1,901
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Paper

Nurturing connection with nature: the role of spending time in different types of nature

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 630-642 | Received 03 Dec 2021, Accepted 27 Oct 2022, Published online: 13 Nov 2022

ABSTRACT

Connection with nature has been associated with greater participation in a range of biodiversity conservation behaviours, and is increasingly being recognised as a potentially useful policy tool to address conservation outcomes. Yet, understanding of how connection with nature may be nurtured remains poorly understood. This research investigates the extent to which spending time in nature, and in different types of nature, predicted change in connection with nature (captured by the CN-12) over a 12-month period. Data were from a representative sample (based on age, gender, and metropolitan/regional residence) of the adult population in the state of Victoria, Australia, collected via an online survey. Results of analyses of variance and paired-samples t-tests suggested spending time in nature at least monthly was associated with higher connection with nature scores. Results from multiple linear regression and mediation analyses suggested that more time spent in nature (generally), and more time spent in protected areas, waterways, and urban parks (specifically), predicted small increases in connection with nature. These findings suggest that policies that encourage spending more time in nature, including in protected areas, waterways, and urban parks, could be useful for increasing connection with nature and, in turn, addressing biodiversity conservation outcomes. The findings of this research should be of interest to policymakers interested in addressing biodiversity conservation issues.

1. Introduction

In recent decades, relationships between humans and nature have gained increased prominence in the conservation literature, with calls to (re)connect people with nature to foster conservation outcomes and sustainability transformations (Zylstra et al. Citation2014; Ives et al. Citation2018; Riechers et al. Citation2021). Researchers have considered human relationships with nature using a range of definitions and constructs, including nature connectedness (Richardson et al. Citation2020), environmental identity (Clayton Citation2003), inclusion with nature (Schultz Citation2002), nature relatedness (Nisbet et al. Citation2009), and human-nature connection or connectedness (Ives et al. Citation2017, Citation2018; Barragan-Jason et al. Citation2021; see also Tam Citation2013; Restall and Conrad Citation2015). While there is nuance in these constructs, commonalities can also be noted, with definitions typically referring to a sense of interrelationship between humans and the natural world that is subjective and personal, relatively stable across time, and includes thoughts, emotions, and behaviours (Hatty et al. Citation2020). Following Ives et al. (Citation2017), we seek to capture the range of terminology and constructs in the literature, adopting the term connection with nature (CN) ‘because it evokes the subtle yet important idea that (1) humans are already an intimate part of nature and (2) that the state imbues a sense of reciprocity and mutualism’ (Zylstra et al. Citation2014, pp. 121–122).

Some have argued that CN is an important driver of behaviours that protect the natural environment (Schultz Citation2002; Zylstra et al. Citation2014; Otto and Pensini Citation2017). Recent work supports this notion, with positive relationships described between CN and a broad range of pro-environmental behaviours (PEB) (Mackay and Schmitt Citation2019; Whitburn et al. Citation2019), and between CN and behaviours that specifically protect and enhance biodiversity, or pro-biodiversity behaviours (PBB) (Prévot et al. Citation2018; Martin et al. Citation2020; Richardson et al. Citation2020).

In recent years, policymakers internationally are increasingly recognising human-nature relationships in conservation. The Convention on Biological Diversity (CBD) includes the vision ‘living in harmony with nature’ (Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Secretariat Citation2011, Citation2021), while the importance of nurturing CN for conservation has been presented at a recent CBD Convention of the Parties meeting (Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Citation2018) and appears in the Global Biodiversity Outlook 5 report (Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Secretariat Citation2020). Such ideas are also reflected in the biodiversity conservation strategies of countries such as New Zealand (Department of Conservation Citation2020) and Malaysia (Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment Citation2016). In Australia, the role of (re)connecting people with nature to leverage biodiversity conservation has been recognised by both federal and state governments, with the assumption that doing so will enhance Australians’ valuing of, and willingness to protect, nature (Department of Environ-ment, Land, Water and Planning [DELWP], Citation2017; Australian Government Citation2019). For (re)connection with nature to be a useful tool in environmental policy and management, however, it is necessary to understand the factors that influence CN over time (Restall and Conrad Citation2015; Hughes et al. Citation2019).

Despite increasing recognition of the important role CN may play in fostering PEB/PBB, understanding how a stable sense of CN develops, is maintained, or may be nurtured, is somewhat limited (Cleary et al. Citation2018; Ives et al. Citation2018). While researchers have identified a number of factors that predict CN (e.g. Lumber et al. Citation2017; Chawla Citation2020; Richardson et al. Citation2020), of particular interest is time spent in nature.

Early research proposed that frequency of time spent in nature (TIN) was among the strongest predictors of CN (Kals et al. Citation1999). Indeed, both frequency of visits to nature (Cleary et al. Citation2018; Prévot et al. Citation2018) and longer duration of TIN (e.g. hours per day/week: Dornhoff et al. Citation2019; Pérez-Ramírez et al. Citation2021) have been associated with greater CN. Further, environmental education programs of longer duration may facilitate higher CN scores post-program than shorter programs (Braun and Dierkes Citation2017; Barrable and Booth Citation2020).

Another question is what types of nature may best facilitate CN. Much research to date has considered CN as a generic construct without considering the context or type of nature people connect to (Ives et al. Citation2017; Giusti et al. Citation2018). Yet, there are many different types of natural spaces where human-nature interactions occur, such as domestic, urban and botanic gardens; beaches and waterways; and protected areas including national parks (Clayton and Myers Citation2009; Keniger et al. Citation2013). People likely perceive, interact with, and respond to these different types of natural spaces in different ways (Davis et al. Citation2016; Pasca et al. Citation2020) which may have implications for how and when they connect with nature (de Bell et al. Citation2018).

Some researchers have explored relationships between time spent in different types of nature and CN. Schultz and Tabanico (Citation2007) reported increased CN following a day at a wildlife park, and positive correlations between CN and the amount of time spent at a beach or hiking trail, but not a golf course. Scopelliti et al. (Citation2016) noted that users of parks with higher natural values (i.e. greater biodiversity) tended to have higher CN scores than users of parks with lower natural values (i.e. less biodiversity), while Mena-García et al. (Citation2020) reported that walking in the countryside was associated with greater increase in CN than walking in urban parks. Wyles et al. (Citation2019) described higher CN following visits to rural than to urban natural areas, and for visits to protected areas than to non-protected areas. Together, these studies suggest that time spent in different types of natural environments may influence CN in different ways, with time spent in areas with higher natural values, such as national parks, likely to have a greater influence on CN than time spent in areas with lower natural values, such as urban parks.

Yet, with increasing urbanisation and decreasing opportunities to experience wilder-type nature (United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs Citation2019; Australian Bureau of Statistics Citation2020), connecting urban residents with nature may require spending time in ‘managed’ contexts such as zoos or urban parks (Clayton Citation2017; Cleary et al. Citation2018). Indeed, future conservation efforts may depend on fostering relationships with urban nature, as this is where nature is most likely to be encountered (Dunn et al. Citation2006; Gaston and Soga Citation2020). Thus, understanding the relationship between time spent in different types of nature and CN may have utility in informing policy and programs intended to (re)connect people with nature (e.g. DELWP, Citation2017), particularly in urban contexts.

The primary aim of this research is to explore relationships between time spent in nature, and in different types of nature, and CN. While previous research has used cross-sectional (e.g. Cleary et al. Citation2018) or pre-post experimental methodologies (e.g. Braun and Dierkes Citation2017), this research will consider change over a 12-month period without an experimental manipulation but allowing for natural variation in experience.

It has been suggested that repeated experiences in nature over time are required to develop a stable sense of CN (Chawla Citation2020; Salazar et al. Citation2020; Carr and Hughes Citation2021; Clayton et al. Citation2021). This research, therefore, considers whether greater frequency of spending time in nature – generally and in different types of nature – over a 12-month period predicts an increase in CN over the same time period.

We propose two hypotheses:

H1

More time spent in nature over the 12-month period – generally and in different types of nature – will be associated with increased CN.

H2

The frequency of time spent in nature (generally and in different types of nature) will mediate the relationship between CN at Time 1 (baseline) and CN at Time 2 (12-months later).

2. Method

2.1. Participants and procedure

We used data described by Meis-Harris et al. (Citation2019) and Hatty et al. (Citation2020), that were part of a larger study exploring attitudes toward, and use of, the natural environment in the state of Victoria, Australia. These surveys were intended to capture a benchmark of Victorians’ attitudes toward and experiences of the natural environment in a broad sense, rather than of specific places or locations. Data were collected using an online panel survey company with participants responding to questionnaires at two time points: September/October 2018 (Time 1) and September/October 2019 (Time 2). The total sample (N = 1069) was broadly representative of the Victorian adult (18+ years) population relative to age, gender, and metropolitan/regional residence. Survey questions captured CN, TIN, and demographics, as described below (Appendix A). The age range of the final sample (n = 1036) was 19 to 88 years (m = 53.065, SD = 14.790) and comprised 48.8% females (n = 506).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Connection with nature (CN)

The CN-12 was used to capture CN at Time 1 and Time 2. This scale (Appendix B) comprises three dimensions (CN-Identity, CN-Experience, CN-Philosophy) that are strongly correlated yet contribute to a higher order construct (CN-Total) (see Hatty et al. Citation2020 for exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of the data). Responses are provided on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Items were averaged to create a composite score (Cronbach’s α: Time 1 = 0.928, Time 2 = 0.924).

2.2.1. Time spent in nature in the past year

The frequency of time spent in nature in the previous 12 months (captured at Time 2) was assessed using seven items. Participants reported how often they had generally spent time in nature in the past year (TIN past year) on a 9-point ordinal scale (1 = never to 9 = every day). Responses were recoded into four categories (1 = never/rarely [twice yearly or less]; 2 = sometimes [monthly]; 3 = often [weekly/fortnightly]; 4 = very often [daily/every other day]) to ensure consistency of response options with questions assessing time spent in different types of nature.

Participants also reported how often they spent time in six different types of natural areas in the past year on a 5-point ordinal scale (1 = never to 5 = very often [e.g. daily or every other day]): 1) a protected or wilderness area (TIN wilderness); 2) the beach or coastal areas (TIN beach); 3) a lake, river or other waterway (TIN waterway); 4) a zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (TIN zoo); 5) an urban park (TIN urban park); and 6) your garden at home, or the garden of a friend, neighbour or family member (TIN garden). No further description or examples of these natural areas were provided, allowing participants to infer their own meanings of these different types of nature. Each variable was recoded to create a 4-point scale (1 = never/rarely [twice yearly or less]; 2 = sometimes [monthly]; 3 = often [weekly/fortnightly]; 4 = very often [daily/every other day]).

2.2.2. Demographics (age, gender, urbanicity)

Previous research has suggested that age and gender are both associated with CN and TIN (e.g. Dornhoff et al. Citation2019; Hughes et al. Citation2019; Richards et al. Citation2020). Therefore, following Cleary et al. (Citation2018) and Richardson and Hamlin (Citation2021), we included age (in years) and gender (male, female, other) as control variables. In addition, time spent in different types of nature is likely influenced by the level of access to different natural areas. We therefore included urbanicity (self-reported place of residence: inner city, urban/suburban, peri-urban, rural) as a control variable.

2.2.3. Data analyses

We ran descriptive statistics and correlations to provide an overview of the data. We initially explored whether spending time in nature (i.e. at least monthly) was associated with higher or lower CN (Time 1 and Time 2) using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) across TIN categories, with planned contrasts (never/rarely vs sometimes + often + very often) (). We then explored whether CN scores increased or decreased over the 12-month period using paired samples t-tests (two-sided p) using CN scores (Time 1 and Time 2) for each frequency category (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often) ( and Appendix C).

Table 1. One-way ANOVA comparing CN scores (Time 1 and Time 2) across time spent in nature categories (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often) for the seven time spent in nature variables. Planned contrasts (never/rarely vs sometimes + often + very often) are shown (n = 1036; df = degrees of freedom).

To assess H1, we conducted two hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses. In both regressions, age, gender, and urbanicity were entered at Step 1, and CN at Time 1 entered at Step 2. In the first regression, time spent in nature (generally) was entered at Step 3; in the second regression, time spent in the six types of nature were entered together at Step 3. To assess H2, we conducted two mediation analyses with 95% bootstrap confidence intervals of indirect effects (5000 bootstrap samples) using the PROCESS v4.1 macro (Hayes Citation2022). Analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 28 (IBM Corp Citation2021).

Data were screened for assumptions (Field Citation2013). Thirty-three cases identified as outliers (CN at Time 1 and/or CN at Time 2 with z scores <> 2.58), with missing data, and/or with standardised residuals greater than 3.29 were removed (final n = 1036). Inspection of P-P and ZPRED*ZRESID plots suggested the assumptions of normality of residuals, linearity, and homoscedasticity (respectively) were met. Durbin-Watson (1.999–2.007), VIF (1.030–1.401), and tolerance (0.718–0.983) statistics indicated the assumptions of independent errors and multicollinearity were met (Field Citation2013). An a priori power analysis suggested the sample would be adequate to detect a small effect (n = 818 required for f2 = 0.020, α = 0.050, power = 0.800, with 10 predictor variables).

3. Results

Descriptive statistics suggested a relatively even distribution of participants spending time in nature generally and in urban parks. In contrast, a majority of respondents had never/rarely spent time in wilderness areas or zoos/wildlife parks/botanic gardens, while most had spent time in domestic gardens often or very often ().

Figure 1. Percentage of respondents spending time in nature over the past year (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often): generally (TIN past year); in protected or wilderness areas (TIN wilderness); at beach or coastal areas (TIN beach); at a lake, river or other waterway (TIN waterway); at a zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (TIN zoo); at an urban park (TIN urban park); and at a garden at home, or the garden of a friend, neighbour or family member (TIN garden) (n = 1036).

Figure 1. Percentage of respondents spending time in nature over the past year (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often): generally (TIN past year); in protected or wilderness areas (TIN wilderness); at beach or coastal areas (TIN beach); at a lake, river or other waterway (TIN waterway); at a zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (TIN zoo); at an urban park (TIN urban park); and at a garden at home, or the garden of a friend, neighbour or family member (TIN garden) (n = 1036).

Levene statistics for one-way ANOVA indicated homogeneous variances (Appendix C). ANOVA results suggested that spending time in nature at least monthly (i.e. sometimes, often, or very often) was associated with higher CN scores (Time 1 and Time 2) than spending little or no time in nature (i.e. never/rarely). This finding was consistent for all TIN variables, with the exception of spending time at a zoo/wildlife park/botanic garden for CN at Time 2 ( and Appendix B).

Paired samples t-tests suggested significant increases in CN (Time 1 to Time 2) for all time spent in nature variables: TIN past year (often); TIN wilderness, TIN beach, TIN zoo (sometimes), TIN waterway (sometimes, often), TIN urban park (often, very often), and TIN garden (very often) ().

Figure 2. Mean CN scores (Time 1 and Time 2) for each category (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often) of time spent in nature over the past year: generally (TIN past year [top]); in protected or wilderness areas (TIN wilderness), at beach or coastal areas (TIN beach); at a lake, river or other waterway (TIN waterway) [centre]; at a zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (TIN zoo), at an urban park (TIN urban park), and at a garden at home, or the garden of a friend, neighbour or family member (TIN garden) [bottom]. Error bars show standard error of the mean. Significant increases from CN at Time 1 to CN at Time 2 (p < 0.05) are indicated by asterisks (n = 1036).

Figure 2. Mean CN scores (Time 1 and Time 2) for each category (never/rarely, sometimes, often, very often) of time spent in nature over the past year: generally (TIN past year [top]); in protected or wilderness areas (TIN wilderness), at beach or coastal areas (TIN beach); at a lake, river or other waterway (TIN waterway) [centre]; at a zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (TIN zoo), at an urban park (TIN urban park), and at a garden at home, or the garden of a friend, neighbour or family member (TIN garden) [bottom]. Error bars show standard error of the mean. Significant increases from CN at Time 1 to CN at Time 2 (p < 0.05) are indicated by asterisks (n = 1036).

3.1. H1: more time spent in nature over the 12-month period – generally and in different types of nature – will be associated with increased CN

For time spent in nature generally (TIN past year), the full regression model () accounted for 66% of the variation in levels of CN at Time 2 (adjusted R2 = 0.657). As expected, CN at Time 1 made the largest contribution to CN at Time 2 (Step 2: ΔR2 = 0.603), with time spent in nature making a small but significant contribution to CN at Time 2 (Step 3: ΔR2 = 0.017).

Table 2. Hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses predicting connection with nature at Time 2 (CN at Time 2), from time spent in nature in the past year (TIN past year: Step 3, left), and time spent in different types of nature (wilderness, beach, waterway, zoo, urban park, garden: Step 3, right) [SE=standard error, 95% confidence intervals in brackets] (n = 1036).

For time spent in different types of nature (protected/wilderness area, beach/coast, lake/rive/other waterway, zoo/wildlife park/botanic garden, urban park, domestic garden), the full regression model () accounted for 66% of the variation in levels of CN at Time 2 (adjusted R2 = 0.662). As in the previous analysis, CN at Time 1 made the largest contribution to CN at Time 2 (Step 2: ΔR2 = 0.603), with time spent in different types of nature together making a small but significant contribution to CN at Time 2 (Step 3: ΔR2 = 0.023). Among the different types of nature, three significantly predicted CN at Time 2: time spent in wilderness/protected areas (TIN wilderness: β = 0.045); lake/river/other waterway (TIN water: β = 0.051); and urban parks (TIN urban park: β = 0.096).

3.2. H2: The frequency of time spent in nature (generally and in different types of nature) will mediate the relationship between CN at Time 1 (baseline) and CN at Time 2 (12-months later).

We conducted a simple mediator analysis to determine whether time spent in nature (generally) mediated the relationship between CN at Time 1 and CN at Time 2. The final model suggested positive and significant direct and indirect effects; the total effect of CN at Time 1 on CN at Time 2 was 0.769 (t = 41.682, p < 0.001), with a direct effect (c) of 0.722 (t = 37.556, p < 0.001), and indirect effect (ab) of 0.047 (t = 5.875, 95% CI [0.032, 0.065] ().

Figure 3. Simple mediation analysis predicting connection with nature at Time 2 (Y) from connection with nature at Time 1 (×) with time spent in nature generally (M) as mediator. Age (C1), gender (C2), and urbanicity (C3) are entered as covariates (***p < 0.001).

Figure 3. Simple mediation analysis predicting connection with nature at Time 2 (Y) from connection with nature at Time 1 (×) with time spent in nature generally (M) as mediator. Age (C1), gender (C2), and urbanicity (C3) are entered as covariates (***p < 0.001).

We then conducted a parallel multiple mediator analysis to determine whether time spent in different types of nature mediated the relationship between CN at Time 1 and CN at Time 2. The final model suggested positive and significant direct and indirect effects. The total effect of CN at Time 1 on CN at Time 2 was 0.769 (t = 41.682, p < 0.001), with a direct effect (c) of 0.712 (t = 36.240, p < 0.001). CN at Time 1 predicted all time spent nature variables (a1-a6). Results suggested specific indirect effects of three mediators (in the presence of other mediators): TIN wilderness (a1b1 = 0.013, t = 2.167, 95% CI [0.001, 0.026]; TIN water (a3b3 = 0.013, t = 2.600, 95% CI [0.003, 0.024]; and TIN urban park (a5b5 = 0.021, t = 4.200, 95% CI [0.011, 0.031] (unstandardized coefficients) ().

Figure 4. Parallel multiple mediation analysis predicting connection with nature at Time 2 (Y) from connection with nature at Time 1 (×) with time spent in six types of nature (protected or wilderness area (M1); beach or coastal area (M2); lake, river or other waterway (M3); zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (M4); urban park (M5); own garden at home (M6) as mediators). Age (C1), gender (C2), and urbanicity (C3) are entered as covariates (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; nsp > 0.05).

Figure 4. Parallel multiple mediation analysis predicting connection with nature at Time 2 (Y) from connection with nature at Time 1 (×) with time spent in six types of nature (protected or wilderness area (M1); beach or coastal area (M2); lake, river or other waterway (M3); zoo, wildlife park, or botanical garden (M4); urban park (M5); own garden at home (M6) as mediators). Age (C1), gender (C2), and urbanicity (C3) are entered as covariates (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; nsp > 0.05).

4. Discussion

For CN to be useful in conservation policy and management, understanding how a stable sense of CN may be nurtured is essential. This research sought to determine the extent to which time spent in nature, and in different types of nature, contributed to an increase in CN over a 12-month period.

As CN is relatively stable over time (Nisbet et al. Citation2011; Hatty et al. Citation2020), it is important to note that small changes in CN at Time 2 were expected. Overall, the findings suggest that more time spent in nature was associated with slightly higher CN scores at Time 2. Among those who spent time in nature at least monthly over the 12-month period, CN scores tended to increase over the same time period, while among those who spent little or no time in nature (i.e. less than monthly), CN scores did not change over the year. Results further suggested that participants who spent more time in wilderness/protected areas, along rivers/lakes or other waterways, and in urban parks tended to have slightly higher CN at Time 2, regardless of CN at Time 1.

Previous research has suggested that areas with higher natural values (e.g. those with greater biodiversity) may be more useful for nurturing CN than areas with lower natural values (Scopelliti et al. Citation2016; Wyles et al. Citation2019; Mena-García et al. Citation2020). The present findings suggest that while spending time in wilder-type areas (i.e. wilderness/protected areas and waterways) does predict an increase in CN over time, so too does spending time in urban parks. These results are consistent with those of Lumber et al. (Citation2017) and Restall et al. (Citation2021) who also reported positive relationships between CN and time spent in both urban and protected areas. These findings also provide support for the notion that repeated experiences in nature over time are needed to foster an enduring sense of CN (e.g. Richardson et al. Citation2020; Clayton et al. Citation2021).

Also consistent with previous research (Lin et al. Citation2014; Colléony et al. Citation2017, Citation2020; Cox et al. Citation2018), results of the two mediation analyses suggest that those higher in CN tended to spend more time in nature – generally, in protected areas, along waterways, and in urban parks – than those lower in CN. While not explicitly tested, it is possible that a bidirectional relationship between TIN and CN may also be present, as has been suggested previously (Rosa and Collado Citation2019; Martin et al. Citation2020) (see also Limitations and future research below).

4.1. Characteristics of nature experiences

These findings highlight the role that time spent in nature may play in fostering CN, and suggest that policies and programs intended to encourage people to spend more time in nature, including different types of nature, could help to foster CN. Yet, the effect sizes were small, and the positive and significant direct (c) and indirect (ab) effects indicate complementary mediation (Zhao et al. Citation2010), together suggesting that other factors also likely influence change in CN over time. Such factors may include what people do while they’re in nature, or how they experience nature (Colléony et al. Citation2020, Citation2020; Richardson et al. Citation2020).

Recent evidence suggests that intentional awareness while in nature (Schutte and Malouff Citation2018) and active engagement with nature through smell or touch (Lumber et al. Citation2017; Colléony et al. Citation2020; Carr and Hughes Citation2021) are associated with higher CN scores. Further, emotions such as wonder and excitement appear to play a role in fostering CN (Giusti et al. Citation2018; Richardson et al. Citation2020; Carr and Hughes Citation2021), perhaps by facilitating learning and openness to experience (Yang et al. Citation2018). Inducing positive emotions such as awe or feeling moved have been associated with higher CN (Moreton et al. Citation2019; Petersen and Martin Citation2020; Ambrose et al. Citation2021), while feeling emotions such as love while experiencing nature has been described as central to the lived experience of CN (Furness Citation2021). Learning something new and a sense of compassion for the natural environment while experiencing nature may also be important for nurturing CN (Lumber et al. Citation2017; Carr and Hughes Citation2021).

Given that CN comprises multiple dimensions (Ives et al. Citation2018), activities that target several dimensions may be most effective in fostering an enduring sense of CN (Zylstra et al. Citation2014). Indeed, Riechers et al. (Citation2020) recently noted that emotional and experiential dimensions of CN appear to have multiple links to other dimensions, thus nurturing emotional and experiential connections could be important mechanisms for enhancing an overall sense of CN. Similarly, activities that involve simultaneous activation of different pathways to CN may be also be valuable (Richardson et al. Citation2020; Carr and Hughes Citation2021). Such activities may include those that encourage emotional reflection about nature while actually experiencing nature (e.g. Pennisi et al. Citation2017), arts-based activities (Muhr Citation2020; Raatikainen et al. Citation2020), or technology-based interventions (Mattijssen et al. Citation2020). These could be particularly useful if they encourage noticing ‘good things’ in nature (McEwan et al. Citation2020) or engagement with natural beauty (Richardson and McEwan Citation2018).

One group of activities that can activate multiple dimensions of, and pathways to, CN are conservation behaviours, and particularly those that occur in and with the natural environment. Research suggests that engaging with nature via meaningful, compassionate, and multisensory activities, such as environmental volunteering (Cosquer et al. Citation2012; Schuttler et al. Citation2018) or litter clean-ups (Wyles et al. Citation2017) could play an important role in nurturing CN. Indeed, CN can also be an important motivator for engaging in these activities (Admiraal et al. Citation2017; Ganzevoort and van den Born Citation2020), highlighting that the relationship between CN and nature-protective behaviours is likely reciprocal (Hamlin and Richardson Citation2022).

In addition to CN, other drivers of nature-protective behaviours include feelings of care or nurturing, beliefs of inherent value in the activity, and a desire for a meaningful life (Admiraal et al. Citation2017; Ganzevoort and van den Born Citation2020). Such motivations reflect relational values, or the ‘preferences, principles and virtues about human-nature relationships’ (Chan et al. Citation2018, p. A1) that can influence how people think about and interact with nature (Chan et al. Citation2016; Kleespies and Dierkes Citation2020; Mattijssen et al. Citation2020). Relational values reflect the many different types and qualities of relationships that people have with nature (e.g. responsibility, moral obligation, identity, a ‘good life’: Chan et al. Citation2016), and may provide a useful leverage point for conservation policy (Mattijssen et al. Citation2020).

4.2. Implications for conservation policy

While policymakers have begun to consider human-nature relationships in conservation planning and management, policies and land management practices that specifically nurture CN are needed (Richardson et al. Citation2021). This research suggests that policies and practices that facilitate spending time in nature, including urban parks, could be useful.

Researchers have investigated a range of different ways to encourage people to spend more time in nature, such as through biophilic urban design. Urban spaces that include different types of natural elements provide opportunities for incidental and intentional interactions with nature (Church Citation2018; Lin et al. Citation2018) which may enhance CN (Cox et al. Citation2017; Shanahan et al. Citation2017). Designing public natural spaces to include food plants and opportunities for urban agriculture (Palliwoda et al. Citation2017; Kingsley et al. Citation2021) and to facilitate interactive and sensory immersion with nature (Souter-Brown Citation2015; Pennisi et al. Citation2017; Pan et al. Citation2020) can also encourage citizens to spend more time in, actively engage with, and connect to nature in such spaces.

Involving citizens in the design, installation, and management of natural spaces may be another mechanism for encouraging time spent in nature and thus nurturing CN (Light Citation2006; Church Citation2018; Mattijssen et al. Citation2020). Citizen engagement in the design and management of natural spaces can increase psychological ownership of, and connection to, those spaces, as well as stewardship behaviours such as picking up litter (de Bell et al. Citation2018; Mullenbach et al. Citation2019; Preston and Gelman Citation2020; Peck et al. Citation2021). Psychological ownership has also been associated with enjoyment of, and connection to, natural areas as well as to PEB and PBB such as environmental volunteering (Ganzevoort and van den Born Citation2020; Kuo et al. Citation2021).

4.3. Limitations and future research

A number of limitations are evident in the current research. Firstly, the measures of time spent in nature were imprecise – memory limitations, for example, may impede accurate recollection of the frequency of time spent in nature over the previous 12-months. Further, time spent in specific types of nature, such as urban parks, does not capture differences in quality (e.g. amount of tree cover, level of biodiversity) of different, albeit similarly classified, types of nature. Future research would benefit from more refined measures of TIN.

Regarding connection with nature, CN scores were relatively high (Time 1: m = 5.263, SD = 0.969; Time 2: m = 5.314, SD = 0.946) thus the small change may be due to a ceiling effect. Further, interventions designed to foster CN typically show the greatest increases among participants with lower CN scores pre-intervention than participants with higher CN scores (Braun and Dierkes Citation2017; Barrable and Booth Citation2020; Chawla Citation2020). While not explicitly tested here, it’s possible that participants with low CN scores at Time 1 who spent more time in nature over the year would show greater increases in CN than participants with higher CN scores at Time 1. Additional investigation is needed to tease out possible differences between people with low versus high CN at Time 1. In addition, evidence suggests that CN scores (e.g. Duffy and Verges Citation2010) and spending time in nature (e.g. Tester-Jones et al. Citation2020) can vary by seasons and weather patterns, thus further research is needed to determine whether CN scores increase or decrease in response to seasons and/or weather. Another area for future research relates to the frequency, total number, or type of nature experiences needed to facilitate a stable sense of CN. While it has been proposed that repeated experiences of nature are needed to develop an enduring trait-like CN (e.g. Richardson et al. Citation2020; Salazar et al. Citation2020; Clayton et al. Citation2021), such experiences may differ across groups. People with lower initial CN scores may, for example, require fewer or less intense experiences of nature to increase CN, while those with higher initial CN scores may require more or greater intensity experiences of nature, such as a multi-day wilderness expedition, to effect an increase in CN (Salazar et al. Citation2020).

Finally, future research should consider the broader socio-cultural and contextual factors that influence peoples’ experiences of natural environments. Cultural and social factors influence landscape preferences and attitudes to natural spaces (Buijs et al. Citation2009; Özgüner Citation2011; Clayton et al. Citation2017) which may impact the ways in which different people experience nature. Investigation of the role of socio-cultural and contextual factors (e.g. ethnicity, socio-economic status) in the TIN-CN relationship warrants further investigation.

5. Conclusion

Connection with nature is increasingly being recognised as a potentially useful policy tool to address conservation issues. Understanding the mechanisms by which connection with nature may be nurtured can usefully inform conservation policies and programs for which nurturing connection with nature is an outcome. Policies that encourage people to spend more time in nature – generally, and in protected areas, waterways, and urban parks – could be useful for increasing a stable sense of connection among the general population which may, in turn, increase engagement in conservation.

Supplemental material

Acknowledgements

Thank you to Kim Lowe, Fern Hames, and the Victorians Value Nature team for their input into this research. Thank you also to the editor and two anonymous reviewers whose input improved the clarity and flow of this research.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

The data and syntax for this research are available at Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/3ca7f/?view_only=82164315080249d49d0eec6e5a34e21f)

Supplementary material

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/26395916.2022.2143570.

Additional information

Funding

This research was commissioned and funded by the Victorian Government Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning (DELWP), and was also funded by an Australian Government Research Training Program (RTP) Scholarship.

References

  • Admiraal JF, Van Den Born RJG, Beringer A, Bonaiuto F, Cicero L, Hiedanpää J, Knights P, Knippenberg LWJ, Molinario E, Musters CJM, et al. 2017. Motivations for committed nature conservation action in Europe. Environ Conserv. 44(2):148–157. doi:10.1017/S037689291700008X.
  • Ambrose LE, Wiezel A, Pages EB, Shiota MN. 2021. Images of nature, nature-self representation, and environmental attitudes. Sustainability. 13(14):8025. doi:10.3390/su13148025.
  • Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2020. Australian historical population statistics, 2016. Cat. No 3105 0.65.001. https://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/[email protected]/mf/3105.0.65.001
  • Australian Government. 2019. Australia’s Strategy for Nature 2019-2030. https://www.australiasnaturehub.gov.au/national-strategy
  • Barrable A, Booth D. 2020. Increasing nature connection in children: a mini review of interventions. Front Psychol. 11(March). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00492.
  • Barragan-Jason G, de Mazancourt C, Parmesan C, Singer MC, Loreau M. 2021. Human–nature connectedness as a pathway to sustainability: a global meta-analysis. Conserv Lett. 15(1):1–7. doi:10.1111/conl.12852.
  • Braun T, Dierkes P. 2017. Connecting students to nature - how intensity of nature experience and student age influence the success of outdoor education programs. Environ Educ Res. 23(7):937–949. doi:10.1080/13504622.2016.1214866.
  • Buijs AE, Elands BHM, Langers F. 2009. No wilderness for immigrants: cultural differences in images of nature and landscape preferences. Landsc Urban Plan. 91(3):113–123. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2008.12.003.
  • Carr V, Hughes J. 2021. Predicting the development of adult nature connection through nature activities: developing the evaluating nature activities for connection tool. Front Psychol. 12(March):1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.618283.
  • Chan KMA, Balvanera P, Benessaiah K, Chapman M, Díaz S, Gómez-Baggethun E, Gould R, Hannahs N, Jax K, Klain S, et al. 2016. Why protect nature? Rethinking values and the environment. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 113(6):1462–1465. doi:10.1073/pnas.1525002113.
  • Chan KM, Gould RK, Pascual U. 2018. Editorial overview: relational values: what are they, and what’s the fuss about? Curr Opin Environ Sustain. 35:A1–7. doi:10.1016/j.cosust.2018.11.003.
  • Chawla L. 2020. Childhood nature connection and constructive hope: a review of research on connecting with nature and coping with environmental loss. People Nat. 2(3):619–642. doi:10.1002/pan3.10128.
  • Church SP. 2018. From street trees to natural areas: retrofitting cities for human connectedness to nature. J Environ Plann Manage. 61(5–6):878–903. doi:10.1080/09640568.2018.1428182.
  • Clayton S. 2003. Environmental identity: a conceptual and operational definition. In: Clayton SD, and Opotow S, editors. Identity and the natural environment: the psychological significance of nature. Cambridge (MA): The MIT Press; p. 45–66.
  • Clayton S. 2017. Social issues and personal life: considering the environment. J Soc Issues. 73(3):667–681. doi:10.1111/josi.12237.
  • Clayton S, Colléony A, Conversy P, Maclouf E, Martin L, Torres AC, Truong MX, Prévot AC. 2017. Transformation of experience: toward a new relationship with nature. Conservation Letters. 10(5):645–651. doi:10.1111/conl.12337.
  • Clayton S, Czellar S, Nartova-Bochaver S, Skibins JC, Salazar G, Tseng Y-C, Irkhin B, Monge-Rodriguez FS. 2021. Cross-cultural validation of a revised Environmental Identity Scale. Sustainability. 13(2387):1–12. doi:10.3390/su13042387.
  • Clayton S, Myers G. 2009. Conservation psychology: understanding and promoting human care for nature. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
  • Cleary A, Fielding KS, Murray Z, Roiko A. 2018. Predictors of nature connection among urban residents: assessing the role of childhood and adult nature experiences. Environ Behav. 52(6):579–610. doi:10.1177/0013916518811431.
  • Colléony A, Cohen-Seffer R, Shwartz A. 2020. Unpacking the causes and consequences of the extinction of experience. Biol Conserv. 251(July):108788. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2020.108788.
  • Colléony A, Levontin L, Shwartz A. 2020. Promoting meaningful and positive nature interactions for visitors to green spaces. Conserv Biol. 34(6):1373–1382. doi:10.1111/cobi.13624.
  • Colléony A, Prévot AC, Saint Jalme M, Clayton S. 2017. What kind of landscape management can counteract the extinction of experience? Landsc Urban Plan. 159:23–31. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.11.010.
  • Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD]. 2018. Recommen-dations for increased focus on connecting people with nature to inspire enhanced action on biodiversity conservation. https://www.cbd.int/doc/strategic-plan/Post2020/postsbi/C%26nn3.pdf
  • Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Secretariat. 2011. Strategic Plan for Biodiversity 2011-2020 and the Aichi Targets. https://www.cbd.int/sp/
  • Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Secretariat. 2020. Global Biodiversity Outlook. https://www.cbd.int/gbo/
  • Convention on Biological Diversity [CBD] Secretariat. 2021. First Draft of the Post-2020 Global Biodiversity Framework. https://www.cbd.int/doc/c/abb5/591f/2e46096d3f0330b08ce87a45/wg2020-03-03-en.pdf
  • Cosquer A, Raymond R, Prevot-Julliard AC. 2012. Observations of everyday biodiversity: a new perspective for conservation? Ecology and Society. 17(4, 4). 10.5751/ES-04955-170402
  • Cox DTC, Hudson HL, Shanahan DF, Fuller RA, Gaston KJ. 2017. The rarity of direct experiences of nature in an urban population. Landsc Urban Plan. 160:79–84. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.12.006.
  • Cox DTC, Shanahan DF, Hudson HL, Fuller RA, Gaston KJ. 2018. The impact of urbanisation on nature dose and the implications for human health. Landsc Urban Plan. 179(July):72–80. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2018.07.013.
  • Davis N, Daams M, van Hinsberg A, Sijtsma F. 2016. How deep is your love – of nature? A psychological and spatial analysis of the depth of feelings towards Dutch nature areas. Appl Geogr. 77:38–48. doi:10.1016/j.apgeog.2016.09.012.
  • de Bell S, Graham H, White PCL. 2018. The role of managed natural spaces in connecting people with urban nature: a comparison of local user, researcher, and provider views. Urban Ecosystems. 21(5):875–886. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11252-018-0762-x.
  • Department of Conservation. 2020. Aotearoa New Zealand biodiversity strategy. doi:10.4135/9781412952453.n56.
  • Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning [DELWP]. 2017. Protecting Victoria’s Environment – Biodiversity 2037. https://www.environment.vic.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0022/51259/Protecting-Victorias-Environment-Biodiversity-2037.pdf
  • Dornhoff M, Sothmann JN, Fiebelkorn F, Menzel S. 2019. Nature relatedness and environmental concern of young people in Ecuador and Germany. Front Psychol. 10(MAR):1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00453.
  • Duffy S, Verges M. 2010. Forces of nature affect implicit connections with nature. Environ Behav. 42(6):723–739. doi:10.1177/0013916509338552.
  • Dunn RR, Gavin MC, Sanchez MC, Solomon JN. 2006. The pigeon paradox: dependence of global conservation on urban nature. Conservation Biology. 20(6):1814–1816. doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00533.x.
  • Field A. 2013. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Carmichael Meditor. 4th. London (UK): SAGE Publications Ltd.
  • Furness E. 2021. Understanding the lived experience of connection to nature. Conservation Science and Practice. 3(7):1–8. doi:10.1111/csp2.440.
  • Ganzevoort W, van den Born RJG. 2020. Understanding citizens’ action for nature: the profile, motivations and experiences of Dutch nature volunteers. Journal for Nature Conservation. 55(December 2019):125824. doi:10.1016/j.jnc.2020.125824.
  • Gaston KJ, Soga M. 2020. Extinction of experience: the need to be more specific. People and Nature. 1–7. doi:10.1002/pan3.10118.
  • Giusti M, Svane U, Raymond CM, Beery TH. 2018. A frame-work to assess where and how children connect to nature. Front Psychol. 8(Jan):1–21. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02283.
  • Hamlin I, Richardson M. 2022. Visible garden biodiversity is associated with noticing nature and nature connectedness. Ecopsychology. 14(2):111–117. doi:10.1089/eco.2021.0064.
  • Hatty MA, Smith LDG, Goodwin D, Mavondo FT. 2020. The CN-12: a brief, multidimensional connection with nature instrument. Front Psychol. 11(1566):1–14. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01566.
  • Hayes AF. (2022). The Process macro for SPSS, SAS, and R. https://www.processmacro.org/version-history.html
  • Hughes J, Rogerson M, Barton J, Bragg R. 2019. Age and connection to nature: when is engagement critical? Front Ecol Environ. 17(5):265–269. doi:10.1002/fee.2035.
  • IBM Corp. 2021. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (28.0). Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
  • Ives CD, Abson DJ, von Wehrden H, Dorninger C, Klaniecki K, Fischer J. 2018. Reconnecting with nature for sustainability. Sustain Sci. 13(5):1–9. doi:10.1007/s11625-018-0542-9.
  • Ives CD, Giusti M, Fischer J, Abson DJ, Klaniecki K, Dorninger C, Laudan J, Barthel S, Abernethy P, Martín-López B, et al. 2017. Human-nature connection: a multidisciplinary review. Curr Opin Environ Sustain. 26–27(May):106–113. doi:10.1016/j.cosust.2017.05.005.
  • Kals E, Schumacher D, Montada L. 1999. Emotional affinity toward nature as a motivational basis to protect nature. Environ Behav. 31(2):178–202. doi:10.1177/00139169921972056.
  • Keniger LE, Gaston KJ, Irvine KN, Fuller RA. 2013. What are the benefits of interacting with nature? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 10(3):913–935. doi:10.3390/ijerph10030913.
  • Kingsley J, Egerer M, Nuttman S, Keniger L, Pettitt P, Frantzeskaki N, Gray T, Ossola A, Lin B, Bailey A, et al. 2021. Urban agriculture as a nature-based solution to address socio-ecological challenges in Australian cities. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 60(November 2020):127059. doi:10.1016/j.ufug.2021.127059.
  • Kleespies MW, Dierkes PW. 2020. Exploring the construct of relational values: an empirical approach. Front Psychol. 11(March):1–14. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00209.
  • Kuo H-M, Su J-Y, Wang C-H, Kiatsakared P, Chen K-Y. 2021. Place attachment and environmentally responsible behavior: the mediating role of destination psychological ownership. Sustainability. 13(12):6809. doi:10.3390/su13126809.
  • Light A. Ecoloigcal citizenship: the demogratic promise of restoration. In: Pratt RH, editor. The human metropolis: people and nature in the 21st-century city. University of Massachusetts Press; 2006. p. 169–181. https://stevenreedjohnson.com/ stevenr1/stevenreedjohnson/GCMore2_files/humaneMetropolis.pdf#page=183
  • Lin BB, Egerer MH, Ossola A. 2018. Urban gardens as a space to engender biophilia: evidence and ways forward. Frontiers in Built Environment. 4(December):1–10. doi:10.3389/fbuil.2018.00079.
  • Lin BB, Fuller RA, Bush R, Gaston KJ, Shanahan DF. 2014. Opportunity or orientation? Who uses urban parks and why. PLoS ONE. 9(1):1–7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0087422.
  • Lumber R, Richardson M, Sheffield D. 2017. Beyond knowing nature: contact, emotion, compassion, meaning, and beauty are pathways to nature connection. PLoS ONE. 12(5):1–24. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0177186.
  • Mackay CML, Schmitt MT. 2019. Do people who feel more connected to nature do more to protect it? A meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol. 65(101323):1–9. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101323.
  • Martin L, White MP, Hunt A, Richardson M, Pahl S, Burt J. 2020. Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. J Environ Psychol. 68(January):101389. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101389.
  • Mattijssen TJM, Ganzevoort W, van den Born RJG, Arts BJM, Breman BC, Buijs AE, van Dam RI, Elands BHM, de Groot WT, Knippenberg LWJ. 2020. Relational values of nature: leverage points for nature policy in Europe. Ecosystems and People. 16(1):402–410. doi:10.1080/26395916.2020.1848926.
  • McEwan K, Ferguson FJ, Richardson M, Cameron R. 2020. The good things in urban nature: a thematic framework for optimising urban planning for nature connectedness. Landsc Urban Plan. 194(October 2019):103687. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2019.103687.
  • Meis-Harris J, Saeri A, Boulet M, Borg K, Faulkner N, Jorgensen B. (2019). Victorians Value Nature: Survey Results. https://www.ari.vic.gov.au/research/people-and-nature/victorians-value-nature
  • Mena-García A, Olivos P, Loureiro A, Navarro O. 2020. Effects of contact with nature on connectedness, environmental identity and evoked contents/Efectos del contacto con la naturaleza en conectividad, identidad ambiental y contenidos evocados. Psyecology. 11(1):21–36. doi:10.1080/21711976.2019.1643663.
  • Ministry of Natural Resources and Environment. (2016). National Policy on Biological Diversity 2016-2025. https://www.cbd.int/doc/world/my/my-nbsap-v2-en.pdf
  • Moreton SG, Arena A, Hornsey M, Crimston C, Tiliopoulos N. 2019. Elevating nature: moral elevation increases feelings of connectedness to nature. J Environ Psychol. 65(January):101332. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101332.
  • Muhr MM. 2020. Beyond words–the potential of arts-based research on human-nature connectedness. Ecosystems and People. 16(1):249–257. doi:10.1080/26395916.2020.1811379.
  • Mullenbach LE, Baker BL, Benfield J, Hickerson B, Mowen AJ. 2019. Assessing the relationship between community engagement and perceived ownership of an urban park in Philadelphia. Journal of Leisure Research. 50(3):201–219. doi:10.1080/00222216.2019.1581719.
  • Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. 2009. The nature relatedness scale: linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ Behav. 41(5):715–740. doi:10.1177/0013916508318748.
  • Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. 2011. Happiness is in our nature: exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. J Happiness Stud. 12(2):303–322. doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9197-7.
  • Otto S, Pensini P. 2017. Nature-based environmental education of children: environmental knowledge and connectedness to nature, together, are related to ecological behaviour. Global Environmental Change. 47(September):88–94. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.09.009.
  • Özgüner H. 2011. Cultural differences in attitudes towards urban parks and green spaces. Landscape Research. 36(5):599–620. doi:10.1080/01426397.2011.560474.
  • Palliwoda J, Kowarik I, von der Lippe M. 2017. Human-biodiversity interactions in urban parks: the species level matters. Landsc Urban Plan. 157:394–406. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.09.003.
  • Pan YT, Yang KK, Wilson K, Hong ZR, Lin HS. 2020. The impact of museum interpretation tour on visitors’ engagement and post-visit conservation intentions and behaviours. International Journal of Tourism Research. 22(5):593–603. doi:10.1002/jtr.2358.
  • Pasca L, Aragonés JI, Fraijo-Sing B. 2020. Categorizing landscapes: approaching the concept of nature (Categorizando paisajes: una aproximación al concepto de naturaleza). Psyecology. 11(3):342–362. doi:10.1080/21711976.2019.1659029.
  • Peck J, Kirk CP, Luangrath AW, Shu SB. 2021. Caring for the commons: using psychological ownership to enhance stewardship behavior for public goods. J Mark. 85(2):33–49. doi:10.1177/0022242920952084.
  • Pennisi L, Lackey NQ, Holland SM. 2017. Can an immersion exhibit inspire connection to nature and environmentally responsible behavior? Journal of Interpretation Research. 22(2):35–49. doi:10.1177/109258721702200204.
  • Pérez-Ramírez I, García-Llorente M, Saban de la Portilla C, Benito A, Castro AJ. 2021. Participatory collective farming as a leverage point for fostering human-nature connectedness. Ecosystems and People. 17(1):222–234. doi:10.1080/26395916.2021.1912185.
  • Petersen E, Martin AJ. 2020. Kama muta (≈ being moved) helps connect people in and to nature: a photo elicitation approach. Ecopsychology. 13(1):37–47. doi:10.1089/eco.2020.0032.
  • Preston SD, Gelman SA. 2020. This land is my land: psychological ownership increases willingness to protect the natural world more than legal ownership. J Environ Psychol. 70(June):101443. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101443.
  • Prévot AC, Cheval H, Raymond R, Cosquer A. 2018. Routine experiences of nature in cities can increase personal commitment toward biodiversity conservation. Biol Conserv. 226(June):1–8. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2018.07.008.
  • Prévot AC, Clayton S, Mathevet R. 2018. The relationship of childhood upbringing and university degree program to environmental identity: experience in nature matters. Environmental Education Research. 24(2):263–279. doi:10.1080/13504622.2016.1249456.
  • Raatikainen KJ, Juhola K, Huhmarniemi M, Peña-Lagos H. 2020. “Face the cow”: reconnecting to nature and increasing capacities for pro-environmental agency. Ecosystems and People. 16(1):273–289. doi:10.1080/26395916.2020.1817151.
  • Restall B, Conrad E. 2015. A literature review of connectedness to nature and its potential for environmental management. J Environ Manage. 159:264–278. doi:10.1016/j.jenvman.2015.05.022.
  • Restall B, Conrad E, Cop C. 2021. Connectedness to nature: mapping the role of protected areas. J Environ Manage. 293(April):112771. doi:10.1016/j.jenvman.2021.112771.
  • Richards DR, Fung TK, Leong RAT, Sachidhanandam U, Drillet Z, Edwards PJ. 2020. Demographic biases in engagement with nature in a tropical Asian city. PLoS ONE. 15(4):e0231576. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0231576.
  • Richardson M, Dobson J, Abson DJ, Lumber R, Hunt A, Young R, Moorhouse B. 2020. Applying the pathways to nature connectedness at a societal scale: a leverage points perspective. Ecosystems and People. 16(1):387–401. doi:10.1080/26395916.2020.1844296.
  • Richardson M, Hamlin I. 2021. Nature engagement for human and nature’s wellbeing during the Corona pandemic. J Public Ment Health. 1–11. doi:10.1108/JPMH-02-2021-0016.
  • Richardson M, Hamlin I, Butler CW, Thomas R, Hunt A. 2021. Actively noticing nature (not just time in nature) helps promote nature connectedness. Ecopsychology. 14(1):1–9. doi:10.1089/eco.2021.0023.
  • Richardson M, McEwan K. 2018. 30 days wild and the relationships between engagement with nature’s beauty, nature connectedness and well-being. Front Psychol. 9(SEP):1–9. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01500.
  • Richardson M, Passmore H, Barbett L, Lumber R, Thomas R, Hunt A, Fish R. 2020. The green care code: how nature connectedness and simple activities help explain pro-nature conservation behaviours. People and Nature. 2(3):1–19. doi:10.1002/pan3.10117.
  • Riechers M, Balázsi Á, Abson DJ, Fischer J. 2020. The influence of landscape change on multiple dimensions of human–nature connectedness. Ecology and Society. 25(3):1–12. doi:10.5751/ES-11651-250303.
  • Riechers M, Balázsi Á, García-Llorente M, Loos J. 2021. Human-nature connectedness as leverage point. Ecosys-tems and People. 17(1):215–221. doi:10.1080/26395916.2021.1912830.
  • Rosa CD, Collado S. 2019. Experiences in nature and environmental attitudes and behaviors: setting the ground for future research. Front Psychol. 10(763):1–9. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00763.
  • Salazar G, Kunkle K, Monroe M. (2020). Practitioner Guide to Assessing Connection to Nature. https://cdn.naaee.org/sites/default/files/assessing_connection_to_nature.7.23.20.pdf
  • Schultz PW. 2002. Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human-nature relations. In: Schmuck P Schultz WP, editors. Psychology of sustainable development. Springer Science and Business Media; pp. 61–78. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4.
  • Schultz PW, Tabanico JJ. 2007. Self, identity, and the natural environment: exploring implicit connections with nature. J Appl Soc Psychol. 37(6):1219–1247. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00210.x.
  • Schutte NS, Malouff JM. 2018. Mindfulness and connectedness to nature: a meta-analytic investigation. Pers Individ Dif. 127:10–14. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.034.
  • Schuttler SG, Sorensen AE, Jordan RC, Cooper C, Shwartz A. 2018. Bridging the nature gap: can citizen science reverse the extinction of experience? Front Ecol Environ. 16(7):405–411. doi:10.1002/fee.1826.
  • Scopelliti M, Carrus G, Adinolfi C, Suarez G, Colangelo G, Lafortezza R, Panno A, Sanesi G. 2016. Staying in touch with nature and well-being in different income groups: the experience of urban parks in Bogotá. Landsc Urban Plan. 148:139–148. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.11.002.
  • Shanahan DF, Cox DTC, Fuller RA, Hancock S, Lin BB, Anderson K, Bush R, Gaston KJ. 2017. Variation in experiences of nature across gradients of tree cover in compact and sprawling cities. Landsc Urban Plan. 157:231–238. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.07.004.
  • Souter-Brown G. 2015. Landscape and urban design for health and well-being: using healing, sensory and therapeutic gardens. London (UK): Routledge.
  • Tam KP. 2013. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. J Environ Psychol. 34:64–78. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004.
  • Tester-Jones M, White MP, Elliott LR, Weinstein N, Grellier J, Economou T, Bratman GN, Cleary A, Gascon M, Korpela KM, et al. 2020. Results from an 18 country cross-sectional study examining experiences of nature for people with common mental health disorders. Sci Rep. 10(1):1–11. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-75825-9.
  • United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. 2019. World Urbanization Prospects. In The 2018 Revision (ST/ESA/SER.A/420). https://population.un.org/wup/Publications/Files/WUP2018-Report.pdf
  • Whitburn J, Linklater W, Abrahamse W. 2019. Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Conservation Biology. 34(1):180–193. doi:10.1111/cobi.13381.
  • Wyles KJ, Pahl S, Holland M, Thompson RC. 2017. Can beach cleans do more than clean-up litter? Comparing beach cleans to other coastal activities. Environ Behav. 49(5):509–535. doi:10.1177/0013916516649412.
  • Wyles KJ, White MP, Hattam C, Pahl S, King H, Austen M. 2019. Are some natural environments more psychologically beneficial than others? The importance of type and quality on connectedness to nature and psychological restoration. Environ Behav. 51(2):111–143. doi:10.1177/0013916517738312.
  • Yang Y, Hu J, Jing F, Nguyen B. 2018. From awe to ecological behavior: the mediating role of connectedness to nature. Sustainability (Switzerland). 10(7):2477. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10072477.
  • Zhao X, Lynch JG, Chen Q. 2010. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. J Consum Res. 37(2):197–206. doi:10.1086/651257.
  • Zylstra MJ, Knight AT, Esler KJ, Le Grange LLL. 2014. Connectedness as a core conservation concern: an interdisciplinary review of theory and a call for practice. Springer Science Reviews. 2(1–2):119–143. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40362-014-0021-3.