714
Views
19
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Conceptualizing anhedonias and implications for depression treatments

, &
Pages 325-335 | Published online: 13 May 2019

Abstract

Anhedonia has been implicated as a core symptom of depression and schizophrenia, and studying anhedonia has yielded a wide array of important findings aiding the understanding and identification of psychological disorders. However, anhedonia is a complex and multifaceted construct; indeed, the term anhedonia has been defined in psychological and psychiatric research as many different concepts, a number of which are theoretically and methodologically independent of one another. In this review alone, we discuss research that separates social aspects of anhedonia from the physical contexts of anhedonia, with the former emphasizing interpersonal relationships as important to anhedonic symptoms, and the latter emphasizing biological and brain-related impairment as potential causes of chronic anhedonia states. We highlight research that distinguishes between interest in (wanting) or experience of (liking) potential pleasure as definitions of anhedonia and also disambiguate methodologically and theoretically distinct ways of assessing 1) trait-level dispositional tendencies, 2) state-level cross-sectional assessments, and 3) symptom-based recent changes from baseline, all of which have been used to indicate anhedonia. Lastly, we describe cutting-edge translations of basic anhedonia research into treatment and discuss how different conceptualizations of anhedonia, guided by recent theoretical and methodological advances, have begun to usher in a science of anhedonia that is consistent with increasingly personalized assessment and treatment. We conclude with a note for future research, emphasizing that continued application of theoretically based operationalizations of anhedonia and sound design are paramount to continue the recent progress toward meaningful and specific use of the anhedonia construct in clinical research.

Introduction

Anhedonia is a complex and multifaceted construct. As this review describes, the term anhedonia has been defined in psychological and psychiatric research in many ways, a number of which are theoretically and methodologically independent of one another. This paper reviews research on anhedonia that has attempted to separate its social or physical context,Citation1 with the former emphasizing interpersonal relationships as important to anhedonic symptoms,Citation2 and the latter emphasizing a deficiency in the ability to experience physical pleasures, such as eating or touch.Citation1 In addition, we also review research that considers biological and brain-related impairment as potential causes of chronic anhedonic states.Citation3 Consideration of social factors that lead to the experience of anhedonia, especially when anhedonia is conceptualized as a symptom and not a trait, run somewhat contrary to considerations of anhedonia as the result of biologically based impairment, and each conceptualization has implications for prospective treatment.

We then highlight research that distinguishes between two definitions of anhedonia that are associated with the potential experience of pleasure: anticipatory anhedonia, or the interest in potential pleasure (wanting)Citation4Citation6 and consummatory anhedonia, or the experience of (liking)Citation7,Citation8 potential pleasure. The distinction between anticipatory and consummatory experience of pleasure has some overlap with the aforementioned social/physical distinction, but a relatively independent line of research has implicated deficits in anticipatory pleasure, in particular, as a candidate for intervention.

We will also disambiguate methodologically and theoretically distinct ways of assessing 1) trait-level dispositional tendencies, 2) state-level cross-sectional assessments, and 3) symptom-based recent changes from baseline,Citation2,Citation4,Citation9Citation11 all of which have been used to operationalize anhedonia. As has been previously noted by our research team and others,Citation2,Citation4,Citation9,Citation12Citation14 these distinctions are of crucial importance when studying anhedonia, and we will summarize why each of these three conceptualizations is, for the most part, independent from one another and quite likely to indicate differential considerations in patients or nonclinical populations. A discussion of specific measures that tap into these methodologically and theoretically distinct anhedonias is beyond the scope of this review, and the reader is referred elsewhere.Citation15,Citation16 Instead, we emphasize the distinction and application of these theoretically based operationalizations of anhedonia, as they continue to foster meaningful research in clinical science and treatment.

We will then compare cutting-edge translations of basic anhedonia research into treatment.Citation17Citation20 These treatments focus on increasing and sustaining positive affect, ie, the experience of positively valenced subjective emotional states. Anhedonia is often operationalized as the absence of positive affect, or the loss of interest in or inability to experience positive emotions, so these treatments carry with them the promise of more personalized approaches for the treatment of depression.

We close with a discussion of how different conceptualizations of anhedonia, guided by recent theoretical and methodological advances, have begun to usher in a science of anhedonia that is consistent with increasingly personalized assessment and treatment. We conclude with a note for future research, emphasizing that continued application of sound design and theoretically based operationalizations of anhedonia – as well as considerations of when anhedonia may indicate complex devaluation of reward instead of dysfunctionCitation13 – are paramount to continue the recent progression toward meaningful and specific use of the anhedonia construct in clinical research and treatment.

Anhedonia in biological context

The biological context for anhedonia begins with central functions of reward in brain systems, as affective states may be elicited by rewards when they are anticipated or obtained. The relationship between these systems for reward and pleasure, including those involved in sensory analysis, valuation of reward, and, ultimately, decision-making is often conceptualized as a neural architectural framework.

Biological decision-making processes related to reward and anhedonia involve communication among complex brain structures, and the type of reward, how it is valued, and the choices one has to make in relation to obtaining or experiencing reward make understanding interactions among these systems even more complex. Nonetheless, neurobiological studies have attempted to delineate structures and mechanisms that are associated with reward, the experience of pleasure, and disorders related to prospective dysfunction in these areas.Citation21 Many of these studies emphasize the role of neuroanatomical connections and dopamine (DA) at a neurobiological level.Citation22 Although nondopaminergic mechanisms also play a role in the experience of pleasure, DA has received considerable attention due to its effects on reward anticipation, learning, motivation, and “wanting” of pleasurable stimuli.Citation23 Thus, anhedonia may be considered at both a molecular (eg, deficits in DA production) and at a structural level (eg, deficient processing among brain structures).

Anhedonia at a molecular level

The ventral tegmental area (VTA) is the origin of dopaminergic neurons, which give rise to the nigrostriatal, mesolimbic, and mesocortical pathways.Citation24 Connections within the nigrostriatal pathway are involved in motor control and habit formation, whereas the mesolimbic pathway is implicated in learning, reward motivation, and reinforcement.Citation23 The mesocortical pathway includes projections to cortical regions, such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and is involved in working memory, attention, and executive functioning.Citation23 Overall, core “wanting” reactions appear to be generated in the mesolimbic pathway, given DA’s association with a willingness to work for and obtain reward, and modulate functioning in other brain structures associated with the experience of pleasure and reward.Citation25

In recent decades, much research has promoted the hypothesis that negative symptoms of schizophrenia (eg, avolition and hedonic deficits) are a result of a deficiency in DA in the mesocortical pathway, starting from the VTA in the midbrain and projecting to areas of the prefrontal cortex.Citation26 A similar process has been proposed to occur in major depression, wherein anhedonia is a result of functional deficits associated with dopaminergic transmission, especially in the mesolimbic and mesocortical dopaminergic pathways.Citation26,Citation27

Anhedonia at a structural level

When considering connections among brain structures themselves, multiple cortical areas and sensory systems (eg, the temporal visual cortex and the anterior portion of the insula) determine what stimulus is present. An important area that receives input from these regions is the OFC, although the ACC and amygdala also receive information at this stage.Citation22 In human subjects, the OFC is associated with the experience of pleasure produced via different sensory rewards.Citation22,Citation28 The OFC projects to areas of the temporal lobe, including the amygdala, cingulate cortex, ventral striatum, the ventrome-dial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), as well as the VTA.Citation28,Citation29 These connections ultimately influence decision-making behaviors and response to emotion and novelty.Citation30

The OFC also involves other important functions and is likely the first stage of cortical processing wherein the value of reward is explicitly represented.Citation25 For example, activation in the OFC is related to the subjective pleasantness and satiety of consumed stimuli, such as the flavor of liquids or foods.Citation31 Perhaps more importantly, this area also appears to play a fundamental role in reward-guided decision, with accumulating evidence that neurons in this region carry information related to reward probability, reward magnitude, and risk.Citation32 In sum, evidence from neuroscience literature suggests that neurons in the OFC are fine-tuned to different sensory experiences and are able to represent more abstract dimensions of reward (eg, probability), which in turns leads to the decision to seek reward.Citation21

The amygdala also plays a role in the representation of sensory features, such as taste and odor, of primary rewards.Citation33 Although the function of the amygdala is similar to the OFC in that it serves affective and value-related functions, it is less directly related to experienced pleasure. That is, it allows conditioned associations (ie, links between a certain response and rewarding or punishing stimuli) to influence one’s affective state, as well as decision-making behavior.Citation34 Across human neuroimaging research, converging evidence suggests the ACC also responds in a similar fashion to the OFC, but contains specific projections to cingulate motor areas that are involved in preparation of actions (eg, the mid-cingulate cortex), which further link actions to reward obtainment.Citation23

Together, the OFC, amygdala, and ACC provide the ability to represent the value of primary and sensory rewards that ultimately translate neural reward value representations into behavioral choices.Citation35 With this shift from valuing reward to choice, the vmPFC plays a central role. For example, a corpus of findings indicate that activation in this region is associated with subjective value during decisions when considering immediate and delayed rewards,Citation36Citation38 expected reward value based from one’s own experience,Citation39,Citation40 and a willingness to pay for primary rewards, such as food.Citation41,Citation42 Although the vmPFC plays a formative role in decision-making processes, behavioral output is also guided by information regarding value of outcomes signaled by the OFC.Citation21 Decision-making in relation to reward is thus extraordinarily complex, especially considering that there are other structures involved in the decision-making process (such as the striatum) that are not always associated with rewarding stimuli or affective experiences.Citation25 Due to this complexity, dysfunction at any of these sites or as a result of their dysfunctional interactivity represents potential candidates for associations with self-reported or behavioralCitation43 indicators of anhedonia.

Evaluating anhedonia in a biological context

In sum, when considering anhedonia in a biological context, its phenomenology may be due in part to deficits or dysfunction at the molecular or structural level. At a molecular level, DA appears to play a crucial role in incentive-based salience processing and motivational aspects of pleasure. At a structural level, any impairments in these areas associated with ultimate decision-making may lead to different expressions of anhedonias. Taken together, even just at the biological level, there is need to continue to consider anhedonia as more than merely reduced subjective experience of pleasure.Citation25 The need is even more readily apparent when considering theory and method of examining anhedonia in social context.

Anhedonia in social context

Social anhedonia may be defined as the experience of decreased pleasure or interest in previously enjoyed activities due to social withdrawal or isolation. Anhedonia emerging in social context can sometimes be overshadowed by research in pursuit of biological causal agents that are potential sites for medical interventions. However, when considering differential types of anhedonia that predict different pathologies discriminately, assessment of the social causes of distress are at least as important as assessment of the biological ones.

A quintessential example of the discriminant nature of social anhedonia is seen in the longitudinal investigation of changes in social anhedonia over time by Blanchard et al.Citation44 They found that social anhedonia was a stable individual difference over time in individuals with schizophrenia, whereas those with major depressive disorder (MDD) had increases in social anhedonia only during depressive episodes. Indeed, other research also suggests that waxing and waning of social anhedonia is predictive of symptoms that are more indicative of depression, while more trait-like expression of social anhedonia and physical-related hedonic deficits emerge more robustly in relation to other schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms;Citation4,Citation9,Citation44,Citation45 some findings even suggest that social anhedonia, at least when measured in relation to recent changes, as is done longitudinally,Citation44 is predictive of distress among individuals with severe depressive symptoms. Although the distinction between social and physical anhedonia has some conceptual overlap with the distinction between anticipatory and consummatory anhedonia, recent research suggests that these social aspects may be more likely to instead interact with anticipatory differences,Citation5,Citation9 the latter of which seem to be predictive of both depression and schizophrenia.Citation4Citation6,Citation8

The implications of social anhedonia, and especially changes, in social anhedonia, being potentially able to discriminate between depression and schizophrenia are important. Depression is often conceptualized as a brain disorder: individuals who are depressed are caused to be so because there is an impairment or dysfunction in, among other regions, the pleasure processing centers and functional interactivity of those centers with important regions of the prefrontal cortex. However, the waxing and waning of social anhedonia in depression suggests the potential that the pleasure systems of the brain is intact, and that variables representing changing social context are some of the most important sites for potential intervention. Moreover, recent empirical and theoretical work seems to suggest that depressed individuals, who are by definition more likely to evidence anhedonic symptoms than nondepressed individuals, may have negative associations with positivity that expresses itself as behavioral avoidance of objectively positive things.Citation13 In other words, these individuals not only approach positivity but they also systematically avoid it. Future research examining social anhedonia, changes from baseline, and the relationship that positivity and prospective happiness play in depressed individuals can shine further light on the intriguing possibility that changes in anhedonia may indicate avoiding prospective positivity.

Methodologically and theoretically distinct anhedonias

Anhedonia is usually defined as a lack of and/or a decreased capacity to experience pleasure; thus, it is dependent on the concept of pleasure itself. However, anhedonia is heterogeneous and more complex than an absent vs present dichotomy, likely occurring on a continuum of hedonic tone as perceived intensity of or interest in pleasantness.Citation13,Citation46 Therefore, in operationalizing and measuring anhedonia, it is helpful to delineate under which circumstances it emerges.

For example, consider Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) criteria for schizophrenia, which include negative symptoms of “diminished emotional expression or avolition”.Citation47 Although research historically has focused on delineating different aspects of anhedonia in schizophrenia (see below), the definition of negative symptoms in present nosology risks conflating distinct elements of the experience of pleasure, such as motivation and hedonic impact.Citation25 The same heterogeneity exists in the loss of interest or pleasure criterion for MDD as well: “markedly diminished interest or [emphasis added] pleasure in all, or almost all, activities most of the day, nearly every day (as indicated by either subjective account or observation)”.Citation47

Although anhedonia means “without pleasure” when translated verbatim, viewing anhedonia solely as an inability to feel pleasure ambiguates important temporal or phenomenological aspects, which can differ among individuals.Citation46 Indeed, methodologically and theoretically distinct anhedonias may serve as differential vulnerability factors.

Recent changes in anhedonia vs trait anhedonia

Historically, anhedonia has primarily been assessed as a trait or state construct, with early measures focusing on anhedonia as a focal life-long neurological dysfunction associated with schizophrenia.Citation1 Thus, those with schizophrenia may have never had any interest in or ability to derive pleasure from social interactions. Along these lines, anhedonia is a widely studied negative symptom of schizophrenia.Citation48 More recently, anhedonia has also been conceptualized as a biological vulnerability for the development of schizophrenia spectrum disorders, ie, a premorbid personality trait predisposing patients to development of an overt disorder.Citation26 Anhedonia here is considered an innate trait leading to (or inferring risk for) onset of psychotic symptoms, given that a long-term loss of pleasure can manifest prior to the prototypical symptoms of schizophrenia, such as eccentric behavior and speech.Citation26 Such a trajectory led to the notion that there was an underlying hereditary defect of the central nervous system, which, in tandem with numerous psychosocial stressors or an unfavorable environment, would lead to the development of schizophrenia.Citation26,Citation48

In addition, anhedonia has also been historically considered a diathesis that could eventually develop into unipolar depression, following a significant amount of stress.Citation26 That is, that individuals who were “hypohedonic” were predisposed to develop MDD.Citation26 However, this trait distinction often focuses on a loss of the ability to feel pleasure, not on other aspects of anhedonia, such as loss of interest in engaging in activities that were once pleasurable.Citation23 Given the heterogeneity of depression, these other elements of anhedonia are important, however. For example, one may have clinical depression without anhedonia, and the presence of anhedonia is related to different symptoms and long-term outcomes, such that anhedonic depression is associated with psychomotor retardation, appetite loss, and rumination.Citation48 Thus, the presence of anhedonia may not be a predictor of a depressive disorder per se, rather, a predictor of different constellations of depressive symptoms.

So, trait measures of anhedonia are commonplace when assessing anhedonia in schizophrenia and depression research, as well as other disorders (eg, substance abuse).Citation1,Citation7,Citation48 However, at least at the level of clinical diagnostic assessment, anhedonia is indicated by a decrease in the ability to derive pleasure.Citation46 That is, if one does not enjoy a particular activity and never enjoyed it in the past, he or she is not endorsing anhedonia with regard to a depressive diagnosis. This is an important clinical distinction, as many extant measures – even beyond trait measures – do not examine changes from baseline, but instead evaluate cross-sectional state experiences within a particular time frame.Citation2,Citation49 In fact, without any baseline, it is often difficult to distinguish between state and trait anhedonia. For example, many state anhedonia measures emphasize the ability to experience pleasure within a particular time frame (such as the past 2 weeks). Trait measures, on the other hand, focus on one’s general ability to experience pleasure, without a temporal component. Thus, a global trait inability to experience pleasure may also be reflected with a state anhedonia assessment.Citation50

What differs when assessing recent changes in anhedonia is a comparative evaluation of one’s current interest in or experience of pleasure with one’s previous interest in or experience of pleasure. For example, one may have never enjoyed being with family or friends (trait anhedonia). In addition, one may have not enjoyed being with family or friends within the past few days (state anhedonia). However, assessing whether or not one has enjoyed being with family or friends as much as one used to (a change in the experience of or interest in pleasure) provides valuable information not otherwise captured by trait or state assessments. Indeed, such measures of recent changes have empirically evidenced incremental validity in this manner compared to trait and state measures, especially in relation to depressive symptoms.Citation2

In summary, recent changes in anhedonia uniquely identify clinically significant symptoms with a built-in baseline. This is potentially vital, as having the ability to recognize recent changes in anhedonia, as opposed to measuring a life-long lack of pleasure or interest, may inform more personalized treatments, as well as it may allow researchers and clinicians to better identify recent distress.Citation49

Social anhedonia vs physical anhedonia

Social anhedonia refers to deriving little or no pleasure from interpersonal situations, whereas physical anhedonia refers to deriving little or no pleasure from nonsocial physical sensations, such as smell, touch, or sound.Citation1,Citation48 Examples of physical anhedonia include lack of enjoyment from sexual activity, lack of interest in listening to music, and lack of pleasure from eating palatable foods. Examples of social anhedonia, on the other hand, may include disinterest in making new friends, lack of closeness with friends and family members, and disinterest in having conversations with others. In addition, these two constructs are not entirely separable, as one can certainly engage in pleasurable physical activities in a social setting (such as eating dinner with friends). This notion is supported by research showing strong correlations between measurements of social and physical anhedonia.Citation51,Citation52 Such aspects of anhedonia have been examined extensively in relation to schizophrenia.Citation1 For example, deficits in intimacy and emotional involvement in a variety of social relations have been documented in patients with schizophreniaCitation26 as well as deficits in physical pleasure.Citation1

In relation to MDD, there is evidence that both physical and social anhedonia are episodically affected. For example, depressed patients with anhedonic symptoms are likely to experience both social withdrawal and appetite loss than patients without anhedonic symptoms, although not all anhedonic patients lack pleasure or interest in all domains.Citation48 That is, some patients may experience a generalized loss of pleasure, though some may only have diminished pleasure in a specific domain (eg, social contact), whereas their experience of pleasure in other domains remain unchanged. Further, there are also some temporal differences between these two aspects. Some evidence suggests that physical anhedonia in the context of major depression is more trait-like and associated with symptom severity, whereas social anhedonia may decline over time as a result of symptom remission.Citation48

Anticipatory vs consummatory anhedonia

Anhedonia may also be differentiated into anticipatory and consummatory facets.Citation25,Citation53 Anticipatory anhedonia is a tendency to not look forward to pleasurable events, whereas consummatory anhedonia is a tendency not to derive pleasure from in-the-moment experiences.Citation53 For example, one who evidences anticipatory anhedonia may have difficulty becoming excited about the prospect of attending a social event that includes many friends. Conversely, one who evidences consummatory anhedonia may experience difficulty deriving pleasure from eating one’s favorite meal. Although physical and consummatory anhedonia share some logical overlap, some evidence suggests that increased anticipatory and consummatory anhedonia are both related to increased physical anhedonia.Citation53 However, the relationship between anticipatory anhedonia and physical anhedonia may be explained by the fact that assessment of anticipatory anhedonia also covers physical pleasure in addition to social pleasure.

Thus, while anticipatory and consummatory anhedonia both include aspects of physical anhedonia, the main difference between anticipatory and consummatory anhedonia is temporal, although there are likely bidirectional relationships between the two. For example, most often, one can only want something that has been previously liked or enjoyed, with evidence indicating that the more one looks forward to a reward, the more one enjoys the reward when he or she actually obtains it.Citation48 The distinction between anticipatory and consummatory anhedonia has also played an important role in delineating different neurobiological underpinnings of reward and pleasure, such as the distinction between deficits in motivation and consummation in relation to anhedonia.Citation23,Citation25 At a more basic level, there are “wanting”, “liking”, and “learning” processes associated with pleasure (and lack thereof). Wanting refers to motivation for, or incentive salience of a reward, closely associated with anticipatory pleasure, whereas liking is in line with consummatory pleasure, referring to the actual pleasure or hedonic impact of a reward.Citation25 Learning processes include associations, representations, and predictions about future rewards based on one’s past experience, indicating that these processes include aspects of both anticipatory and consummatory pleasure.Citation25,Citation27 Thus, anhedonia may be a product of deficient processing in either one of these components (see section “Anhedonia in biological context” for more detail).

In sum, anhedonia may likely function as a transdiagnostic construct, depending on how it is operationalized. Whereas anhedonia is often conceptualized in relation to schizophrenia and MDD, aspects of anhedonia are present in other disorders as well.Citation48 However, the phenomenology of anhedonia may differ across disorders. Researchers and clinicians are thus well-served to carefully consider how anhedonia is conceptualized, which keys how and why anhedonia would be associated with different symptoms and disorders.

Cutting-edge translational treatments

Existing depression treatments often place an emphasis on understanding and reducing negative affect; however, individuals experiencing anhedonia may be less responsive to these treatments if they predominantly experience reduced positive affect.Citation54 Indeed, some studies examining treatment outcomes for anhedonia have found low positive affect or sustained symptoms of anhedonia to be a significant predictor of poorer treatment outcomes.Citation2,Citation17 Thus, recent interventions have emerged that target increased positive affect to attempt more personalized interventions for individuals experiencing anhedonia.Citation17Citation20

Positive affect stimulation and sustainment (PASS)

Individuals who are depressed often have difficulty maintaining positive affect, thus PASS was developed to target positive affect functioning (PAF).Citation19 PASS incorporates skills from behavioral activation by focusing on planned events and enhancing the positive emotions associated with them, in line with traditional behavioral treatment. It also incorporates the savoring component of positive psychotherapy with the added focus on extending the effect of positive experiences to past daily activities.Citation19

PASS also incorporates behavioral and cognitive strategies to increase savoring of positive emotions by having individuals recall and write about a recent, vivid, positive event.Citation19 Additionally, individuals associate this event with future positive events to increase anticipatory savoring. Lastly, they are asked to identify their behavioral contributions to this event and make positive attributions about their role in the positive event.

PASS has produced initially heartening results of decreased depression beyond treatment as usual interventions in a developmental trial aimed at increasing PAF.Citation55 However, PASS is conceptualized as a component to add to existing cognitive behavioral treatment, not as a fully formed treatment.

Positive affect treatment (PAT)

PAT was also developed to target the deficits in reward processing that are associated with anhedoniaCitation56 and has the advantage of being conceptualized as a protocol-based and standalone treatment. To improve positive affect, PAT targets three subdomains that are associated with reward processing affect: anticipation, consumption, and learning of reward. Treatment consists of three different modules to address these subdomains over 15 different sessions.

The first module, pleasant events scheduling, targets anticipation and learning of reward by first having individuals schedule activities that are enjoyable to them or were in the past and then focusing on increasing positive reinforcement by associating positive emotions with the planned activity, similar to behavioral activation.Citation56 These two aspects are then combined with consumption of learning by helping individuals savor the positive emotions associated with the activity.Citation56

The second module focuses on three cognitive exercises that can help individuals identify positive stimuli, recognize behavioral contributions to positive outcomes, and imagine future positive events.Citation56 The first exercise allows individuals to repeatedly identify any positive aspects of situations to help train them to orientate their attention toward positive information. The second exercise focuses on attributing positive outcomes to the self and savoring the positive emotions associated with these outcomes. The third exercise aims to build positive mood by having individuals imagine the positive aspects associated with a future event.

The last module places an emphasis on the consumption of reward by having individuals cultivate and savor their positive experiences through audio scripts.Citation56 These sessions include practicing loving-kindness to the self, engaging in daily acts of generosity, wishing happiness on the self and others, and being grateful.

Positive activity intervention (PAI)

Somewhat similar to PAT, Taylor et al detail a ten-session treatment protocol called PAI, another novel treatment approach that aims to train one’s attention to positive events, as well as savor positive experiences that are in turn associated with increased positive emotions and subjective well-being.Citation20 In their initial sessions, patients undergo psychoeducation regarding the nature and utility of positive emotions. They also learn to monitor and document positive events, such as writing about events from the past week for which they are grateful.

As patients come to understand the nature of positive emotions, they turn to increasing the awareness of their personal strengths, identify important personal values, and undergo a variety of activities to foster these positive emotions in themselves and others (eg, by recalling another’s kindness and writing a gratitude letter). Lastly, participants learn to develop a “positive activity plan”, working with a clinician to create a plan that promotes continued commitment to engagement in these positive activities after termination.

Compared to a no intervention control group, Taylor et al found that those who engaged in the PAI protocol evidenced significantly greater increases in positive affect, as well as a significant reduction in negative affect-related outcomes, such as reduced anxiety and depressive symptoms.Citation20 Thus, PAI is a very promising treatment protocol that may be beneficial in reducing anhedonic symptoms.

Augmenting traditional CBT

A rather interesting line of recent research has emphasized additional techniques and strategies to supplement CBT by targeting anhedonia specifically. Traditionally, some researchers argue that CBT emphasizes identifying behaviors and cognitions associated with increases in negative mood, whereas thoughts and behaviors that lead to a decrease in positive mood are neglected.Citation57 Indeed, elevated negative affect is rarely the sole component of depression, and, as this review emphasizes, diminished interest and/or pleasure is an important clinical phenomenon deserving of a greater dialog between the affective and clinical sciences.Citation54

To this end, one can implement an integrationist approach by drawing ideas from other treatments, such as acceptance and commitment therapy or mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. For example, a clinician may seek to validate concerns a depressed or anhedonic patient may have in approaching positive or pleasurable activities. When introducing this agenda, the clinician may acknowledge that feeling happy can be difficult when depressed and that initial efforts to engage in positive activities may not warrant immediate success.Citation57 In addition, depressed patients can be taught to facilitate processing of positive experiences by learning to savor these in-the-moment experiences, similar to the techniques detailed in the modules provided by PAT and PAI.

One recent protocol aimed at treating anhedonia and increasing well-being specifically is augmented depression therapy (ADepT). Drawing from a behavioral activation and an individualized therapy approach, replete with solution-focused and cognitive techniques, ADepT consists of 15 acute treatment sessions with up to 5 booster sessions.Citation18 Primary sessions consist of patients and clients clarifying their values, setting behavioral goals that meet these values, and breaking down these goals into reasonable steps and systematically working toward completing these steps.Citation18 Clinicians work with patients and clients to provide encouragement in carrying out the steps, while building the capability to reach these goals. Preliminary results with a group of 13 depressed patients indicate that the treatment led to significant improvements in levels of well-being and reductions in anhedonic symptoms, as well as overall depressive symptoms.Citation18 Thus, initial findings are promising, and the pilot randomized clinical trial (RCT) for ADepT may provide more detailed information about which strategies and techniques may best augment a brief cognitive-behavioral approach to the treatment of anhedonia.

Ketamine

A novel biologically based treatment that may positively impact severe anhedonia and associated suicidal ideation is intravenous ketamine treatment.Citation58Citation60 Ketamine is an antagonist of N-methyl-d-aspartate that prospectively acts on glutamatergic receptors and enhances release of neurotransmitters related to mood regulations.Citation60 A recent comprehensive meta-analysis found a large and significant effect of ketamine decreasing suicidal ideation, though the overall evidence was deemed quite low because of the relatively few number of existing RCTs.Citation60 In contrast to other antidepressants that may have lag times of weeks in improving depressive symptoms, ketamine has been shown to improve depressive symptoms, including anhedonia and suicidal ideation, within hours of administration.Citation61 Moreover, reported improved mood was sustained for a week after a single dose of ketamine.Citation61

So, there is only budding evidence of ketamine’s overall efficacy. However, a recent longitudinal study that found a relationship between ketamine treatment and reduced suicidal ideation also found that shifts in anhedonia may be a mechanism through which ketamine causes reduction in suicidal ideation,Citation58 even when considered independently of other depressive symptoms. Thus, further research examining ketamine as a biological treatment of anhedonia is warranted.

Amygdala neurofeedback and depressive symptoms

Expanding upon potential biological treatments afforded by ketamine, one recent RCT evaluated the effects of real-time functional MRI neurofeedback (rtfMRI-nf) from the amygdala in depressed patients.Citation63 By utilizing an rtfMRI-nf approach, patients receive information regarding their blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal in real-time and learn to self-modulate this signal. As mentioned previously, the amygdala plays an important role in relation to depressive symptoms, with the logic behind rtfMRI-nf being that enhancement of processing of positive stimuli in the amygdala may provide treatment gains not otherwise seen in traditional therapy.

To this end, Young et al compared participants randomly assigned to receive rtfMRI-nf from either 1) the left amygdala or 2) the intraparietal sulcus, a region not readily activated in emotion regulation.Citation63 Participants in both conditions were instructed to retrieve positive emotions while monitoring their own blood flow to the associated regions. Results suggest that training to enhance the amygdala’s hemodynamic response to positive memory recall was associated with a significant reduction in depressive symptoms in participants presenting with clinically significant symptoms. Although this approach has produced initial efficacious results, it may also prove beneficial to patients presenting with anhedonic symptoms, as general autobiographical recall is an important clinical correlate across depression and schizophrenia.Citation64,Citation65

Evaluating novel anhedonia treatments

Overall, these treatments hold the promise of targeting reward processing deficits associated with anhedonia in a more precise manner than traditional depression treatments. The interventions discussed above aim to target positive affect and increasing positive experiences through sustainment and reward anticipation, unlike traditional depression treatments, including cognitive-behavioral therapy, which focuses mainly on targeting negative affect and reducing negative experiences. Although these traditional treatments have been shown to improve positive affect, even first-line treatments often fail to sustain these gains due to multiple obstacles to sustained symptom remission;Citation66Citation68 thus, these treatments focusing on sustaining positive affect through savoring and attribution, and biologically based treatments have promise. Moreover, they have produced initially heartening results beyond treatment as usual interventions.Citation19,Citation58

However, there are two important considerations that will need to be addressed by future research on these novel treatments. The first is empirical and will be answered by more well-controlled studies that can raise the evidence base.Citation60 Additionally, future studies should further examine both these novel interventions and traditional depression treatments in order to investigate the effectiveness of the different treatments in reducing negative affect and increasing positive affect. The second is theoretical and will be answered by careful contemplation of why some anhedonic individuals not only approach positive events but also actively avoid them.

Moving forward with anhedonia research: the challenge of reward devaluation theory

So far, a corpus of research outlines different elements of anhedonia (or entirely unique conceptualizations of anhedonia) that are core predictors of differential psychopathological states. Biopsychosocial context informs these assessments and has resulted in novel treatments, which suggest hope for a way forward for precision medicine approaches to anhedonia interventions.

However, extant interventions privilege increased and expanded experience of positive affect with the assumption that positivity is merely being undervalued (or processed dysfunctionally) by anhedonic individuals. These conceptual and treatment-based models do not address whether positive affect is being devalued because of its positive content due to fear of negative results, however.

In other words, a crucial consideration moving forward is how to cross the intersection of social and biological contexts when considering reward devaluation. Potentially all biological findings demonstrating deficits in reward learning and positive affect are limited by the possibility that some individuals with anhedonia may have a complex relationship with positivity that is only cursorily explained via brain dysfunction and impairment. This is because some individuals may exhibit attentional biases away from positive information due to a previous association of positivity with negative outcomes.Citation13 Although prospective positivity may have once been viewed as rewarding, it is likely that it ended up being harmful and thus resulted in disappointment. Repeated disappointment in response to positive information may have thus created lasting, learned associations of positivity with negative outcomes; positive information is then viewed as a threat and can become more dangerous than neutral or even negative information.Citation16

The learned association that positivity has come to represent potential threat results in individuals devaluing positivity and avoiding what was once viewed as reward.Citation13 Reward devaluation theory thus posits that individuals who devalue reward do not simply lack the ability to find positive information rewarding, but they instead actively avoid this information by diverting their attention away from positivity when exposed to it.

Extensive meta-analytic evidence supporting this theory suggests that depressed individuals exhibit such avoidance to positivity,Citation13 consistent with other evidence of depressed individuals’ tendency to initially approach positive information but quickly avoid it due to their association of positivity with threat.Citation2,Citation13,Citation69 Importantly, both depressed individuals and other clinical groups exhibit vigilance toward threat, but avoidance of positivity is unique to individuals with primary symptoms of depression.

Recent changes in anhedonia that include loss of interest or pleasure may be specific to individuals avoiding positivity.Citation4,Citation12,Citation13,Citation70 Thus, avoidance of reward is a crucial candidate variable for future research that will unpack how and why individuals develop and maintain anhedonic symptoms.

Conclusion

Anhedonia can be conceptualized and measured in a number of unique ways. This review has included considerations of social and physical context; of interest in (wanting) or experience of (liking) potential pleasure; and of the comparison of trait-level dispositional tendencies, state-level cross-sectional assessments, and symptom-based recent changes from baseline. We have also described translations of basic anhedonia research into treatment that hold future promise, including psychosocial (eg, PAT and PAI) and biological (eg, ketamine) treatments. We lastly offered a challenge to the field to consider how reward devaluation theory might expand upon current conceptualizations of anhedonia to include motivated avoidance of positivity as a potential target for research hypotheses and personalized treatment.

Anhedonia, as we have seen, is a complex and multifaceted construct. Future work should emphasize continued application of theoretically based operationalizations of anhedonia to continue the recent progress toward meaningful and specific use of the anhedonia construct in theory, clinical research, and treatment.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • ChapmanLJChapmanJPRaulinMLScales for physical and social anhedoniaJ Abnorm Psychol1976854374382956504
  • WinerESVeilleuxJCGingerEJDevelopment and validation of the specific loss of interest and pleasure scale (SLIPS)J Affect Disord201415215419320125618002
  • TreadwayMTZaldDHReconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscienceNeurosci Biobehav Rev201135353755520603146
  • JordanDGWinerESSalemTKilgoreJLongitudinal evaluation of anhedonia as a mediator of fear of positive evaluation and other depressive symptomsCogn Emot20183271437144728278732
  • GeaneyJTTreadwayMTSmillieLDTrait anticipatory pleasure predicts effort expenditure for rewardPloS One2015106e013135726115223
  • SherdellLWaughCEGotlibIHAnticipatory pleasure predicts motivation for reward in major depressionJ Abnorm Psychol20121211516021842963
  • GardDEGardMGKringAMJohnOPAnticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: a scale development studyJ Res Pers200640610861102
  • GardDEKringAMGardMGHoranWPGreenMFAnhedonia in schizophrenia: distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasureSchizophr Res2007931–325326017490858
  • WinerESBryantJBartoszekGRojasENadorffMRKilgoreJMapping the relationship between anxiety, anhedonia, and depressionJ Affect Disord201722128929628668590
  • ThomsenKRMeasuring anhedonia: impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about rewardFront Psychol20156Suppl 1140926441781
  • LoasGAnhedonia and risk of suicide: an overview Anhedonia: A Comprehensive Handbook Volume IINew York, NYSpringer2014247253
  • SalemTWinerESNadorffMRCombined behavioural markers of cognitive biases are associated with anhedoniaCogn Emot201832242243028359184
  • WinerESSalemTReward devaluation: dot-probe meta-analytic evidence of avoidance of positive information in depressed personsPsychol Bull20161421187826619211
  • ZielinskiMJVeilleuxJCWinerESNadorffMRA short-term longitudinal examination of the relations between depression, anhedonia, and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors in adults with a history of self-injuryCompr Psychiatry20177318719528040576
  • HoranWPKringAMBlanchardJJAnhedonia in schizophrenia: a review of assessment strategiesSchizophr Bull200632225927316221997
  • GsDDichterGSAnhedonia in unipolar major depressive disorder: a reviewOpen Psychiatr J2010419
  • CraskeMGMeuretAERitzTTreanorMDourHJTreatment for anhedonia: a neuroscience driven approachDepress Anxiety2016331092793827699943
  • DunnBDWidnallEReedNEvaluating augmented depression therapy (ADEPT): study protocol for a pilot randomised controlled trialPilot and Feasibility Studies2019
  • McMakinDLSiegleGJShirkSRPositive affect stimulation and sustainment (PASS) module for depressed mood: a preliminary investigation of treatment-related effectsCogn Ther Res2011353217226
  • TaylorCTLyubomirskySSteinMBUpregulating the positive affect system in anxiety and depression: outcomes of a positive activity interventionDepress Anxiety201734326728028060463
  • BlumKOscar-BermanMGardnerELSimpaticoTBravermanERGoldMSNeurogenetics and neurobiology of dopamine in anhedoniaRitsnerMSAnhedonia: A Comprehensive Handbook. Vol. INew York, NYSpringer2014179208
  • RollsETGrabenhorstFThe orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-makingProg Neurobiol200886321624418824074
  • TreadwayMTZaldDHReconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscienceNeurosci Biobehav Rev201135353755520603146
  • BjörklundADunnettSBDopamine neuron systems in the brain: an updateTrends Neurosci200730519420217408759
  • ThomsenKRWhybrowPCKringelbachMLReconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brainFront Behav Neurosci2015912325653603
  • PelizzaLPupoSFerrariAAnhedonia in schizophrenia and major depression: state or trait? Review of the literatureJ Psychopathol201218145155
  • TreadwayMTZaldDHTranslational models of dopaminergic mechanisms for motivational deficits in anhedonic patientsRitsnerMSAnhedonia: A Comprehensive Handbook vol INew York, NYSpringer2014107117
  • RollsETThe functions of the orbitofrontal cortexNeurocase199954301312
  • ZaldDHAndreottiCNeuropsychological assessment of the orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortexNeuropsychologia201048123377339120728457
  • RollsETThe orbitofrontal cortex and emotion in health and disease, including depressionNeuropsychologia Epub2017924
  • GrabenhorstFRollsETParrisBAD’SouzaAAHow the brain represents the reward value of fat in the mouthCereb Cortex20102051082109119684248
  • O’NeillMSchultzWCoding of reward risk by orbitofrontal neurons is mostly distinct from coding of reward valueNeuron201068478980021092866
  • McGaughJLThe amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiencesAnnu Rev Neurosci200427112815217324
  • LeDouxJEEmotion: clues from the brainAnnu Rev Psychol19954612092357872730
  • GrabenhorstFRollsETValue, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortexTrends Cogn Sci2011152566721216655
  • BernsGSMcClureSMPagnoniGMontaguePRPredictability modulates human brain response to rewardJ Neurosci20012182793279811306631
  • McClureSMLaibsonDILoewensteinGCohenJDSeparate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewardsScience2004306569550350715486304
  • KableJWGlimcherPWThe neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choiceNat Neurosci200710121625163317982449
  • BehrensTEJHuntLTWoolrichMWRushworthMFSAssociative learning of social valueNature2008456721924524919005555
  • BoormanEDBehrensTEJWoolrichMWRushworthMFHow green is the grass on the other side? Frontopolar cortex and the evidence in favor of alternative courses of actionNeuron200962573374319524531
  • ChibVSRangelAShimojoSO’DohertyJPEvidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortexJ Neurosci20092939123151232019793990
  • PlassmannHO’DohertyJRangelAOrbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactionsJ Neurosci200727379984998817855612
  • TreadwayMTBuckholtzJWSchwartzmanANLambertWEZaldDHWorth the “EEfRT”? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedoniaPLoS One200948e659819672310
  • BlanchardJLHoranWPBrownSADiagnostic differences in social anhedonia: a longitudinal study of schizophrenia and major depressive disorderJ Abnorm Psychol2001110336337111502079
  • JoinerTEBrownJSMetalskyGIA test of the tripartite model’s prediction of anhedonia’s specificity to depression: patients with major depression versus patients with schizophreniaPsychiatry Res2003119324325012914895
  • HoNSommersMAnhedonia: a concept analysisArch Psychiatr Nurs201327312112923706888
  • American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders5th edArlington, VAAmerican Psychiatric Association2013
  • ShankmanSAKatzACDeLizzaAASarapasCGorkaSMCampbellMLThe different facets of anhedonia and their associations with different PsychopathologiesRitsnerMSAnhedonia: A Comprehensive Handbook Vol INew York, NYSpringer2014322
  • WinerESNadorffMREllisTEAllenJGHerreraSSalemTAnhedonia predicts suicidal ideation in a large psychiatric inpatient samplePsychiatry Res20142181–212412824774075
  • LoasGMonestesJLIngelaereANoisetteCHerbenerESStability and relationships between trait or state anhedonia and schizophrenic symptoms in schizophrenia: a 13-year follow-up studyPsychiatry Res20091662–313214019272653
  • OlinoTMHortonLEVersellaMVA comparison of psychometric and convergent validity for social anhedonia and social closenessPsychol Assess201628111465147426882080
  • BlanchardJJMueserKTBellackASAnhedonia, positive and negative affect, and social functioning in schizophreniaSchizophr Bull19982434134249718633
  • GardDEGardMGKringAMJohnOPAnticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: a scale development studyJ Res Pers200640610861102
  • DunnBDHelping depressed clients reconnect to positive emotion experience: current insights and future directionsClin Psychol Psychother201219432634022674611
  • McMakinDLSiegleGJShirkSRPositive affect stimulation and sustainment (PASS) module for depressed mood: a preliminary investigation of treatment-related effectsCognit Ther Res2011353217226
  • CraskeMGMeuretAERitzTTreanorMDourHJTreatment for anhedonia: a neuroscience driven approachDepress Anxiety2016331092793827699943
  • DunnBDAugmenting cognitive behavioral therapy to build positive mood in depressionGruberJThe Oxford Handbook of Positive Emotion and PsychopathologyNew York, NYOxford University PressIn press
  • BallardEDWillsKLallyNAnhedonia as a clinical correlate of suicidal thoughts in clinical ketamine trialsJ Affect Disord201721819520028477497
  • BallardEDIonescuDFVande VoortJLImprovement in suicidal ideation after ketamine infusion: relationship to reductions in depression and anxietyJ Psychiatr Res20145816116625169854
  • BartoliFRiboldiICrocamoCDi BritaCClericiMCarràGKetamine as a rapid-acting agent for suicidal ideation: a meta-analysisNeurosci Biobehav Rev20177723223628342764
  • ZarateCASinghJBCarlsonPJA randomized trial of an N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist in treatment-resistant major depressionArch Gen Psychiatry200663885686416894061
  • MarcusSMYoungEAKerberKBGender differences in depression: findings from the STAR*D studyJ Affect Disord2005872–314115015982748
  • YoungKDSiegleGJZotevVRandomized clinical trial of real-time fMRI amygdala neurofeedback for major depressive disorder: effects on symptoms and autobiographical memory recallAm J Psychiatry2017174874875528407727
  • KingMJMacDougallAGFerrisSMLevineBMacQueenGMMcKinnonMCA review of factors that moderate autobiographical memory performance in patients with major depressive disorderJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol201032101122114420544462
  • McLeodHJWoodNBrewinCRAutobiographical memory deficits in schizophreniaCogn Emot2006203–453654726529221
  • DimidjianSBarreraMJrMuñozRFMuñozRFLewinsohnPMThe origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depressionAnnu Rev Clin Psychol20117113821275642
  • DimidjianSHollonSDDobsonKSRandomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with major depressionJ Consult Clin Psychol200674465867016881773
  • ScottJTeasdaleJDPaykelESEffects of cognitive therapy on psychological symptoms and social functioning in residual depressionBr J Psychiatry2000177544044611059998
  • FrewenPDozoisDJoanisseMNeufeldRSelective attention to threat versus reward: meta-analysis and neural-network modeling of the dot-probe taskClin Psychol Rev200828230733717618023
  • BryantJWinerESSalemTNadorffMRStruggling toward reward: recent experience of anhedonia interacts with motivation to predict reward pursuit in the face of a stressful manipulationPloS One2017123e017343928273126