88
Views
16
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention program of patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus: a conjoint analysis

, , , , , & show all
Pages 537-546 | Published online: 02 Nov 2011

Abstract

Background

Several studies suggest that lifestyle interventions can be effective for people with, or at risk for, diabetes. The participation in lifestyle interventions is generally low. Financial incentives may encourage participation in lifestyle intervention programs.

Objective

The main aim of this exploratory analysis is to study empirically potential effects of financial incentives on diabetes patients’ willingness to participate in lifestyle interventions. One financial incentive is negative (“copayment”) and the other incentive is positive (“bonus”). The key part of this research is to contrast both incentives. The second aim is to investigate the factors that influence participation in a lifestyle intervention program.

Methods

Conjoint analysis techniques were used to empirically identify factors that influence willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention. For this purpose diabetic patients received a questionnaire with descriptions of various forms of hypothetical lifestyle interventions. They were asked if they would be willing to participate in these hypothetical programs.

Results

In total, 174 observations were rated by 46 respondents. Analysis showed that money was an important factor independently associated with respondents’ willingness to participate. Receiving a bonus seemed to be associated with a higher willingness to participate, but having to pay was negatively associated with participation in the lifestyle intervention.

Conclusion

Conjoint analysis results suggest that financial considerations may influence willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs. Financial disincentives in the form of copayments might discourage participation. Although the positive impact of bonuses is smaller than the negative impact of copayments, bonuses could still be used to encourage willingness to participate.

View correction statement:
Erratum

Background

Lifestyle intervention programs can be defined as a planned and targeted approach to change the behavior of people with the aim of promoting health or preventing disease. Lifestyle intervention programs can have a positive impact on public health, for example, by reducing risk factors for diabetes complications like high blood pressure, high cholesterol level, and high body weight.Citation1Citation3 These interventions could potentially also slow down the increasing prevalence of type 2 diabetes mellitus,Citation4Citation6 expected to be 366 million worldwide by the year 2030.Citation7

A major challenge in lifestyle intervention programs is to increase willingness to join, as participation in such programs is in general relatively low. For instance, a weight-loss program for obese patients in the United States only had a 40% participation rate, ie, adherence and compliance.Citation8,Citation9 Also, only 15% of eligible persons participated in a German disease management program.Citation10 In the latter study, the main reason for participation was that their health insurer offered clients to join.

The lower the participation rate in a lifestyle program, the smaller the public health impact might be.Citation8,Citation11 It is therefore important to get a better understanding of peoples’ willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs. There are different techniques to measure why people participate in interventions. One of them is the conjoint analysis technique.Citation12Citation15 This quantifies the relative importance of characteristics of a lifestyle intervention in program participation decisions. Obviously, this information can also be used to explore why people are not willing to participate in lifestyle intervention programs. Previous conjoint analysis study results suggest that people are willing to pay to participate in lifestyle interventions but they do not seem willing to pay full program costs.Citation16 Moreover, individuals with high perceived risks seem willing to pay more compared with individuals with low perceived risks. An alternative technique for conjoint analysis is to use a standard questionnaire and simply ask why people decided to (or not) participate in a lifestyle intervention program. Studies that investigated reasons for not participating using this technique, suggest that affordability is a barrier to participate.Citation17,Citation18 For instance, potential participants indicate that they cannot afford to pay the relatively higher prices for better quality food or they cannot afford to pay for use of sport facilities. The conjoint analysis and standard questionnaire findings suggest that money is associated with participation in lifestyle intervention programs. Germany seems the first country to use positive financial incentives (bonuses) to encourage active participation in primary and secondary prevention.Citation19

To the best of the authors’ knowledge no empirical research has been performed to analyze the effect of various types of financial incentives on participation in lifestyle intervention programs. The main aim of this paper is to explore empirically potential differences of two types of financial incentives on diabetes patients’ willingness to participate in lifestyle interventions. One financial incentive is negative (“copayment”) and the other is positive (“bonus”). The key part of this research is to contrast both incentives. The secondary aim is to examine which other factors of a lifestyle intervention program are associated with diabetes patients’ willingness to participate.

Methods

Conjoint analysis

The conjoint analysis technique was used to examine which factors are associated with willingness to participate in a hypothetical lifestyle intervention program. In a conjoint analysis a hypothetical lifestyle intervention program is described in terms of characteristics (attributes). The technique presents hypothetical scenarios (vignettes) to respondents that vary with respect to the levels of the attributes. Citation20 Respondents are asked if they would be willing to participate in the hypothetical lifestyle intervention program. Conjoint analysis has an important advantage compared with standard questionnaire research: people are forced to make trade-offs and they cannot indicate that all factors of a lifestyle intervention program are of equal importance.

Literature to determine conjoint scenarios

A literature search was carried out to determine the vignette attributes. Search terms were: lifestyle program, lifestyle modification, physical activity, exercise, adherence, compliance, and participation. For this search the databases PubMed, Cochrane, PsycINFO, and Picarta were used.

Factors that seem to influence willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs include time, costs, counseling, the kind of sports activity in the program, and the composition of the group.Citation17,Citation18,Citation21Citation24 Based on the literature findings six attributes were created. They are presented in . Next, levels were assigned to the attributes. Majority of the attribute levels were dummies and the money attributes were chosen in a range wide enough to likely influence respondents hypothetical choices. Obviously, how much they influence the hypothetical choices is the empirical question trying to be answered.

Table 1 Attributes and levels

The main aim of this study was to test for potential differences between a negative (copayment) and a positive (bonus) financial incentive to participate in hypothetical lifestyle intervention programs. Therefore, the research population was randomly divided into two groups. One group received a conjoint questionnaire in which they had to pay a hypothetical copayment to participate in the hypothetical lifestyle intervention and the other group received a conjoint questionnaire in which they received a hypothetical bonus to participate in the hypothetical lifestyle intervention. Please note that all other attributes were exactly similar between both versions of the conjoint questionnaire. Appendix A presents the details of the design of the conjoint experiment that was used in this study.

Study population

Patient questionnaires were distributed in 2009 among type 2 diabetes patients by two specialized nurses in general practices. None of the diabetes patients had previously participated in a lifestyle intervention program, nor had complications or comorbidities. One nurse distributed 63 questionnaires to patients visiting the clinic and another nurse sent out 192 questionnaires to a random selection of her patients. All respondents were asked to complete the conjoint analysis on a voluntary basis. The study did not require approval of a medical ethics committee as the questionnaire did not include questions which can intervene in the psychological state of the participants.

Statistical analysis

Respondents’ characteristics were analyzed using frequencies. A logistic regression was used to analyze conjoint scenarios as the focus was on the willingness of respondents to participate in the hypothetical lifestyle program: answering categories yes vs no. Results of the linear probability model were also presented for ease of interpretation. The influence of respondents’ characteristics on the willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention program was also analyzed. The statistical program used for the analyses was Stata (v 11; StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).

As the key part of the analysis was to test for potential differences between the hypothetical copayment and bonus, a spline was included in the logistic regression analyses. A spline is a combination of variables in a regression analysis. It consists of a main effect, a dummy variable indicating a possible bend in the regression slope (the knot), and an interaction variable. The spline enables a test of whether the slope of the copayment differs from the slope of the bonus.Citation25 In other words, it was tested if the coefficients of copayment and bonus have the same impact on willingness to participate using an F-test. Moreover, the dummy variable enables a test for whether respondents’ values for not receiving a bonus and not having to pay copayment differ between scenarios in which they received a bonus compared with scenarios in which they had to pay a copayment. The main reason for including a spline in the regression models is that people might react differently to a scenario with a negative financial incentive compared with a scenario with a positive financial incentive. Appendix B gives a detailed explanation of the construction of the spline and a detailed explanation of its interpretation.

Results

Study population

In total, 174 observations were rated by 46 respondents (18% of the distributed questionnaires) implying that not all respondents answered four conjoint scenarios.

The majority of respondents were Dutch (97.7%), and men and women were equally represented (50.0%). Their mean age was 67.3 years (standard deviation 11.2) and about 25% was above 75 years of age. Mean body mass index (BMI) was 28.8 kg/m² (standard deviation 5.7). Twenty percent of the respondents had a low income and most of them were retired (53.7%). As the characteristics age, sex, nationality, and BMI of this study population correspond to those characteristics found in a large observational study on diabetic patients in the Netherlands,Citation26,Citation27 this sample can be considered representative of the Dutch diabetic population.

Regression analyses

presents the results of the regression models without correction for confounders. The signs of the logistic regression coefficients (B) represent the relations between the attribute levels and respondents’ willingness (or not) to participate. The P values are only statistically significant for the attribute “money” (P = 0.01) and the interaction “money*receiving” P = 0.050). As only these money attributes are statistically significant, it seems that only money influences willingness to participate. The positive sign of the “money” attribute suggests a positive association between money (either a bonus or a decreasing copayment) and willingness to participate. The negative sign of the interaction “receiving*money” indicates that the effect on participation is smaller in the case where the patient receives a bonus. The negative coefficient of “ receiving” reflects the difference of the zero option (the value of 0 of the attribute money) between respondents which had answered conjoint scenarios with only bonuses vs conjoint scenarios containing only copayments.

Table 2 Results of regression models based on the attributes of participation in the hypothetical lifestyle interventions (N = 46; N observations = 174)

The corresponding coefficient of the linear probability model suggest that money increases the willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention program with 0.08% per euro increase in bonus or decrease in copayment (B = 0.0008). However, the statistically significant interaction variable of the spline (B = −0.057; P = 0.050) suggests that the slope for paying money differs from the slope of receiving money. For paying money the slope is positive (main effect of money B = 0.0008; P = 0.014), but for receiving money the slope flattens out (for receiving money the B of the interaction variable “money*receiving” is added to the main effect [0.0008 + −0.0006 = 0.0002]). The (nontabulated) F-test was significant for both linear and logistic analyses (linear F = 4.93, P = 0.032; logistic F = 4.73, P = 0.029). Although the coefficients of the slope of receiving seem very small, in absolute terms with a bonus €500 per year the associated increase in willingness to participate is substantial (10%).

The results of the analyses that include background variables age, BMI, and sex are presented in . These additional analyses showed that the probability of participating is positively influenced by “limiting a lifestyle program to only diabetes patients” and “by supervision and counseling of an instructor” by 13.4% and 15.8%, respectively (linear probability model). Moreover, older people are less willing to participate in a lifestyle intervention program. Additional analyses showed that the increasing number of statistical significant attributes compared to was caused by the exclusion of respondents with missing background variables and not by the case-mix adjustments (results not shown). Since it is likely that income may influence the results we also present the analysis including income. However, information on income was only available for 32 respondents (). Even in this analysis with fewer respondents the effect of money on willingness to participate remains.

Table 3a Results of regression models based on the attributes of participation in the hypothetical lifestyle interventions: adjusted for gender, age, and BMI (N = 37; N observations = 158)

Table 3b Results of regression models based on the attributes of participation in the hypothetical lifestyle interventions: adjusted for gender, age, BMI, and income (N = 32; N observations = 125)

It is worth noting, however, that in and the dummy variable “receiving” seems statistically significant. This seems to indicate that the zero option (‘not having to pay a copayment’ in case of copayments or ‘not receiving a bonus’ in case of bonuses) is valued lower by respondents that received a questionnaire including bonuses compared with respondents that received a questionnaire with copayments.

Discussion

Lifestyle intervention programs can have a positive impact on public health but program participation is often quite low. This study investigated the factors, particular financial factors, underlying type 2 diabetes patients’ willingness to participate in a lifestyle program. A conjoint analysis was applied to measure respondents’ preferences regarding a lifestyle program. This study explored empirically potential differences between positive (bonus) and negative (copayment) financial incentives on diabetes patients’ willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs.

The main finding of this study suggests that copayment is inversely related to the willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention program. In other words, copayment can discourage participation in the program. This finding is consistent with an earlier finding using conjoint analysis, which showed unwillingness to pay by high-risk individuals for diabetes risk-reduction programs.Citation16 Receiving a bonus seems to be associated with a higher willingness to participate in the program although the coefficients for each euro received is smaller compared with the negative coefficients of copayments. This result seems also consistent with findings in the literature. Rewards for following a lifestyle program are less effective in contrast to rewards for attendance of vaccination or screening programs,Citation28 and there is insufficient evidence of the effectiveness of rewards to promote lifestyle changes.Citation29Citation31 The analysis corrected for potential confounders like age, BMI, sex, and income showing that the main conclusion does not change.

A strength of this study is that the conjoint analysis included both a copayment and a bonus. This enabled a test for possible differences between positive and negative incentives. The main advantage of a conjoint analysis is that it can be used to gain insights in the relative importance of intervention attributes and the way people value these attributes.

A limitation of this study is the small group of respondents. Although the 174 observations are sufficient to report the results on the vignette-level, a proper analysis among subgroups of patients requires a much larger sample.Citation32Citation34 Factors underlying willingness to participate in a lifestyle intervention should be studied more extensively. Therefore further research should preferably use larger samples facilitating analysis of relevant subgroups. Another limitation of this study is the limited knowledge on the external validity of the answers of the conjoint analysis. To what extent do the answers reflect hypothetical scenario choices made in everyday life? Although progress in comparing, stated in a questionnaire, with revealed preferences (people’s actual behavior) has been performed in other healthcare settings,Citation35 evidence in the field of health promotion is scarce. Therefore empirical research to test for potential differences between the impact of copayments and bonuses on willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs using revealed preferences compared with stated preferences is encouraged.

It would be interesting if future research could explore the differences in respondents’ values for not having to pay to participate (related to scenarios with a bonus) compared to having to pay a copayment (related to scenarios with copayments). It is worth taking these differences into account in study designs as this sample was split, but one could consider testing if this result remains the same if the same people value zeros compared with a bonus and with a copayment.

Conclusion

This study showed that financial considerations may influence willingness to participate in lifestyle intervention programs. Copayments could therefore discourage participation. Although the positive impact of bonuses is smaller than the negative impact of copayments, bonuses could still be used to encourage willingness to participate.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • ToobertDJGlasgowREStryckerLABarreraMJrRitzwollerDPWeidnerGLong-term effects of the Mediterranean lifestyle program: a randomized clinical trial for postmenopausal women with type 2 diabetesInt J Behav Nutr Phys Act20074117229325
  • Pi-SunyerXBlackburnGBrancatiFLReduction in weight and cardiovascular disease risk factors in individuals with type 2 diabetes: one-year results of the look AHEAD trialDiabetes Care20073061374138317363746
  • DaviesMJHellerSSkinnerTCEffectiveness of the diabetes education and self management for ongoing and newly diagnosed (DESMOND) programme for people with newly diagnosed type 2 diabetes: cluster randomised controlled trialBMJ20083367642491495
  • Diabetes Prevention Program Research GroupThe Diabetes Prevention Program (DPP): description of lifestyle interventionDiabetes Care200225122165217112453955
  • KnowlerWCBarrett-ConnorEFowlerSEReduction in the incidence of type 2 diabetes with lifestyle intervention or metforminN Engl J Med2002346639340311832527
  • LindstromJIlanne-ParikkaPPeltonenMSustained reduction in the incidence of type 2 diabetes by lifestyle intervention: follow-up of the Finnish Diabetes Prevention StudyLancet200636895481673167917098085
  • WildSRoglicGGreenASicreeRKingHGlobal prevalence of diabetes: estimates for the year 2000 and projections for 2030Diabetes Care20042751047105315111519
  • ZhaoGFordESLiCMokdadAHCompliance with physical activity recommendations in US adults with diabetesDiabet Med200825222122718201213
  • MokdadAHBowmanBAFordESVinicorFMarksJSKoplanJPThe continuing epidemics of obesity and diabetes in the United StatesJAMA2001286101195120011559264
  • JavaherPSeidelGDierksMLParticipation in disease management of a health insurance company: characteristics and assessment of the process and outcome parameters in the programmeJ Public Health20061413742
  • IcksARathmannWHaastertBClinical and cost-effectiveness of primary prevention of Type 2 diabetes in a ‘real world’ routine healthcare setting: model based on the KORA Survey 2000Diabet Med200724547348017381502
  • PhillipsKAMaddalaTJohnsonFRMeasuring preferences for health care interventions using conjoint analysis: an application to HIV testingHealth Serv Res20023761681170512546292
  • BishopAJMarteauTMArmstrongDWomen and health care professionals’ preferences for Down’s Syndrome screening tests: a conjoint analysis studyBJOG2004111877577915270923
  • van den BergBAlMvan ExelJKoopmanschapMBrouwerWEconomic valuation of informal care: conjoint analysis applied in a heterogeneous population of informal caregiversValue Health20081171041105018489514
  • van den BergBVan DommelenPStamPLaske-AldershofTBuchmuellerTSchutFTPreferences and choices for care and health insuranceSoc Sci Med200866122448245918400349
  • JohnsonFRManjunathRMansfieldCAClaytonLJHoergerTJZhangPHigh-risk individuals’ willingness to pay for diabetes risk-reduction programsDiabetes Care20062961351135616732020
  • ZieblandSThorogoodMYudkinPJonesLCoulterALack of willpower or lack of wherewithal? “Internal” and “external” barriers to changing diet and exercise in a three year follow-up of participants in a health checkSoc Sci Med1998464–54614659460826
  • ChinnDJWhiteMHarlandJDrinkwaterCRaybouldSBarriers to physical activity and socioeconomic position: implications for health promotionJ Epidemiol Community Health199953319119210396499
  • SchmidtHBonuses as incentives and rewards for health responsibility: a good thing?J Med Philos200833319822018567903
  • RyanMMajorKSkatunDUsing discrete choice experiments to go beyond clinical outcomes when evaluating clinical practiceJ Eval Clin Pract200511432833816011645
  • Culos-ReedSNRejeskiWJMcAuleyEOckeneJKRoterDLPredictors of adherence to behavior change interventions in the elderlyControl Clin Trials2000215 Suppl200S205S11018576
  • FerrandCPerrinCNasarreSMotives for regular physical activity in women and men: a qualitative study in French adults with type 2 diabetes, belonging to a patients’ associationHealth Soc Care Community200816551152018355245
  • JonesMJollyKRafteryJLipGYGreenfieldSBRUM Steering Committee‘DNA’ may not mean ‘did not participate’: a qualitative study of reasons for non-adherence at home- and centre-based cardiac rehabilitationFam Pract200724434335717630271
  • NagelkerkJReickKMeengsLPerceived barriers and effective strategies to diabetes self-managementJ Adv Nurs200654215115816553701
  • MarshLCCormierDRSpline Regression ModelsThousand Oaks, CASage Publications, Inc2002
  • StruijsJNvan TilJTBaanCAExperimenting with a Bundled Payment System for Diabetes Care in The Netherlands: The First Tangible Effects2009 Available from: http://www.rivm.nl/bibliotheek/rapporten/260224002.pdfAccessed September 6, 2011
  • StruijsJNBaanCAIntegrating care through bundled payments-lessons from The NetherlandsN Engl J Med20113641199099121410368
  • VoigtKIncentives, health promotion and equalityHealth Econ Policy Law201012120854723
  • JochelsonKPaying the patient: improving health using financial incentives2007 Available from: http://www.wpro.who.int/NR/rdonlyres/BCC2F5C3-B685-684FF1-AE77-39C52F4ED247/0/payingthepatient.pdfAccessed September 2, 2011
  • SutherlandKLeathermanSChristiansonJPaying the Patient: Does it Work? A Review of Patient-Targeted IncentivesLondon, UKThe Health Foundation2008
  • WilkinsonECan you pay people to be healthy?Lancet200837196211325132618429344
  • RyanMDiackJWatsonVSmithNRapid prenatal diagnostic testing for Down syndrome only or longer wait for full karyotype: the views of pregnant womenPrenat Diagn200525131206121116353286
  • BaltussenRStolkEChisholmDAikinsMTowards a multi-criteria approach for priority setting: an application to GhanaHealth Econ200615768969616491464
  • Cheraghi-SohiSBowerPMeadNMcDonaldRWhalleyDRolandMMaking sense of patient priorities: applying discrete choice methods in primary care using ‘think aloud’ techniqueFam Pract200724327628217478438
  • BlumenscheinKBlomquistGCJohannessonMHornNFreemanPEliciting willingness to pay without bias: evidence from a field experimentEcon J2008118525114137
  • LambooijMFlacheASandersKSiegersJEncouraging employees to co-operate: the effects of sponsored training and promotion practices on employees’ willingness to work overtimeInt J Hum Resource Manage2007181017481767
  • van den BergBAlMBrouwerWvan ExelJKoopmanschapMEconomic valuation of informal care: the conjoint measurement method applied to informal caregivingSoc Sci Med20056161342135515869835

Appendix A

Design conjoint analysis: attributes and levels

The combination of six attributes and levels (two attributes with four levels, one attribute with three levels, and one attribute with four levels) gave rise to 2Citation4 × 3Citation1 × 4Citation1 = 192 hypothetical lifestyle intervention programs (possible scenarios). Each participant randomly received a set of four out of 48 sets of scenarios (the 192 scenarios were clustered in 48 sets of four scenarios).

One of the challenges in designing a conjoint analysis experiment is to avoid the combination of unrealistic attributes and levels in one scenario.Citation36 In order to make sure that presented scenarios were realistic, several items were rephrased. Another design challenge is to avoid respondents being confronted with scenario-attributes that are similar (similarity in the set of four scenarios they received), resulting in reduced motivation for the respondents due to a boring task. Therefore, it was tested if more than three questionnaires had four or more scenario-attributes that were similar. If this was the case, a new randomization was started. A second measure was that the questionnaires were constructed by multiple vignette-universes (ie, a set of all possible vignette combinations): five datasets with the 192 vignettes were constructed and they were each separately randomly ordered. This meant that each unique vignette could be rated five times, but that it was always combined with other vignettes. This decreased the risk that any vignette was combined more than once with other vignettes that were similar. So even when a vignette was judged by a respondent who became less motivated because of too little variation between the vignettes, a similar vignette would be judged by another respondent who was still motivated because the choices in their questionnaire varied sufficiently.

Conjoint analysis: survey questions

Respondents were first asked to rate the four hypothetical lifestyle interventions with a grade ranging from “1” to “10,” where “1” was defined as really dislike the lifestyle intervention and “10” as really like the lifestyle intervention. The rating exercise measures which aspects of the lifestyle intervention participants valued.Citation14,Citation37 Subsequently, respondents were asked whether or not they would be willing to participate in the hypothetical lifestyle interventions (answering categories “yes” or “no”).

A communication expert was consulted to adjust the wording of the conjoint analysis questionnaire in an attempt to avoid bias due to incorrect interpretation of the conjoint questions. presents an example of a hypothetical life style intervention (a scenario).

Figure A1 Example scenario.

Note: *Results were not used in this paper.
Figure A1 Example scenario.

Appendix B

Spline in regression models

A spline enables a test of whether the slope of a negative financial incentive (“copayment”) differs from the slope of a positive financial incentive (“bonus”) for participation in the hypothetical lifestyle intervention (scenario).Citation25 The main reason for including a spline is that people might react differently to a scenario when they are asked to pay compared with receiving money for a similar scenario. A spline was constructed using three variables. The first variable expressed the amount of money received or paid (ranging from −€500 to €500; see ). By means of this variable, the association between the amount of money and the probability of participation in the lifestyle intervention is tested. The second variable is a dummy with the value 0 in case of a set of scenarios stating that the respondents would have to pay vs the value 1 for scenarios indicating that people would receive money in return for participation in the lifestyle intervention program. This dummy is called the “knot” and it defines the place where the slope of the model may change. Third, an interaction of these two variables was entered into the logistic regression analyses. When the slope before the knot (here: negative values of money, or the negative side of the x-axis) differs from the slope after the knot (here: positive values of money, or the positive side of the x-axis), this interaction will yield a statistically significant result. Together these three variables enable a test of whether the slopes and the continuance of the logistic regression analyses differ between negative and positive financial incentives for participation in hypothetical lifestyle intervention programs.