153
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Social cognitive training for adults with Noonan syndrome: a feasibility study

, , &
Pages 611-626 | Published online: 26 Feb 2019

Abstract

Purpose

Noonan syndrome (NS) is a genetic disorder that is associated with social cognitive problems. While treatment aimed at the improvement of social cognition is available for other neuropsychiatric disorders, no such interventions yet exist for NS patients. In this study, the development of the first social cognitive training for NS patients is described and its applicability and feasibility evaluated.

Methods

Eleven adult patients with NS participated in this controlled proof-of-principle study. Six patients were included in the treatment group and five in the control group. Neuropsychological testing was performed in both groups at baseline and posttreatment. Social cognition was a primary outcome measure and nonsocial cognition and psychopathology secondary outcome measures. Differences between pre- and posttest were investigated with Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, and a process evaluation was performed to aid interpretation of the results.

Results

Both groups were comparable with regard to age, estimated intelligence, and baseline performance. Although no significant differences were found between pre- and posttest scores on primary and secondary outcome measures in either group, a medium–large effect size was found on emotion recognition in the treatment group. Also, the process evaluation demonstrated the feasibility of the training.

Conclusion

This first social cognitive training for adult patients with NS has proven to be feasible for this population and showed some encouraging results regarding emotion recognition, although the training protocol could be optimized. Further investigation is required using a randomized controlled design in a larger sample, in order to substantiate the overall effectiveness of the training.

Introduction

Noonan syndrome (NS) is an autosomal-dominant genetic disorder characterized by distinctive facial features, congenital heart disease, short stature, and varying degrees of developmental delay. Other features include chest deformities, a broad or webbed neck, cryptorchidism, coagulation defects, and lymphatic dysplasia.Citation1,Citation2 The estimated prevalence is one in 1,000–2,500 live births.Citation3 NS is caused by germ-line mutations encoding components of the Ras–MAPK pathway, a signal-transduction cascade involved in several developmental processes, such as cell-cycle regulation, differentiation, growth, apoptosis, and cell senescence.Citation2,Citation4 NS is one of the “RASopathies”, referring to a class of related developmental disorders that are caused by gene mutations, resulting in dysregulation of the Ras–MAPK pathway.Citation5 To date, mutations in 17 genes have been associated with NS, of which those in PTPN11 (50%), SOS1 (10%), and RAF1 (10%) are most frequently identified.Citation6 In approximately 75% of patients with NS, a genetic mutation can be identified, although diagnosis is still primarily based on clinical criteria.Citation7Citation9

In children with NS, difficulties have been found in several cognitive domains, ie, language, motor function, attention, executive function, and social cognition.Citation10 Regarding the cognitive profile of adults with NS, (low) average intelligence and lowered speed of information processing were found in comparison to matched controls.Citation11 Performance on other cognitive domains, such as memory, executive functioning, and visuoconstruction, seems to be relatively intact in adulthood, although patients report subjective executive problems.Citation11 With regard to social cognition – the mental processes underlying social functioningCitation12 – in adults with NS, difficulty in identifying and describing one’s own emotional feelings (ie, higher levels of alexithymia), slight overall problems in facial emotion recognition and social discomfort have been described.Citation13 Alexithymia (from Greek for ‘no words for emotions’) is a dimensional personality trait, reflecting weaknesses in cognitive processing and regulation of one’s own emotions. Alexithymia is characterized by difficulties in identifying and describing feelings, limited imaginal capacity, and an externally oriented cognitive style.Citation20 In a variety of clinical and healthy populations, alexithymia has been related to social cognitive impairments, such as problems in the automatic processing of emotional stimuli, affect recognition, empathy, and mentalizing.Citation65Citation76 Studies on a behavioral level in patients with NS showed higher prevalence of autism traits and weaker social skills, although it should be noted that these studies focused predominantly on children and only two included a (normally developing) control group.Citation14Citation17 Even though only a few studies have investigated the presence of psychiatric disorders in adults with NS, mood and anxiety problems are suggested to be rather frequent.Citation3,Citation18,Citation19 Despite the mental health risks associated with social cognitive impairments and alexithymia,Citation12,Citation20 no social cognitive intervention incorporating alexithymia training is yet available for patients with NS.

Various social cognitive interventions have demonstrated improvements in social cognition in adult neuropsychiatric patients (ie, patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, autism-spectrum disorders, and acquired brain injury).Citation21 These treatments included targeted interventions, focusing on one social cognitive subdomain, eg, emotion recognition or theory of mind (ToM), comprehensive interventions that addressed more than one social cognitive subdomain, eg, emotion recognition and ToM training, and broad-based interventions in which social cognition training was embedded in the context of other interventions, such as neurocognitive remediation or social skill training. In general, these social cognitive interventions were most effective in improving emotion perception, but ToM, social perception, and social functioning could also be trained effectively. Training of emotion perception appears to be an important component of effective social cognitive interventions, while addressing social functioning and providing sufficient practice opportunities in daily life are beneficial for generalization.Citation21 Some well-described social cognitive interventions for various neuropsychiatric disorders include social cognition and interaction training,Citation22 treatment of affect recognition,Citation23 cognitive enhancement therapy,Citation24 and treatment for impairments in social cognition and emotion regulation.Citation25 Several social cognitive interventions exist for children with autism-spectrum disorder, but well-studied treatments for adults are scarce, although results seem promising.Citation21,Citation26 Given the fact that alexithymia is related to social cognitive difficulties, it is remarkable that social cognitive interventions for neuropsychiatric disorders do not address alexithymia.

Almost four decades ago, KrystalCitation27 recommended the use of specific therapeutic techniques in alexithymic patients, such as making patients aware of their alexithymic problems and emotional feelings, developing affect tolerance, and providing psychoeducation regarding emotions (ie, their signal function, duration, and intensity), and helping patients to recognize and verbalize their emotions. A more recent narrative review on psychological interventions aimed at reducing alexithymia in psychiatric, medical, and healthy populations suggests that alexithymia is partly modifiable and that reductions in alexithymia scores can be obtained when alexithymia is directly trained.Citation28 Some studies have used relaxation techniques and mindfulness to diminish stress levels and increase awareness of inner experiences and physical sensations.Citation29Citation31 Psychoeducation regarding emotions and their function, as well as the identification of antecedents of alexithymia (eg, emotional expression in family of origin), have also been described.Citation30Citation32 Since alexithymia is correlated with decrements in emotion recognition, many alexithymia interventions incorporate training of emotion perception, identification, and differentiation.Citation29Citation33 Mentalizing and differentiating bodily feelings, thoughts, emotions, and behavior have also been addressed.Citation31,Citation32 In order to improve verbalization of emotions, training affect labeling, extension of the “emotional vocabulary”, and training of self-expression skills are used.Citation31Citation33 Listening to music, creating fantasy stories, creative writing, noting dreams, reading literary fiction, or creating poetry can stimulate fantasizing.Citation29,Citation34 Lastly, Taylor and BagbyCitation20 suggested that alexithymia interventions are preferably provided in a group-therapy setting, because a group offers relevant interpersonal practice. However, they suggest that the groups should be small and consist of no more than three patients.

Because the existing social cognitive treatments do not incorporate alexithymia training, they are not completely suitable for NS patients, in whom specific difficulties in the identification and verbalization of their own emotions have been demonstrated. For this reason, a social cognitive training was developed, based on the promising results of previous social cognitive and alexithymia interventions and tailored to the specific (social) cognitive profile of adult patients with NS. The training consists of ten weekly group sessions comprising the three phases of social cognition: perception of one’s own emotions and the emotions of others, interpretation and integration of socially relevant information, and execution and regulation of social behavior.Citation35 In this proof-of-principle study, the development, applicability, and feasibility of the new training protocol will be described in a group of patients with NS in comparison to an NS control group. Social cognition, operationalized by the three aforementioned phases of affective information processing, was a primary outcome measure. Given the cognitive profile of patients with NS and relation with social cognition, nonsocial cognition (speed of information processing, inhibition, planning abilities), psychopathology ([social] anxiety, depression), quality of life, and self-efficacy were the secondary outcome measures.

Methods

Design and procedure

Pilot sessions of the training were held at meetings of the Dutch Noonan Syndrome Foundation to investigate both interest and support for a social cognitive training, as well as to obtain input from patients and their relatives regarding important aspects of everyday functioning, eg, inclusion of training partners. Subsequently, a training protocol was designed by RR and EW, including important elements of evidence-based social cognitive interventions (eg, training of emotion recognition, ToM, social behavior) and exercises from alexithymia treatments. This resulted in a structured, 10-week group intervention of 90-minute sessions in which the three phases of social cognition were addressed (perception, interpretation, and reaction). The training protocol included psychoeducation and in-session exercises, incorporating emotion-recognition training, ToM and mentalizing strategies, and exercises to improve identification, labeling, and expression of (own) emotional feelings (see for topics and content per session). Participants also performed homework exercises aimed at increasing awareness of one’s own emotional feelings and social behavior and allowed them to practice with the training topics. Each training session was structured similarly, starting with the evaluation of homework exercises, psychoeducation concerning a social cognitive phase and/or subdomain, practice in the group, and introducing new homework. The group size was intentionally limited, with a maximum of five patients per group. Participants were asked to select a training partner, who was invited for two training sessions and performed homework exercises with the patients, in order to provide practice opportunities in daily life.

Table 1 Overview of topic and content of each training session

The current study had a controlled pre- vs posttest within-subject design with a treatment group and a control group. Patients in the control group received treatment as usual (eg, psycho-education, psychological counseling, consultation with general practitioner and/or medical specialist). Before and after training, a neuropsychological test battery, including several (social) cognitive tasks and self-report questionnaires, was administered in both groups. The primary outcome measures reflected all three phases of affective information processing, thus including emotion recognition, ToM, social perception, alexithymia, and social behavioral problems. As secondary outcome measures, subjective and objective measures of nonsocial cognition, psychopathology, quality of life, and self-efficacy were included. Proxies of patients in the treatment group were asked to complete two questionnaires as well. Training sessions were held at a central location in the Netherlands within an acceptable travel distance for all participants. For patients in the treatment group, pre- and posttests were planned at the same location and combined with the first and the last training session, if possible. Participants in the control group were tested at a location of their preference. However, most testing took place at the same location as for the treatment group and at about the same time. Control participants received a gift voucher as compensation for their travel expenses. Further information regarding the procedure is provided in the process analysis in .

Table 2 Results from the process analysis organized as per process-evaluation components, related process measures, and process variables

Participants

Eleven patients with NS participated in this study. Four more patients were eligible for inclusion, but did not participate, due to current work or study obligations or nonresponse. Due to the small number of patients who volunteered to participate in the training study, it was not possible to allocate patients randomly to the treatment and control conditions. The treatment group consisted of six patients, who followed the training in two consecutive groups. The control group comprised five patients. Participants were recruited by the Centre of Excellence for Neuropsychiatry of Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, the Department of Medical Genetics of Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, and the Dutch Noonan Syndrome Foundation. Written informed consent was obtained, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Inclusion criteria were a confirmed clinical diagnosis of NS or other RASopathy, minimum age of 16 years, and sufficient verbal capacities estimated during the intake procedure by two neuropsychologists (RR and EW). The presence of subjective or objective social cognitive impairments was required for participation in the treatment group. More information regarding the recruitment and inclusion procedures is presented in .

The mean age in the treatment group was 47.7 years (SD 12.8, range 29–60) and 35.8 years in the control group (SD 9.8, range 23–46). Mean IQ, estimated by the Dutch version of the National Adult Reading Test (NART),Citation36 was 85.33 in the treatment group (SD 17.86, range 69–113) and 83.40 (SD 17.59, range 55–103) in the control group. Education level, according to the Dutch educational system and ranging from category 1 (<6 years of primary education) to 7 (academic degree),Citation37 varied from 3 to 7 in the treatment group (mode 4) and from 2 to 5 in the control group (mode 5). In the treatment group, 17% of the participants were men, while in the control group 60% men were included. Half the participants in the treatment group had a mutation in the PTPN11 gene compared to 40% in the control group. One patient in the treatment group was diagnosed with NS with multiple lentigines and one patient in the control group with NS with loose anagen hair. After a Mann–Whitney U test, there appeared to be no significant differences between the treatment group and the control group with regard to NART IQ (treatment median 78, control median 85, U=13, z=−0.37, r=−0.11; P=0.76), and age (treatment median 50 years, control median 40 years, U=7.50, z=−1.38, r=−0.42; P=0.19), although the effect size for age was medium–large.

Five patients in the treatment group reported previous psychological problems (mood problems, anxiety, stress-related psychological complaints, alcohol abuse), and three had current psychological complaints (mainly mood problems). Two patients were still receiving psychosocial counseling, which did not focus on improving social cognition. All participants in the control group had received psychological treatment once in their lives for anxiety and compulsions, posttraumatic stress disorder, suicidal ideation, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, or a history of being bullied. Two participants in the control group reported current psychological problems, and one was still receiving psychological and psychiatric treatment, not aimed at reducing social cognitive problems, at another mental healthcare facility.

Baseline measures

The Dutch version of the NART was administered at baseline as an estimate of intellectual functioning.Citation36

Primary outcome measures

Emotion recognition

The Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) was used as a measure of facial emotion recognition.Citation38 The ERT is a computer task in which participants are presented with dynamic images of the faces of four actors expressing the six basic emotional expressions: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. A computer program enables real-time interactive morphing between a neutral expression and the emotional expression in four intensities (40%, 60%, 80%, and 100%).Citation38 After each video clip, the participant is asked to choose among the six emotions, displayed as buttons on the test computer. Performance is defined by the total number of correctly identified emotional expressions. Scores range from 0 to 96, and higher scores represent better facial emotion recognition. The ERT has been validated in a wide range of clinical groups and normative data of healthy participants are available.Citation38

Theory of mind

Two subtests of the short Dutch version of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT-NL) were used as measures of ToM.Citation39,Citation40 A combined score of the subtests social inference – minimal (SI-M) and social inference – enriched (SI-E) was used, because these subtests measure aspects of ToM, while the first subtest – the emotion-evaluation test – addresses emotion recognition.Citation40 The TASIT-NL is the only ToM test that uses videos of actual persons in familiar social situations. Subtest SI-M includes nine videos with three sincere, three sarcastic, and three paradoxical sarcastic scenes. After each video, the participant has to answer four questions about what the actor was doing, saying, thinking, and feeling. Subtest SI-E consists of eight other videos (four lies, four sarcastic scenes) with additional contextual cues to assist the understanding of the situation. After each video, the participant has to answer the same four questions (do, say, think, feel). The combined score for the two subtests ranges from 0 to 68, with higher scores reflecting better ToM abilities. The TASIT-NL has two parallel forms: form A (used at pretest) and form B (at posttest). Although some differences were found between the two forms regarding difficulty at item level and presence of a sequence effect for subtest SI-M (lower performance on form B when participants first completed form A), no differences were found between forms A and B on subtest total scores and overall score, indicating that both forms are comparable. Overall performance on the alternate forms was correlated moderately (r=0.45).Citation40

Social perception

The social interpretation test (SIT) was used as a measure of social perception.Citation41 The SIT consists of a colored drawing depicting a social situation (an accident and reactions of people in the street). Nine open questions are asked to the participant, such as “Can you tell me something about the picture?”; “Do you think there is something striking or strange in the picture?”; and “How are these people involved in the situation?”. The SIT contains a scoring system with 24 statements, and correct answers are scored as 1 point. The SIT has a parallel version using a different yet comparable drawing. Form A was used at pretest and form B at posttest. A maximum score of 21 could be obtained, because only those statements that were thought to be plausible analogies between the two forms were used.Citation41 Performance on the two forms was highly correlated (r=0.82).Citation41

Alexithymia

The Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ) was administered to measure alexithymia.Citation42 This questionnaire consists of 40 items, which are rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The BVAQ includes five factor-based subscales (emotionalizing, fantasizing, identifying, analyzing, and verbalizing), and two higher-order scales (affective and cognitive dimensions). The total score on the BVAQ (40–200) was used in this study, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of alexithymia. Patients in both groups completed a self-report version of this questionnaire. In addition, for participants in the treatment group, a partner or other person close to the patient (ie, their training partner) was asked to complete a proxy version of this list constructed by the researchers. The reliability and validity of the self-report version of the BVAQ are acceptable, and normative data of healthy participants are available.Citation42,Citation43

Social behavioral problems

Based on the factor-analysis study of Bodenburg and Dopslaff,Citation44 a subscale of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire was calculated measuring social conventions and the ability to incorporate social interaction in one’s own behavior (DEX-SC).Citation45 The subscale consists of four items. Total scores range from 0 to 16, with higher scores reflecting more social behavioral problems. Patients in the treatment and control groups completed the self-report version of the DEX-SC, and for participants in the treatment group a proxy version of this list was completed by a partner or other person close to the patient (ie, their training partner).

Secondary outcome measures

Speed of information processing and inhibition

The Stroop color-word test (CWT) was assessed to measure processing speed and response inhibition.Citation46,Citation47 The average time to complete the first two conditions is used as a measure of mental speed. A ratio score (completion time on third condition divided by the average time on conditions 1 and 2) was calculated to reflect the ability to inhibit an automatic response. Lower ratio scores reflect better inhibition. The reliability of the Stroop CWT is considered good and construct validity sufficient.Citation48

Planning

An adapted version of the modified six-elements test (MSET), a subtest of the behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive-syndrome battery, was used to measure planning abilities.Citation45,Citation49 The adapted MSET was used, because it has two parallel forms (version 1 was used at pretest and version 2 at posttest), reduced ceiling effects in mildly impaired individuals, and a more differentiated scoring system.Citation50 In the adapted MSET, the original dictation subtask has been replaced with a sorting task. The other subtasks include arithmetic (version 1), categorizing pictures (version 1), picture naming (version 2), and categorizing words (version 2). The test procedure of the adapted MSET is comparable to the original task. In this study, the scoring method of the adapted MSET ([time of longest subtasks – time of shortest subtasks]/[number of executed tasks – rule breaks]) was used.Citation50 Lower adapted MSET scores indicate better planning performance. Normative data of healthy participants are available.Citation50

Social distress

The Scale for Interpersonal Behavior (SIB) was used to reflect engagement in social behavior and related distress.Citation51 The SIB includes 50 items reflecting social behavior, of which the participant has to evaluate (on two separate 5-point scales) the probability of engaging in this behavior (performance scale) and the level of stress it evokes (distress scale). For the purpose of this study, only the distress scale was used. Higher scores reflect higher levels of social distress. The construct validity and reliability of the SIB are considered good and criterion validity sufficient.Citation52 Normative data of healthy participants are available.Citation77

Anxiety and depression

The Dutch version of the Symptom Checklist 90 – revised (SCL-90-R) was used to measure general psychological complaints.Citation53 The questionnaire consists of 90 items, which are rated on a 5-point scale. For the purpose of this study, only the depression (16 items) and anxiety (10 items) subscales were administered. Higher scores reflect a higher level of complaints. The reliability, construct validity, and criterion validity of the SCL-90-R are considered good.Citation54 Normative data of healthy participants are available.Citation53

Quality of life and self-efficacy

Two short self-report questionnaires of quality of life (three items) and self-efficacy regarding social cognitive functioning (eight items) were administered. These questionnaires were developed by the authors, based on items of the Dutch version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire and the Lancashire Quality of Life Questionnaire,Citation55,Citation56 and adjusted to the NS population. Higher scores on these questionnaires reflect a higher level of quality of life or self-efficacy.

Data analyses

Pretest scores on the primary and secondary outcome measures of the treatment and control groups were compared using a Mann–Whitney U tests. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were performed to investigate pre- vs posttest differences in the treatment and control groups. Effect sizes were calculated manually using the equation: r=z/(√n).Citation57,Citation58 Analyses were performed with SPSS version 23.0.0.0.

For tests for which normative data of healthy participants were available and no adjusted scores were used in the current study (ie, ERT, BVAQ, MSET, SIB, SCL-90-R), individual test scores were classified as low (<−2 SD), below average (−1.5 to −2 SD), above average (1.5–2 SD), or high (>2 SD). A process evaluation (ie, a structured exploration of each process of an intervention) was added, as it can provide valuable information regarding the understanding of both positive and negative outcome results.Citation59 In the current study, the process evaluation was structured into three components, as suggested by Reelick et al:Citation59 success rate of recruitment and quality of study population, quality of execution of complex intervention, and process of acquisition of evaluation data.

Results

Baseline testing

The results of the treatment group and control group at baseline are displayed in . On primary and secondary outcome measures, no significant differences were found between the groups, even though medium–large effect sizes were present for differences on the ERT, DEX-SC, quality of life, self-efficacy, SCL-90-R – anxiety, and Stroop CWT – inhibition.

Table 3 Baseline performance of treatment group compared to control group

Table 4 Results of treatment-group patients on primary outcome measures at pre- and posttest

Table 5 Results of control-group patients on primary outcome measures at pre- and posttest

Table 6 Results of treatment-group patients on secondary outcome measures at pre- and posttest

Table 7 Results of control-group patients on secondary outcome measures at pre- and posttest

Compared to normative data, one patient in the treatment group had below-average performance on the ERT and one patient in this group performed at a low level. Two patients had above-average alexithymia levels on the BVAQ and two patients had high levels. For two patients, proxy reports showed above-average levels of alexithymia, while for two patients high levels of alexithymia were reported by proxies. Two patients reported high levels of depression on the SCL-90-R. In the control group, two patients reported high levels of alexithymia. Two patients in this group reported high levels of depression on the SCL-90-R and one patient reported high levels of anxiety on the same questionnaire. One patient reported an above-average level of social distress on the SIB and one patient reported high levels of social distress. One patient showed a low planning performance on the MSET. The classifications are displayed in .

Pretest vs posttest

There appeared to be no significant differences on primary outcome measures between pre- and posttest scores in the treatment group or the control group ( and ). However, the effect size of improvement on the ERT was medium–large in the treatment group, while it was only small–medium in the control group.

Regarding secondary outcome measures, there were no significant differences between pre- and posttest scores in either group, although improvement on the adapted MSET in the treatment group and the faster performance of the control group on Stroop CWT processing speed almost reached significance and effect sizes were large ( and ). Despite the lack of significant results, the effect size of the improvement in inhibition scores on the Stroop CWT in the treatment group was medium–large, while decreased performance on this task in the control group showed a large effect. Furthermore, large effects were found in the control group with regard to the (nonsignificant) improvement in planning abilities on the adapted MSET and the decrease in anxiety on the SCL-90-R, while a medium effect was found for the decrease in depression. Since both groups showed a (medium to) large effect on improvement on the adapted MSET, this most likely reflected a practice effect.

Process evaluation

In , detailed information regarding the process evaluation is displayed. The results of the process analysis regarding selection of the study population showed that the number of included patients was smaller than intended. It is hypothesized that the lower inclusion may have been influenced by the nature of the (social) cognitive deficits in patients with NS. There were no dropouts among the included patients.

With regard to the evaluation of the intervention itself, the results of the process evaluation indicated that all three phases of the training were delivered successfully. All participants reported benefiting from the training. Increased awareness of social cognitive difficulties (related to NS) and the acquisition of useful strategies to improve social cognitive functioning were mentioned most frequently, though participants also reported that they had experienced the training as intensive and demanding in terms of the emotional load and practical issues (eg, time investment). In addition to the content of the training, participants also benefited from the support they felt from their group members. It was observed that patients were inclined to spend much time on sharing their experiences in a detailed manner during the sessions, at the expense of training time. Supposedly, most patients with NS seldom meet other patients and greatly valued the recognition of one another’s experiences and (social cognitive) problems. Alternatively, the observed tendency to share experiences extensively may reflect avoidance of (potentially distressing) exercises during the training or may be the result of verbosity, caused by difficulties in executive functioning. Although homework exercises were performed satisfactorily, participants tended to avoid exercises with their training partners. Patients who involved their training partners seemed to profit from the participation of their proxies. Three of the five training partners who shared their experiences after training reported that the participants benefited from the training in their opinion.

Lastly, the results of the process evaluation of outcome measures showed that the neuropsychological test data were complete, but that not all questionnaires were returned. This could suggest a difficulty with completing the questionnaires. The missing data may have influenced the findings in this study, because conservative methods were used to compensate for these. The perceived benefit by patients and their training partners was not reflected by the quantitative data.

Discussion

In the present study, applicability and feasibility of a new social cognitive training for adult patients with NS was evaluated. The training appeared feasible, and promising results were found with regard to enhancement of emotion recognition, although the overall effectiveness of the training could not be quantified, due to the small sample.

With regard to baseline measures, no significant differences were found between the treatment and control groups. However, several outcome measures showed medium–large effect sizes when comparing the two groups, which may suggest that meaningful differences between the treatment and control groups on these variables could not be demonstrated due to a power problem. Baseline variables that potentially differed were higher age, poorer emotion recognition, lower levels of quality of life and self-efficacy, more social behavioral problems, more anxiety, and a worse inhibition score in the treatment group in comparison to the control group. Possible baseline differences between the groups might have resulted from the inclusion of patients with subjective or objective social cognitive impairments in the treatment group.

Within each group, no significant differences between pre- and posttest were found for primary outcome measures. However, the medium–large effect size of the pre- vs posttest-difference on ERT in the treatment group compared to the small–medium effect sizes in the control group may indicate that the effects on emotion recognition were stronger in the treatment group. An improvement in emotion recognition was expected, because it has been a consistent finding in other social cognitive interventions.Citation21 Finding no significant improvement in the treatment group on subjective measures of social cognitive functioning may reflect increased awareness of social cognitive difficulties. Awareness of one’s (social) cognitive problems can be considered an initial step toward behavioral change.

Regarding secondary outcome measures, no significant differences were found between pre- and posttest in either group. However, the improvement in adapted MSET scores in the treatment group and faster processing on the Stroop CWT in the control group almost reached significance. In addition, the medium–large effect sizes of the (nonsignificant) improvement on the Stroop CWT – inhibition in the treatment group and the (nonsignificant) improvement on the adapted MSET and SCL-90-R and (nonsignificant) decrease on Stroop CWT – inhibition in the control group may reflect differences that could not be demonstrated by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test due to power problems. Moreover, large effects were found in both groups with regard to the (non-significant) improvement in planning abilities on the adapted MSET, which most likely reflected a practice effect.

The process evaluation showed that there were no dropouts, all three phases of the intervention were feasible, and patients reported benefits from the training. Strong aspects concerned the (small) group-based format, in which NS patients learned from one another’s experiences, gained more awareness of their social cognitive difficulties, and were able to practice strategies. The number of patients included in this study was smaller than intended, which could be related to the nature of the (social) cognitive deficits in patients with NS. Providing more information concerning the beneficial effects of group training and considering alternative training locations in order to reduce the time investment may be helpful in this regard. eHealth interventions can also be considered, although exercising social skills in real-life interaction remains necessary. The evaluation also showed that more time may be spent on practicing social cognitive skills and performing exercises and to a lesser extent on psychoeducation and discussion of experiences and social cognitive complaints. Additional exercises can be considered, focusing for instance on emotion-regulation strategies, task-concentration training, and elements of acceptance and commitment therapy, which may be helpful in diminishing alexithymic problems and social anxiety and in increasing acceptance of emotions. Furthermore, training partners were less involved in the training than intended, which could have hampered the generalization of training effects to daily life. The selection of appropriate training partners, their role, and the benefits of their involvement should be addressed more elaborately during both the intake procedure and the sessions in which they are present. It also appeared that patients had some difficulty with filling out the questionnaires. Supervised administration of the questionnaires and the use of shorter instruments, eg, replacing the BVAQ with the shorter Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20, may be helpful in this regard.Citation60 Furthermore, a revision of the test battery is suggested. Although the TASIT has many benefits (ie, alternate forms and videos of real-life social situations) and is used frequently, the Dutch version is still time-consuming and its suitability for repeated administration is questionable.Citation61 Other ToM tasks may be considered, eg, a recognition of faux pas task.Citation62Citation64

Despite the fact that this is the first controlled study to evaluate a new social cognitive training for adult patients with NS, there are also some limitations. Firstly, the study sample was small, which has consequences for the power. Furthermore, because subjective or objective social cognitive impairments were required for inclusion in the treatment group, a selection bias could be present, which limits the interpretation of pre- vs posttest results to this group. Although no baseline differences were found between the groups, the presence of several medium–large effect sizes on baseline comparisons may suggest that meaningful differences between the groups could not be demonstrated due to limited statistical power. Also, patients were not randomly allocated to the groups, and we emphasize that these preliminary findings should be interpreted with caution and replicated in a randomized controlled trial. Because only nonparametric analyses were performed, no interaction effects between group (treatment vs control) and time (pre vs post) could be explored. As a result of the large heterogeneity in cognitive functioning in the NS population, possible outliers may have influenced the results of the analyses. However, these effects are considered minimal, due to the use of nonparametric testing. Lastly, all participants in the treatment group underwent neuropsychological assessment at the Center of Excellence of Neuropsychiatry for different periods before the training (approximately 1–8 years). Some of the outcome measures in the current test battery overlapped with this neuropsychological assessment, which could have led to practice effects.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this first social cognitive training for adult patients with NS, based on existing evidence-based and best-practice social cognition and alexithymia interventions, has proven to be a feasible training for this population. Moreover, encouraging results were found with regard to the enhancement of emotion recognition. Based on the process evaluation, some alterations in the training protocol and test battery may be helpful in order to optimize the procedure. Future studies are needed using a larger sample and randomized controlled design, in order to evaluate further the effectiveness of the training.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • AllansonJERobertsAENoonan syndromeAdamMPArdingerHHPagonRAGeneReviewsSeattle (WA)University of Washington2016
  • RobertsAEAllansonJETartagliaMGelbBDNoonan syndromeLancet2013381986333334223312968
  • NoonanJANoonan syndromeGoldsteinSReynoldsCRHandbook of Neurodevelopmental and Genetic Disorders in AdultsNew YorkThe Guilford Press2005308319
  • TidymanWERauenKAPathogenetics of the RASopathiesHum Mol Genet201625R2R123R13227412009
  • RauenKAThe RASopathiesAnnu Rev Genomics Hum Genet201314135536923875798
  • TajanMPaccoudRBrankaSEdouardTYartAThe RASopathy family: consequences of germline activation of the Ras/MAPK pathwayEndocr Rev201839567670029924299
  • TartagliaMGelbBDZenkerMNoonan syndrome and clinically related disordersBest Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab201125116117921396583
  • TurnerAMNoonan syndromeJ Paediatr Child Health20145010E14E2021771153
  • van der BurgtINoonan syndromeOrphanet J Rare Dis2007241617204147
  • PierpontEINeuropsychological functioning in individuals with Noonan syndrome: a systematic literature review with educational and treatment recommendationsJ Pediatr Neuropsychol201621–21433
  • WingbermühleERoelofsRLvan der BurgtICognitive functioning of adults with Noonan syndrome: a case-control studyGenes Brain Behav201211778579322783933
  • BeauchampMHAndersonVSocial: an integrative framework for the development of social skillsPsychol Bull20101361396420063925
  • WingbermühleEEggerJIMVerhoevenWMAvan der BurgtIKesselsRPCAffective functioning and social cognition in Noonan syndromePsychol Med2012420241942621740625
  • AdvientoBCorbinILWidjajaFAutism traits in the RASopathiesJ Med Genet2014511102024101678
  • AlfieriPPicciniGCacioloCBehavioral profile in RASopathiesAm J Med Genet A20141644934942
  • GargSBrooksABurnsAAutism spectrum disorder and other neurobehavioural comorbidities in rare disorders of the Ras/MAPK pathwayDev Med Child Neurol201759554454928160302
  • PierpontEIHudockRLFoyAMSocial skills in children with RASopathies: a comparison of Noonan syndrome and neurofibromatosis type 1J Neurodev Disord201810111129329511
  • SmpokouPTworog-DubeEKucherlapatiRSRobertsAEMedical complications, clinical findings, and educational outcomes in adults with Noonan syndromeAm J Med Genet A2012158A123106311123165751
  • VerhoevenWWingbermühleEEggerJvan der BurgtITuinierSNoonan syndrome: psychological and psychiatric aspectsAm J Med Genet A2008146A219119618080322
  • TaylorGJBagbyMThe alexithymia personality dimensionWidigerTAThe Oxford Handbook of Personality DisordersNew YorkOxford University Press2012648673
  • RoelofsRLWingbermühleEEggerJIMKesselsRPCSocial cognitive interventions in neuropsychiatric patients: a meta-analysisBrain Impairment20171801138173
  • RobertsDLPennDLCombsDRSocial Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT), Group Psychotherapy for Schizophrenia and Other Psychotic Disorders: Clinician GuideNew YorkOxford University Press2016
  • WölwerWFrommannNHalfmannSPiaszekAStreitMGaebelWRemediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity of a new training programSchizophr Res2005802–329530316125367
  • HogartyGEGreenwaldDPCognitive Enhancement Therapy: The Training ManualPittsburgh, PAUniversity of Pittsburgh2006
  • Westerhof-EversHJVisser-KeizerACFasottiLEffectiveness of a treatment for impairments in social cognition and emotion regulation (T-ScEmo) after traumatic brain injury: a randomized controlled trialJ Head Trauma Rehabil201732529630728786854
  • Bishop-FitzpatrickLMinshewNJEackSMA systematic review of psychosocial interventions for adults with autism spectrum disordersJ Autism Dev Disord201343368769422825929
  • KrystalHAlexithymia and psychotherapyAm J Psychother19793311731464164
  • CameronKOgrodniczukJHadjipavlouGChanges in alexithymia following psychological intervention: a reviewHarv Rev Psychiatry201422316217824736520
  • BeresnevaiteMExploring the benefits of group psychotherapy in reducing alexithymia in coronary heart disease patients: a preliminary studyPsychother Psychosom200069311712210773774
  • ByrneGBogueJEganRLonerganE“Identifying and describing emotions”: measuring the effectiveness of a brief, alexithymia-specific, intervention for a sex offender populationSex Abuse201628759961925420556
  • NeumannDMalecJFHammondFMReductions in Alexithymia and emotion dysregulation after training emotional Self-Awareness following traumatic brain injury: a phase I trialJ Head Trauma Rehabil201732528629528060205
  • KennedyMFranklinJSkills-based treatment for alexithymia: an exploratory case seriesBehav Change2002193158171
  • LevantRFHalterMJHaydenEWWilliamsCMThe efficacy of alexithymia reduction treatment: a pilot studyJ Mens Stud20091717584
  • SamurDTopsMSchlinkertCQuirinMCuijpersPKooleSLFour decades of research on alexithymia: moving toward clinical applicationsFront Psychol201341423382719
  • AdolphsRThe neurobiology of social cognitionCurr Opin Neurobiol200111223123911301245
  • SchmandBLindeboomJVan HarskampFNederlandse Leestest voor Volwassenen: Handleiding [Dutch Version of the National Adult Reading Test: Manual]Lisse, The NetherlandsSwets & Zeitlinger1992
  • DuitsAKesselsRSchatten van het premorbide functioneren [Estimating premorbid functioning]HendriksMKesselsRGorissenMNeuropsychologische diagnostiek: De klinische praktijk [Neuropsychological Assessment: Clinical practice]AmsterdamThe Netherlands Boom2014173186
  • KesselsRPMontagneBHendriksAWPerrettDIde HaanEHAssessment of perception of morphed facial expressions using the emotion recognition task: normative data from healthy participants aged 8–75J Neuropsychol201481759323409767
  • McDonaldSFlanaganSRollinsJKinchJTASIT: a new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injuryJ Head Trauma Rehabil200318321923812802165
  • Westerhof-EversHJVisser-KeizerACMcDonaldSSpikmanJMPerformance of healthy subjects on an ecologically valid test for social cognition: the short, Dutch version of the awareness of social inference test (TASIT)J Clin Exp Neuropsychol201436101031104125380130
  • VijftigschildWBergerHJCVan SpaendonckJASSociale ubterpretatie testVerbale interpretatie van een alledaags gebeuren [Social interpretation test Verbal interpretation of an everyday situation]Amsterdam, The NetherlandsSwets & Zeitlinger1969
  • VorstHCMBermondBValidity and reliability of the Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia questionnairePers Individ Dif2001303413434
  • MüllerJBühnerMEllgringHThe assessment of alexithymia: psychometric properties and validity of the Bermond–Vorst alexithymia questionnairePers Individ Dif2004372373391
  • BodenburgSDopslaffNThe Dysexecutive questionnaire advanced: item and test score characteristics, 4-factor solution, and severity classificationJ Nerv Ment Dis20081961757818195646
  • WilsonBAAldermanNBurgessPWBADS: Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive SyndromeKrabbendamLKalffACLondonPearson1996
  • BoumaJMMulderJAltenaEStroop kleur-woord test [Stroop color-word test]BoumaJMMulderJLindeboomJHandboek neuropsychologische diagnostiek [Handbook of neuropsychological assessment]Amsterdam, The NetherlandsPearson Assessment and Information B.V2012475509
  • StroopJRStudies of interference in serial verbal reactionsJ Exp Psychol1935186643662
  • EgberinkIJLHolly-MiddelkampFRVermeulenCSMStroop [Stroop Colour Word Test]COTAN review 2003 Available from: https://www-1cotandocumentatie-1nl-10004359x146f.hanhosting.vakliteratuur.info/beoordelingen/b/13536/stroop-kleur-woordtest/Accessed December 8, 2017 Dutch
  • BertensDFrankenmolenNBoelenDHKesselsRPFasottiLValidity of an adapted scoring method for a modified six elements test in individuals with brain injuryArch Clin Neuropsychol201530212212925547466
  • BertensDFasottiLEggerJIBoelenDHKesselsRPReliability of an adapted version of the modified six elements test as a measure of executive functionAppl Neuropsychol Adult2016231354226111243
  • ArrindellWABridgesKRvan der EndeJNormative studies with the scale for interpersonal behaviour (SIB): II. US students. A cross-cultural comparison with Dutch dataBehav Res Ther200139121461147911760731
  • EgberinkIJLHolly-MiddelkampFRVermeulenCSMSchaal voor Interpersoonlijk Gedrag [Scale for Interpersonal Behaviour]COTAN review 1998 Available from: https://www-1cotandoc-umentatie-1nl-10004359x146f.hanhosting.vakliteratuur.info/beoordelingen/b/14504/schaal-voor-interpersoonlijk-gedrag/Accessed December 8, 2017 Dutch
  • ArrindellWAEttemaJHMSCL90: Handleiding bij een multidimensionele psychopathologie-indicator [SCL90: Manual of a multidimensional psychopathology indicator]Lisse, The NetherlandsSwets & Zeitlinger2003
  • EgberinkIJLHolly-MiddelkampFRVermeulenCSMSymptom Checklist-90 RevisedCOTAN review 2004 Available from: https://www-1cotandocumentatie-1nl-10004359x146f.hanhosting.vakliteratuur.info/beoordelingen/b/13561/klachtenlijst/Accessed December 8, 2017 Dutch
  • JacobsNKleenMDe GrootFHet meten van experiëntiële vermijding: De Nederlandse versie van de acceptance and action questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) [Measuring experiential avoidance: The Dutch version of the acceptance and action questionnaire-II]Gedragstherapie200841349361
  • van NieuwenhuizenCScheneAHKoeterMWHuxleyPJThe Lancashire quality of life profile: modification and psychometric evaluationSoc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol2001361364411320806
  • RosenthalRMeta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research2nd edNewbury Park, CASage1991
  • PerdicesMNull hypothesis significance testing, p-values, effects sizes and confidence intervalsBrain Impairment20181917080
  • ReelickMFFaesMCEsselinkRAKesselsRPOlde RikkertMGHow to perform a preplanned process evaluation for complex interventions in geriatric medicine: exemplified with the process evaluation of a complex falls-prevention program for community-dwelling frail older fallersJ Am Med Dir Assoc201112533133621450245
  • BagbyRMParkerJDTaylorGJThe twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale – I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structureJ Psychosom Res199438123328126686
  • KellyMMcDonaldSFrithMHJAssessment and rehabilitation of social cognition impairment after brain injury: surveying practices of cliniciansBrain Impairment201718011135
  • GregoryCLoughSStoneVTheory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implicationsBrain2002125Pt 475276411912109
  • StoneVEBaron-CohenSKnightRTFrontal lobe contributions to theory of mindJ Cogn Neurosci19981056406569802997
  • CorcoranRMercerGFrithCDSchizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophreniaSchizophr Res19951715138541250
  • BirdGCookRMixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autismTransl Psychiatry201337e28523880881
  • GrynbergDChangBCorneilleOAlexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectivesPLoS One201278e4242922927931
  • LaneRDSechrestLRiedelRShapiroDEKaszniakAWPervasive emotion recognition deficit common to alexithymia and the repressive coping stylePsychosom Med200062449250110949094
  • ParkerJDTaylorGJBagbyRMAlexithymia and the recognition of facial expressions of emotionPsychother Psychosom1993593–41972028416096
  • MoriguchiYOhnishiTLaneRDImpaired self-awareness and theory of mind: an fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymiaNeuroimage20063231472148216798016
  • SwartMKortekaasRAlemanADealing with feelings: characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strategies and cognitive-emotional processingPLoS One200946e575119492045
  • DongesUSSuslowTAlexithymia and automatic processing of emotional stimuli: a systematic reviewRev Neurosci201728311827718496
  • NeumannDZupanBMalecJFHammondFRelationships between alexithymia, affect recognition, and empathy after traumatic brain injuryJ Head Trauma Rehabil2014291E18E27
  • MilosavljevicBCarter LenoVSimonoffEAlexithymia in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: its relationship to Internalising difficulties, sensory modulation and social cognitionJ Autism Dev Disord20164641354136726659552
  • PoquérusseJPastoreLDellantonioSAlexithymia and autism spectrum disorder: a complex relationshipFront Psychol2018911029410639
  • WearneTOsborne-CrowleyKRosenbergHDethierMMcDonaldSEmotion recognition depends on subjective emotional experience and not on facial expressivity: evidence from traumatic brain injuryBrain Inj Epub2018108
  • WilliamsCWoodRLAlexithymia and emotional empathy following traumatic brain injuryJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol201032325926719548166
  • ArrindellWADe GrootPMWalburgJADe schaal voor interpersoonlijk gedrag [The scale for interpersonal behavior]LisseSwets & Zeitlinger1984