cognitive emotion regulation strategies and mental health outcomes. We conducted regression analyses using both cross-sectional and longitudinal data. In the concurrent analysis predicting mental health outcomes at Time 1, we included age, gender, and big five personality traits as covariates. In the longitudinal analysis predicting mental health outcomes at Time 2, we included age, gender, big five personality traits and outcomes at Time 1 as covariates. Variance inflation factors (VIF) were computed using the “car” package (Fox & Weisberg, Citation2011) to check for multicollinearity.

4. Results

Results of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) are shown in Table . CFA using data from Times 1 and 2 both suggested adequate fit for the nine-factor model; although most fit indices showed values below cut-off points (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999), values of CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR all suggested better fit than the original CERQ-J (Sakakibara, Citation2015). Results of separate CFAs for each gender group also indicated that the nine-factor model was acceptable for both groups. Next, we tested the measurement invariance of the CERQ-RJ across gender groups; results revealed intercept invariance across gender.

Table 1. Confirmatory factor analysis of the CERQ-RJ

Table shows the factor loadings and descriptive statistics of the CERQ-RJ. All items showed acceptable factor loadings (>.40), except for 2 items: “I think that I cannot change anything about it” (Acceptance; factor loading = .292) and “I want to understand why I feel the way I do about what I have experienced” (Rumination; factor loading = .390). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients suggested adequate internal consistency for most factors (α = .70-.86), except for “putting into perspective” (Time 1: α = .58, ω = .57; Time 2: α = .63, ω = .63). Intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) indicated that the test-retest reliability between Times 1 and 2 were acceptable (ICC (2,1) = .408-.586).

Table 2. Factor loadings and descriptive statistics of the CERQ-RJ

Inter-correlations among variables at Time 1 are shown in .Footnote4 Results showed similar associations with Sakakibara (Citation2015): most cognitive strategies showed moderate inter-correlations, whereas putatively adaptive strategies (e.g., positive reappraisal, refocus on planning) and putatively maladaptive strategies (e.g., blaming others, catastrophizing) showed relatively weaker associations with each other. Also, all emotion regulation strategies showed predicted associations with depressive symptoms; associations between strategies and well-being were mostly in the predicted direction, although acceptance and blaming others showed non-significant associations. Next, results of regression analyses are presented in Table . VIFs for regression models were all below 3, indicating that multicollinearity was not a problem. Rumination, refocus on planning, and catastrophizing showed significant associations with Time 1 psychological distress. When Time 1 well-being was the dependent variable, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, positive reappraisal, and catastrophizing showed significant associations. When using longitudinal data, all strategies showed non-significant associations with mental health outcomes.

Table 3. Correlations among subscales of the CERQ-RJ (n = 721)

Table 4. Correlations between strategies and other variables (n = 721)

Table 5. Associations between the CERQ-RJ and mental health outcomes

5. Discussion

In the present study, we developed the revised version of CERQ-J (CERQ-RJ), which measures use of nine cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Specifically, we re-examined the original Japanese translated items (Sakakibara, Citation2015), and modified translations. The CERQ-RJ showed better fit for the predicted nine-factor model than the originally translated CERQ-J, and acceptable reliability and validity; it also showed acceptable fit for both gender groups. Results also revealed scalar invariance of the CERQ-RJ across gender groups; this means that the nine-factor structure holds for both gender groups, and that mean differences between gender are not due to scale properties but gender differences. Lastly, most strategies showed predicted associations with mental health outcomes in concurrent analyses.

Results from the present study suggest that the CERQ-RJ is a useful scale to measure cognitive emotion regulation strategies among Japanese participants. However, a single item each from rumination and acceptance showed low factor loadings. Despite changes made in translations, similar results were shown in the original translation of the CERQ (CERQ-J): Sakakibara (Citation2015) reported that “I want to understand why I feel the way I do about what I have experienced” showed factor loadings of .53, and “I think that I cannot change anything about it” showed factor loadings of .20. Hence, these results may not be due to the quality of translation, but to the lack of validity of items and factor structures in the original CERQ. First, the item from rumination may have shown low factor loadings because it reflects the positive aspect of self-focus. Trapnell and Campbell (Citation1999) differentiated between self-rumination and self-reflection, the former representing negative self-focus and the latter representing positive self-focus. The particular item from rumination in the CERQ-RJ may have reflected self-reflection and not rumination, which may have led to the low factor loading. Next, the item from acceptance may have shown low factor loadings because this item reflects passive acceptance, and not active acceptance (Garnefski & Kraaij, Citation2006; Sakakibara & Kitahara, Citation2016). Garnefski and Kraaij (Citation2006) have pointed out that acceptance in the CERQ shows positive associations with depression scores, because it measures acceptance as a passive form of resignation, rather than acceptance as an active process of self-affirmation to negative experiences. Although it is hard to determine which items measure the passive or the active type, we could assume that the specific item rather reflects the passive form of acceptance (Sakakibara & Kitahara, Citation2016), which may have resulted in the low factor loadings.

Another possible explanation is that emotion regulation processes differ between Eastern and Western populations. A study showed that expressive suppression, which is commonly known as a putatively maladaptive strategy, was associated with negative outcomes only among European-American participants, while non-significant among Eastern Asian participants (Soto et al., Citation2011). Another study found that trait emotional intelligence was negatively associated with expressive suppression among European-American participants, whereas positive associations were found among Japanese participants (Nozaki, Citation2018). These results indicate that the adaptiveness of emotion regulation strategies may differ across cultures. There may even be a possibility that the ways in which we regulate our emotions differ by cultures; for example, studies show that disengaging or resigning from an experience (“akirameru” in Japanese) may have positive influences on mental health among Japanese people (e.g., Suganuma, Citation2013). These cultural differences may have led to poor factor loadings in the CERQ-RJ. Lastly, most strategies showed predicted associations with mental health measures concurrently; however, in the prospective analyses, all strategies showed non-significant results. Similar results were obtained in Garnefski and Kraaij (Citation2007): when predicting anxiety symptoms at follow-up (one year after baseline), fewer strategies showed significant associations. This may mean that cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotional health are inseparable constructs that affect each other concurrently, rather than prospectively predict one another.

6. Limitations

There were several limitations that need to be noted. First, the present study was conducted among participants from a single Internet survey company, which limits generalization of the findings. To increase generalizability, future studies should check whether results are replicable among diverse samples. Next, as noted above, we did not consider cultural differences in emotion regulation. Future research should re-examine differences in emotion regulation styles in the Eastern and Western countries, and develop new scales if needed. Lastly, although psychometric properties showed some improvements, several items still showed low factor loadings. Further scale validations should be conducted in future studies.

Despite these limitations, the present study developed a revised version of the CERQ-J, which showed better psychometric properties than the original Japanese version (Sakakibara, Citation2015). Newly developed scales are rarely re-tested for their psychometric properties; measures lacking in validity are often used without further scale validation (Haebara, Citation2011). However, in measuring psychological constructs, it is crucial that utilized measures are well validated; hence, more attempts should be made in improving scale validity of existing scales.

Correction

This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (216.7 KB)

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the participants in this study. This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant number 20K14133. Portions of the present study were presented at the 85th Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association. The funders played no role in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, preparation of the article, or the decision to submit for publication.

Declaration statement

There is no conflict of interest regarding this manuscript.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/jzmgp).

Supplemental data

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed here.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science [20K14133].

Notes on contributors

Yuhei Urano

Yuhei Urano (Ph.D. The University of Tokyo) is a Lecturer at Yamagata University. His research interests include emotion regulation, social support, and mental health.

Ryota Kobayashi

Ryota Kobayashi (Ph.D. Hiroshima University) is a Lecturer at Fukuoka Prefectural University. His research interests include emotion regulation and interoception.

Ryota Sakakibara

Ryota Sakakibara (Ph.D. The University of Tokyo) is an Associate Professor at Kagoshima University. His research interests include emotion regulation and mental health.

Notes

1. We decided to collect longitudinal data after pre-registration on the Open Science Framework. Longitudinal data collection and analysis was conducted to examine the test-retest reliability and predictive validity of the CERQ-RJ.

2. For re-translation, permission was obtained from the author of the original scale.

3. Revised items are shown as supplementary materials.

4. We conducted the same analyses using Time 2 data as well. Results are shown as supplementary materials.

References

  • Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
  • Awata, S., Bech, P., Yoshida, S., Hirai, M., Suzuki, S., Yamashita, M., Ohara, A., Hinokio, Y., Matsuoka, H., & OKA, Y. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the World Health Organization-Five Well-Being Index in the context of detecting depression in diabetic patients. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(1), 112–119. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01619.x
  • Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R Companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Sage.
  • Furukawa, T. A., Kawakami, N., Saitoh, M., Ono, Y., Nakane, Y., Nakamura, Y., Tachimori, H., Iwata, N., Uda, H., Nakane, H., Watanabe, M., Naganuma, Y., Hata, Y., Kobayashi, M., Miyake, Y., Takeshima, T., & Kikkawa, T. (2008). The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(3), 152–158. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.257
  • Gamer, M., Lemon, J., Gamer, M. M., Robinson, A., & Kendall’s, W. (2012). Package ‘irr’. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/irr/irr.pdf.
  • Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1659–1669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.009
  • Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141–149. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141
  • Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2018). Specificity of relations between adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 32(7), 1401–1408. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1232698
  • Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(8), 1311–1327. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00113-6
  • Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
  • Haebara, T. (2011). Quantitative research methods. (H. Shimoyama, Series Ed.) Studies in Clinical Psychology Vol. 7. Japan: University of Tokyo Press.
  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., d’Acremont, M., & Zermatten, A. (2006). Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ): Confirmatory factor analysis and psychometric properties of the French translation. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 126–131. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.126
  • Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., & Rosseel, Y. Miller, P, Quick, C, Garnier-Villarreal, M, Selig, J, Boulton, A, Preacher, K, Coffman, D, Rhemtulla, M, Ben-Shachar, M.S., Johnson, A.R. (2021). semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling. R package version 0.5-4. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semTools
  • Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S. L., Walters, E., & Zaslavsky, A. M. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non– Specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32(6), 959–976. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702006074
  • Kobayashi, R., Shigematsu, J., Miyatani, M., & Nakao, T. (2020). Cognitive reappraisal facilitates decentering: A longitudinal cross-lagged analysis study. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 103. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00103
  • Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(1), 4–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802619031
  • Koole, S. L., & Veenstra, L. (2015). Does emotion regulation occur only inside people’s heads? Toward a situated cognition analysis of emotion-regulatory dynamics. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 61–68 https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2015.964657.
  • Megreya, A. M., Latzman, R. D., Al-Attiyah, A. A., & Alrashidi, M. (2016). The robustness of the nine-factor structure of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire across four arabic-speaking Middle Eastern countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47(6), 875–890. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116644785
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 161–187. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109
  • Nozaki, Y. (2018). Cross-cultural comparison of the association between trait emotional intelligence and emotion regulation in European-American and Japanese populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 130, 150–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.013
  • Oshio, A., Abe, S., & Cutrone, P. (2012). Development, reliability, and validity of the Japanese version of ten item personality inventory (TIPI-J). Japanese Journal of Personality, 21 (1), 40–52. [In Japanese with English abstract] https://doi.org/10.2132/personality.21.40
  • R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Revelle, W. (2020). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Northwestern University. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychVersion=2.1.3
  • Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02
  • Sakakibara, R. The usage tendency of and the relation with psychological health of cognitive emotion regulation: Development of Japanese-version cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire and focus on the effects of negative emotional intensity. (2015). Japanese Journal of Research on Emotions, 23(1), 46–58. [In Japanese with English abstract] https://doi.org/10.4092/jsre.23.46
  • Sakakibara, R., & Kitahara, M. (2016). The relationship between cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ) and depression, anxiety: Meta-analysis. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 87 (2), 179–185. [In Japanese with English abstract] https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15302
  • Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11(6), 1450–1455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023340
  • Suganuma, S. (2013). Definition and structure of resignation (Akirameru) during adolescence. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 61 (3) , 265–276. [In Japanese with English abstract]. https://doi.org/10.5926/jjep
  • Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284–304. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284
  • Zhu, X., Auerbach, R. P., Yao, S., Abela, J. R. Z., Xiao, J., & Tong, X. (2008). Psychometric properties of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: Chinese version. Cognition and Emotion, 22(2), 288–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701369035