114
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Among substance-abusing traffic offenders, poor sleep and poor general health predict lower driving skills but not slower reaction times

, , , , , & show all
Pages 557-566 | Published online: 09 Nov 2018

Abstract

Background

Traffic accidents are a significant health issue in Iran. Explanations for such accidents have included single consideration of the role of poor sleep and negative psychological trait and state variables. In this study, we examined whether and to what extent sleep, general health, and aggression can concomitantly predict driving behavior.

Methods

A total of 360 male traffic offenders (driving under substance use; mean age: 31 years) participated in this study. They completed the questionnaires covering sociodemographic, sleep-related, and behavior-related variables. In addition, their visual and acoustic reaction times were objectively tested.

Results

Poor sleep, poor general health, and higher aggression scores were associated with self-rated poor driving behavior. Poor sleep was directly associated with poor driving behavior and indirectly via poor general health and aggression. In contrast, visual and acoustic reaction times were unrelated to sleep, general health, aggression, or self-rated driving behavior.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study in Iran to assess concomitantly poor sleep, poor general health, and higher aggression scores as independent predictors of poor driving behavior among a larger sample of substance-abusing traffic offenders. Furthermore, visual and acoustic reaction times were unrelated to sleep, general health, aggression, and driving behavior. Finally, importantly, poor sleep predicted both directly and indirectly poor driving behavior.

Introduction

In Iran, cars and motorcycles are the most common means of transport, and not surprisingly, compared with Western countries (North America and Europe), the traffic-related prevalence of mortality is high.Citation1Citation3 Despite a decreasing trend in traffic-related deaths,Citation4 traffic accidents remain the second largest cause of mortality in Iran.Citation5,Citation6 Although the World Health Organization reported 24.1 traffic deaths per 100,000 people every year in Iran, the prevalence rate is substantially lower in, for example, Switzerland (3.4), Germany (4.3), and USA (11.6). Furthermore, traffic accidents are the main cause of injuries requiring surgical intervention in Iran.Citation7Citation9

There is a broad consensus that up to 95% of traffic accidents are not due to the technical malfunctions but due to the poor driving behavior. The three possible factors in poor driving behavior are as follows: 1) trait variables, 2) state variables, and 3) sleep.

As regards trait variables, a line of research has examined associations between particular personality traits and poor driving behavior.Citation10Citation14 Lucida et al and Mallia et alCitation10,Citation11 identified excitement-seeking as a risk factor for poor driving, whereas Pourabdian and AzmoonCitation14 focused on trait anxiety as a possible risk factor for poor driving. In contrast, Dahlen et alCitation13 identified narcissism as a risk factor for poor driving, whereas Beanland et alCitation12 considered the impact of antagonism, negative affectivity, and disinhibition. These approaches thus point to a particular constellation of personality traits as a source of poor driving behavior.

In addition, there is evidence that current psychological state and traits can have an impact on driving behavior. For example, Hilton et alCitation15 showed that current self-reported symptoms of depression were associated with increased road accidents among professional heavy goods vehicle drivers. Importantly, these authors claimed that lack of concentration and poor sleep, rather than symptoms of depression as such, might have negative effects on driving behavior. Also, Scott-Parker et alCitation16 identified symptoms of depression as predictors of risky behavior among novice drivers, while in a further analyses, the same authors concluded that symptoms of anxiety rather than depression predicted risky driving among female but not male novice drivers.

Aggression merits particular attention, given that aggressive behavior might be regarded both as a trait and as a state variable, and also that aggressive driving is acknowledged as a contributor to motor vehicle accidents. Not surprising, the Manchester Driving Behavior Questionnaire (MDBQ)Citation17 explicitly includes aggressive reactions (ie, aggressive violations and ordinary violations) as factors adversely affecting driving. In this regard, Beanland et alCitation12 reported an overlap between state aggression behavior and the personality traits of antagonism, negative affectivity, and disinhibition. Likewise, in a survey carried out in the UK and the Irish Republic, Stephens and SullmanCitation18 identified trait aggression as a predictor of crash-related behaviors. Specifically, aggressive forms of expression were higher for drivers who reported initiating road rage incidents, and the total scores for aggressive expression were also higher for drivers who reported recent crash-related events, such as loss of concentration, losing control of their vehicle, moving violations, near-misses, and major crashes.Citation18

Next, as regards mental health status, Possis et alCitation19 showed that, among veterans, risky driving behavior and poor mental health status were associated with poor driving behavior. Furthermore, in previous studies,Citation2,Citation3 we have showed that, among both traffic offenders and non-offenders, poor mental health status was associated with both poor driving behavior and aggression, while aggression was also associated with poor driving behavior. However, when both poor health status and aggression were entered in a regression equation, only poor health status emerged as a predictor of poor driving behavior.

As regards sleep, there is an extensive literature showing that poor sleep is associated with various negative cognitive,Citation20Citation23 emotional,Citation23Citation25 social,Citation26 and behavioral characteristics.Citation27,Citation28 Poor sleep is causally related to work place problems and work place absenteeism.Citation29,Citation30 In this context, not surprisingly, poor sleep is also associated with increased risks of traffic issues.Citation31Citation37 Thus, it is estimated that 10%–20% of all traffic accidents are causally related to poor sleep.Citation38 For example, Hilton et alCitation15 identified, in addition to symptoms of depression, poor sleep as a risk factor for lack of concentration, which in turn impacted negatively on driving behavior. Likewise, Garbarino et alCitation39 showed among 949 truck drivers that up to 28% suffered from insomnia; most dramatically, insomniacs had a markedly higher likelihood of reporting both accidents and near-miss accidents. Recently, Bioulac et alCitation40 confirmed the association between poor sleep and poor driving behavior in their systematic review and meta-analysis. Thomann et alCitation41 identified an explanation for this association in the impaired scores on psychomotor vigilance of people with excessive daytime sleepiness (due to poor night sleep) when compared with healthy controls. However, in their recent review of the associations between poor sleep and sleepiness on the one hand and objective reaction times on driving simulators on the other hand, Schreier et alCitation42 found that the associations were trivial and that it was not sleepiness as such, but rather the self-perception of sleepiness that more reliably explained the association between poor sleep and poor driving behavior. Thus, Hallvig et alCitation43 showed that performance on a driving simulator and real driving were only weakly related and that this association was further weakened after taking subjective sleepiness into account. Finally, based on a systematic review and meta-analysis, Tabrizi et alCitation44 showed that up to 50% of all drivers in Iran reported sleep complaints.

To summarize, trait variables, state variables, and poor sleep need to be considered in explanations of poor driving behavior. In addition, we believe that investigating such associations is particularly important in Iran, given the evidence that fatal traffic accidents are from 5.5 to 7 times higher than those in countries such as Switzerland or Germany. Next, the relationship between performance on driving simulators and sleep is not straightforward, and the role of trait and state variables in this relationship has yet to be considered. Accordingly, this study aimed at shedding light on the association between subjective sleep patterns, driving behavior (both subjective and objective),Citation1 and trait and state variables in a large sample of Iranian traffic offenders. In this context, we put particular emphasis on the direct and indirect associations between subjective sleep and driving behavior (subjective and objective) via their association with general health status; in our previous studies,Citation2,Citation3 it was proved that poor mental health status was related to poor driving behavior.

The following five hypotheses were formulated. First, we expected poor sleep, poor driving behavior, poor general health, and high aggression scores to be associated.Citation2,Citation3,Citation12,Citation15,Citation18,Citation44 Second, we anticipated that, compared with good sleepers, poor sleepers would report more impaired subjective driving behavior and have lower general health, higher aggression scores, and slower reaction times.Citation31Citation37 Third, we predicted that poor driving behavior would be independently predicted by poor sleep, poor general health, and higher aggression scores.Citation2,Citation3,Citation12,Citation15,Citation18 Fourth, we expected that poor sleep would predict poor driving behavior indirectly as well as directly. Finally, based on the studies by Schreier et alCitation42 and Hallvig et al,Citation43 we assumed that reaction time on a driving simulator as a proxy for objective driving behavior would not be related to subjective driving behavior or to psychological functioning (general health, subjective sleep, and aggression).

Methods

Procedures

Substance-abusing male traffic offenders undergoing a thorough driving-related assessment took part in the present cross-sectional study. Although the assessment was mandatory for all traffic offenders as a consequence of violating traffic rules under the influence of psychotropic substances, taking part in this study and providing data for scientific ends were not mandatory. Accordingly, all eligible participants were informed about the aim of this study and the anonymous data handling. Thereafter, they signed a written informed consent. Participants came once to our research center, and all assessments took place in the late morning during 11 am and 2 pm. The first part of the assessment lasted for about 20 minutes, which includes the following: study information, signing the written informed consent, and completing the self-rating questionnaires. The second part lasted for about 15 minutes, which includes the following: measuring auditory and visual reaction time (see details below). Collectively, the entire assessment including welcome and discharge lasted for about 45 minutes. Data collection took place between June 2016 and April 2017. The review board of the Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences (KUMS; Kermanshah, Iran) approved the study, which was performed in accordance with the rules laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Sample

A total of 485 substance-abusing male traffic offenders, whose driving license had been withdrawn, were approached; of whom, 360 (74.23%; mean age: 30.97 years; SD=9.51) agreed to participate in this study. After a brief somatic and psychiatric interview, and based on medical records and records from the penalty register of the traffic authorities, psychiatrists or clinical psychologists identified substance abuse as the main mental illness. Eighty-five percent of the participants were opium/opioid users, 13% were methamphetamine users, and 3% were alcohol and cannabis users. Seventy-three percent of the participants had car crashes and 27% had accidents with motor cycles. Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) age 18 years or older; 2) male; 3) written informed consent; 4) willing and able to complete self-rating questionnaires; and 5) willing and able to undergo reaction time testing. Exclusion criteria were as follows: 1) current state of suicidality and 2) current state of psychosis.

Tools

Sociodemographic and driver-related information

This questionnaire asked for gender, age (years), highest educational level (primary school, high school, diploma, and university degree), civil status (single, married, and divorced/widowed), and years of driving experience.

Driving behavior

Participants completed the MDBQ.Citation17,Citation45,Citation46 The MDBQ is a self-rating questionnaire consisting of 50 items measuring aberrant driving behaviors and focuses on aggressive violations (eg, “Sound your horn to indicate your annoyance to another road user”; “Become angered by another driver and give chase with the intention of giving him/her a piece of your mind”), ordinary violations (eg, “Disregard the speed limit on a residential road/motor way”; “Overtake a slow driver on the inside”; and “I drive so close to the car in front that it would be difficult to stop in an emergency”), errors (eg, “Underestimate the speed of an oncoming vehicle when overtaking”; “Fail to check your rear-view mirror before pulling out, changing lanes, etc.”), and lapses (eg, “Attempt to drive away from the traffic lights in third gear”; “Forget where you left your car in a car park”; and “Intending to drive to destination A, you ‘wake up’ to find yourself on the road to destination B”). Responses are given on six-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (= never) to 5 (= nearly all the time), with higher mean scores reflecting more numerous violations, errors, and lapses (Cronbach’s alpha =0.89).

General health

Participants completed the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ;Citation47 Farsi versionCitation48). The GHQ is a self-rating questionnaire to identify psychological distress. It consists of 28 items and assesses anxiety and insomnia, depression, social dysfunction, and somatic health. Answers are given on four-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (= not at all) to 3 (= more than usual), with higher total scores reflecting more severe health issues (Cronbach’s alpha =0.91).

Aggression

Participants completed the Aggression Questionnaire (AGQ;Citation49 Farsi versionCitation2,Citation3). The AGQ is a self-rating questionnaire; it consists of 29 items and covers physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, and hostility. Answers are given on seven-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (= extremely uncharacteristic of me) to 7 (= extremely characteristic of me), with higher scores reflecting a greater tendency toward aggressive behavior (Cronbach’s alpha =0.90).

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)

Subjective sleep: PSQI

The PSQI was designed by Buysse et alCitation50 to measure sleep quality and to help diagnose people with good or poor sleep. The PSQI is a self-report scale that takes 5 minutes to complete; it consists of 19 items and contains seven subscales (subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep disturbance, sleeping medication, and daytime dysfunction), each weighted equally on a scale from 0 to 3, with higher scores indicating poorer sleep quality. The seven components are then summed to obtain an overall PSQI score, ranging from 0 (good sleep quality) to 21 (poor sleep quality). Total scores of ≥5 reflect poor sleep, associated with considerable sleep complaints. The psychometric properties of the Farsi/Persian version were tested by Farrahi et al,Citation51 with a sample of adult psychiatric patients and healthy controls.

Measuring auditory and visual reaction time

To measure the reaction time to auditory, visual and combined auditory–visual stimuli, participants underwent testing on a driving simulator. The apparatus mimicked the equipment of the driver’s position in a car, consisting of a seat, wheel, three pedals (brake, accelerator, and clutch), handbrake, gear shift, and direction indicator control. In addition, a screen and a loudspeaker were positioned in front of the participant. The tasks were as follows: on the screen, a virtual roadway was presented, suggesting continuous driving. At random, visual or acoustic stimuli were presented, and at these points, participants had to use the brakes. The outcome variable was the time lapse between the stimulus and the behavioral reaction (brakes). Two sets of 30 random stimuli were presented, reaction time was automatically recorded, and the average reaction time in milliseconds was calculated. The entire test lasted for about 15 minutes.

Statistical analysis

Preliminary calculations: first, we calculated whether educational level, civil status, age, or driving experience systematically changed the pattern of results. It turned out that neither civil status nor educational level had any effect. However, lower age was associated with higher aggression scores, while longer driving experience was associated with better driving behavior.

Main calculations: first, correlations were computed between sleep patterns (PSQI: subjective sleep quality, sleep duration, sleep latency, sleep efficacy, sleep disturbances, sleep medication, daytime dysfunction; overall score), driving behavior (MDBQ: overall score), general health (GHQ:2 overall score), aggression (overall score), and reaction times (auditory, visual, and visual–auditory stimuli). Second, t-tests for unrelated samples were carried out with the PSQI cutoff score of less than five points (no sleep disturbances) or five and higher points (sleep disturbances) as independent variables and driving behavior, general health, aggression, and reaction time as dependent variables. Third, a multiple regression analysis (stepwise; backward) was performed with the MDBQ total score as a dependent variable, and subjective sleep, general health, and aggression as predictors. Fourth, a path analysis was executed, again with the MDBQ total score as a dependent variable; we tested the direct effects of sleep on driving behavior and indirect effects via general health. The nominal level of statistical significance was set at alpha <0.05. Statistical computations were performed with SPSS® 25.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) for Apple® Mac®.

Results

Sociodemographic and driving-related information

reports the descriptive sociodemographic and driving-related information.

Table 1 Descriptive statistical overview of sociodemographic information (N=360)

Associations between sleep patterns, driving behavior, general health, and aggression

reports the correlations between sleep patterns, general health, aggression, and driving behavior. Sleep, driving behavior, general health, and aggression scores were significantly associated, while this was not the case for reaction times.

Table 2 Overview of correlation coefficients (Pearson’s correlation coefficients) between dimensions of sleep, driving behavior, aggression, general health, and objective reaction time

More specifically, all dimensions of negative PSQI scores (total score, sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficacy, sleep disturbances, sleep medication, and daytime functioning) were associated with poorer driving behavior (MDBQ), higher aggression, and poorer general health ().

Table 3 Overview of correlation coefficients (Pearson’s correlation coefficients) between dimensions of sleep (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index) and driving behavior, aggression, general health, and objective reaction time

Driving behavior, general health, and aggression between participants with and without sleep disturbances

reports the descriptive and inferential statistical indices comparing participants with (PSQI scores ≥5 points) and without sleep disturbances (PSQI scores <5 points). Compared to those without sleep complaints (n=21; 5.8%), participants with sleep complaints (n=339; 94.2%) reported poorer driving behavior, poorer general health, and higher aggression scores. No differences were found for reaction times.

Table 4 Descriptive and inferential statistical indices of sleep, driving behavior, aggression, and general health between poor and good sleepers

Predicting driving behavior from sleep, general health, and aggression

A multiple regression analysis (stepwise backward; ) was performed. Driving behavior was the dependent variable; sleep, general health, and aggression were predictors. Poor sleep and poor general health predicted poor driving behavior, while aggression did not reach statistical significance; aggression was therefore not entered in the equation.

Table 5 Multiple linear regression model to describe the influence of sleep quality, general health, and aggression on driving behavior

Direct and indirect effects of sleep on driving behavior, with general health as a mediating variable

As shown in , sleep had both direct effects on driving behavior and indirect effects via general health. The equation is as follows: r=0.483 (r sleep and driving) = b=0.367 (sleep on driving) +0.485 (r sleep and general health) × b=0.23 (general health on driving).

Figure 1 Direct and indirect effects of sleep quality on driving behavior.

Figure 1 Direct and indirect effects of sleep quality on driving behavior.

Post-hoc analyses

Neither the type of substance use (opium/opioids, methamphetamine, cannabis) nor the type of accident (car or motor cycle) was systematically related to psychological functioning, sleep, and reaction time (all Fs<1.0, ps>0.43).

Discussion

The key findings of this study were that among a sample of male traffic offenders guilty of substance abuse, poor sleep was both directly and indirectly associated with poor subjective driving behavior, but not with objective reaction time. Poor sleepers also recorded higher scores for state variables such as depression, anxiety, and aggression. The pattern of results adds to the current literature in showing that poor sleep was both directly and indirectly associated with poor driving behavior. However, reaction times on a driving simulator were not associated with subjective sleep, psychological state, or subjective driving behavior.

Five hypotheses were formulated, and each of them is considered now in turn.

Our first hypothesis was that poor sleep, poor driving behavior, poor general health, and higher aggression scores would be associated, and this was confirmed. The pattern of results is therefore consistent with previous findings,Citation2,Citation3,Citation12,Citation15,Citation18,Citation44 although we believe that these results add to previous findings in that the associations were found among young Iranian males guilty of traffic offences related to substance abuse. A further novelty of this study is that the triad of trait (aggression), state (depression and anxiety), and sleep were found to contribute to the explanation of poor subjective driving behavior (). Moreover, more negative scores on all dimensions of the PSQI (overall score, sleep duration, sleep quality, sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, sleep medication, and daytime functioning) were associated with poorer driving (MDBQ), higher aggression scores, and poorer general health (). These patterns of results suggest that it is not a single facet of sleep such as duration, efficacy, or sleep onset latency, but sleep as an overarching phenomenon that is related to poor driving behavior. Consequently, we believe that interventions to treat sleep problems might have a positive impact on driving performance, along with benefits for general health and aggression (as a proxy for impulse control).

Our second hypothesis was that, compared to good sleepers, poor sleepers would have more impaired subjective driving behavior, lower general health, and higher aggression scores, and again this was confirmed (). Thus, the present results accord well with the majority of the literature on this topic.Citation31Citation37 The novel feature of the study is that these results were obtained from a sample of young Iranian males committing traffic offenses related to substance abuse and that poor sleep (n=339; 94.2% vs good sleep: n=21; 5.8%) is clearly a health problem among traffic offenders.

To gain a deeper insight into the associations between driving behavior, sleep, general health, and aggression, with the third hypothesis, we examined the extent to which poor driving behavior was independently predicted by poor sleep, poor general health, and aggression. The results here were not entirely clear-cut (). Specifically, while poor sleep and poor general health explained 29% of the variance in poor driving behavior (), the relative contribution of aggression as an additional predictor did not reach statistical significance and thus did not add to our understanding of poor driving behavior.

Fourth, given the impact of sleep quality on driving behavior, and given the relationship between sleep and general health, we hypothesized that poor sleep would predict poor driving behavior indirectly as well as directly, and this hypothesis was supported (). Thus, we add to the current literatureCitation2,Citation3,Citation12,Citation15,Citation18 in an important way, confirming the prominent role of sleep in driving behavior.

The data available in this study are insufficient to provide a deeper psychological or neurophysiological explanation as to why poor sleep should be associated with poor driving behavior. The following speculations therefore draw upon previous research. First, Riemann et alCitation52 with their hyper-arousal model proposed an interactional and dynamic relationship between (dysfunctional) cognitive–emotional and physiological processes, leading to poor sleep and distressed behavior. It therefore seems possible that the experience of problems related to poor sleep could be related to problems in concentration and impulse control, leading ultimately to poorer driving behavior.Citation15 Second, we note that dysfunctional coping strategies are associated with impaired sleep,Citation53Citation55 and therefore, the link between poor sleep and poor driving behavior may be mediated in this way. Third, another theoretical and empirical approach focuses on the importance of prefrontal activity (PFC) and its impairment during poor quality sleep. Yoo et alCitation56 performed imaging studies with adults suffering from insomnia and healthy controls. They found that under sleep restriction the amygdala displayed dramatically increased neuronal activation and responsiveness to negative emotional stimuli, which, most importantly, suggests a decrease in top-down, prefrontal control. Yoo et al speculated that “a night of sleep seems to ‘reset’ the correct brain reactivity to next-day emotional challenges by maintaining functional integrity of this amygdala circuit, and thus govern appropriate behavioral repertoires”.Citation56 Therefore, the effect of sleep quality on PFC control might account for the link we found between sleep and driving behavior.

Our last hypothesis followed Schreier et alCitation42 and Hallvig et alCitation43 in anticipating that the objective reaction time on a driving simulator as a proxy of objective driving behavior would not be related to subjective driving behavior or to psychological functioning (general health, subjective sleep, and aggression), and this expectation was supported. While the lack of association between sleep patterns and objective reaction time does not accord with the findings from numerous studies on the relationship between poor sleep and poor cognitive performance,Citation20Citation23 there is also an absence of explanations as to why this association might not be found. One possible explanation is that, while previous studies have focused on the effect of longer-term sleep deprivation on long-term cognitive performanceCitation20,Citation22 or of experimentally manipulated sleep restriction on specific cognitive tasks such as divergent thinking,Citation23 none of these studies took into account possible motivational factors in completing a cognitive task. We believe that participant motivation was critical in this study. Participants knew that their performance on the driving simulator could have an impact on the authorities’ decision to release or not to release their driving licenses in the near future. Thus, participants may have been particularly concerned to perform well. Second, being tested on a driving simulator might have had the character of an exciting and thrilling game, thus increasing participants’ arousal. Third, we know from studies on the cognitive performance of patients with Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS) that, despite their seriously impaired sleep quality, their performance on cognitive tests was not necessarily lower and could even be higherCitation57 than that of healthy controls,Citation58Citation60 but for contrary findings see, for instance, Fulda et al.Citation61,Citation62 To explain the lack of poorer cognitive performance among patients with RLS, Gamaldo et alCitation57 speculated that these patients might have undergone a process of sleep loss adaptation. That is to say, despite their relative loss of sleep, they are able to produce proficient cognitive performances when necessary. The Iranian traffic offenders in this study may have undergone a similar sleep loss adaptation, apparent when tested under challenging and perhaps motivating conditions. Fourth, it is also conceivable that further latent but unassessed psychological or physiological processes might have biased the association between subjective sleep and reaction time on the driving simulator.

Despite the novelty of the findings, the following limitations warrant against overgeneralization of the findings. First, we only assessed relatively young male participants who were prosecuted by the authorities because they were driving while under influence of psychotropic substances. Thus, the present pattern of results may not generalize to female drivers, to Iranian drivers undergoing driving tests to obtain an international driving license, or, for example, to the elderly. Second, although a brief medical and psychiatric screening was performed to exclude acute suicidality and psychotic states, we did rely entirely on self-reports to assess psychological functioning (general health and aggression). A systematic bias due to self-ratings cannot therefore be excluded. Third, we also relied upon self-ratings to assess sleep quality. Actigraphs, for example, would have allowed a more objective estimation of sleep continuity. However, we also note that self-report measures of sleep characteristics are the gold standard for assessing sleep among large samples.Citation63 Fourth, as already noted, it is also conceivable that further latent but unassessed neurophysiological and psychological dimensions might have biased two or more variables in the same or opposite direction. Fifth, the cross-sectional design of the study means that it is unable to shed light on the causal relationships between sleep, psychological functioning, driving behavior, and reaction time on the driving simulator. Finally, to estimate whether and to what extent the present pattern of results is unique for the present sample, it would be interesting to compare the present data with other samples such as both healthy male and female drivers without traffic violations.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study in Iran to assess concomitantly poor sleep, poor general health, and higher aggression scores as independent predictors of poor driving behavior among a larger sample of substance-abusing traffic offenders. Furthermore, visual and acoustic reaction times were unrelated to sleep, general health, aggression, and driving behavior, putting into question, whether and to what extent objective visual and acoustic reaction times might be suitable to estimate the participants’ driving quality. Finally, importantly, poor sleep predicted both directly and indirectly poor driving behavior.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Nick Emler (University of Surrey, Surrey UK) for proofreading the manuscript. The entire study was performed without external funding.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • MoradiSKhademiASurvey of victims of car accidents year 1387J Forensic Med (Persian)2009152128
  • AbdoliNFarniaVDelavarAMental health status, aggression, and poor driving distinguish traffic offenders from non-offenders but health status predicts driving behavior in both groupsNeuropsychiatr Dis Treat2015112063207026300646
  • AbdoliNFarniaVDelavarAPoor mental health status and aggression are associated with poor driving behavior among male traffic offendersNeuropsychiatr Dis Treat2015112071207826316753
  • HamzehBNajafiFKaramimatinBAhmadijouybariTSalariAMoradinazarMEpidemiology of traffic crash mortality in west of Iran in a 9 year periodChin J Traumatol2016192707427140212
  • MontazeriARoad-traffic-related mortality in Iran: a descriptive studyPublic Health2004118211011315037040
  • Zangooei DovomHShafahiYZangooei DovomMFatal accident distribution by age, gender and head injury, and death probability at accident scene in Mashhad, Iran, 2006-2009Int J Inj Contr Saf Promot201320212113322681408
  • ZargarMKhajiAKarbakhshMZareiMREpidemiology study of facial injuries during a 13 month of trauma registry in TehranIndian J Med Sci200458310911415051905
  • MotamediMHDadgarEEbrahimiAShiraniGHaghighatAJamal-pourMRPattern of maxillofacial fractures: a 5-year analysis of 8,818 patientsJ Trauma Acute Care Surg201477463063425250606
  • MohajeraniSHAsghariSPattern of mid-facial fractures in TehranIran Dent Traumatol201127213113421385314
  • LucidiFMalliaLLazurasLViolaniCPersonality and attitudes as predictors of risky driving among older driversAccid Anal Prev20147231832425108900
  • MalliaLLazurasLViolaniCLucidiFCrash risk and aberrant driving behaviors among bus drivers: the role of personality and attitudes towards traffic safetyAccid Anal Prev20157914515125823904
  • BeanlandVSellbomMJohnsonAKPersonality domains and traits that predict self-reported aberrant driving behaviours in a southeastern US university sampleAccid Anal Prev20147218419225075715
  • DahlenEREdwardsBDTubréTZyphurMJWarrenCRTaking a look behind the wheel: an investigation into the personality predictors of aggressive drivingAccid Anal Prev20124519
  • PourabdianSAzmoonHThe relationship between trait anxiety and driving behavior with regard to self-reported Iranian accident involving driversInt J Prev Med20134101115112124319550
  • HiltonMFStaddonZSheridanJWhitefordHAThe impact of mental health symptoms on heavy goods vehicle drivers’ performanceAccid Anal Prev200941345346119393792
  • Scott-ParkerBWatsonBKingMJHydeMKA further exploration of sensation seeking propensity, reward sensitivity, depression, anxiety, and the risky behaviour of young novice drivers in a structural equation modelAccid Analysis Prevention201350465471
  • ReasonJMansteadAStradlingSBaxterJCampbellKErrors and violations on the roads: a real distinction?Ergonomics19903310–111315133220073122
  • StephensANSullmanMJMTrait predictors of aggression and crash-related behaviors across drivers from the United Kingdom and the Irish RepublicRisk Analysis20153591730174525809573
  • PossisEBuiTGavianMDriving difficulties among military veterans: clinical needs and current intervention statusMil Med2014179663363924902130
  • CurcioGFerraraMDegennaroLLossSSleep loss, learning capacity and academic performanceSleep Med Rev200610532333716564189
  • van DongenHPAMaislinGMullingtonJMDingesDFThe cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivationSleep200326211712612683469
  • KillgoreWDEffects of sleep deprivation on cognitionProg Brain Res201018510512921075236
  • KillgoreWDSKahn-GreeneETLipizziELNewmanRAKamimoriGHBalkinTJSleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skillsSleep Med20089551752617765011
  • BrandSKirovRKalakNPoor sleep is related to lower emotional competence among adolescentsBehav Sleep Med201614660261426507446
  • KirovRBrandSKolevVYordanovaJThe sleeping brain and the neural basis of emotionsBehav Brain Sci2012350315515622617663
  • MikoteitTBrandSBeckJVisually detected NREM Stage 2 sleep spindles in kindergarten children are associated with current and future emotional and behavioural characteristicsJ Sleep Res201322212913623046065
  • CatrettCDGaultneyJFPossible insomnia predicts some risky behaviors among adolescents when controlling for depressive symptomsJ Genet Psychol2009170428730920034186
  • WongMMRobertsonGCDysonRBProspective relationship between poor sleep and substance-related problems in a national sample of adolescentsAlcohol Clin Exp Res201539235536225598438
  • LegerDWorking with poor sleepSleep20143791401140325142563
  • LégerDBayonVSocietal costs of insomniaSleep Med Rev201014637938920359916
  • AkerstedtTPhilipPCapelliAKecklundGSleep loss and accidents-work hours, life style, and sleep pathologyProg Brain Res201119016918821531252
  • LégerDBayonVOhayonMMInsomnia and accidents: cross-sectional study (EQUINOX) on sleep-related home, work and car accidents in 5293 subjects with insomnia from 10 countriesJ Sleep Res201423214315224237855
  • CzeislerCAWickwireEMBargerLKSleep-deprived motor vehicle operators are unfit to drive: a multidisciplinary expert consensus statement on drowsy drivingSleep Health201622949928923267
  • GuptaRPandi-PerumalSRAlmeneessierASBahammamASHypersomnolence and Traffic SafetySleep Med Clin201712348949928778244
  • Shekari SoleimanlooSWhiteMJGarcia-HansenVSmithSSThe effects of sleep loss on young drivers’ performance: a systematic reviewPLoS One2017128e018400228859144
  • StrohlKPBrownDBCollopNATS Ad Hoc Committee on Sleep Apnea, Sleepiness, and Driving Risk in Noncommercial DriversAn official American thoracic society clinical practice guideline: sleep apnea, sleepiness, and driving risk in noncommercial drivers. An update of a 1994 statementAm J Respir Crit Care Med2013187111259126623725615
  • American Academy of Sleep Medicine Board of DirectorsWatsonNFMorgenthalerTConfronting drowsy driving: the American academy of sleep medicine perspectiveJ Clin Sleep Med201511111335133626414989
  • GonçalvesMAmiciRLucasRNational Representatives as Study CollaboratorsSleepiness at the wheel across Europe: a survey of 19 countriesJ Sleep Res201524324225325581328
  • GarbarinoSMagnavitaNGuglielmiOInsomnia is associated with road accidents. Further evidence from a study on truck driversPLoS One20171210e018725629088276
  • BioulacSMicoulaud-FranchiJ-AArnaudMRisk of motor vehicle accidents related to sleepiness at the wheel: a systematic review and meta-analysisSleep20174010
  • ThomannJBaumannCRLandoltHPWerthEPsychomotor vigilance task demonstrates impaired vigilance in disorders with excessive daytime sleepinessJ Clin Sleep Med20141091019102425142762
  • SchreierDRBanksCMathisJDriving simulators in the clinical assessment of fitness to drive in sleepy individuals: a systematic reviewSleep Med Rev2017388610028647501
  • HallvigDAnundAForsCSleepy driving on the real road and in the simulator—A comparisonAccid Anal Prev201350445023149323
  • TabriziRMoosazadehMRazzaghiAPrevalence of sleep quality disorder among Iranian drivers: a systematic review and meta-analysisJ Inj Violence Res2018101535929362294
  • AlaviSSMohammadiMSooriHIranian version of Manchester Driving Behavior Questionnaire (MDBQ): psychometric propertiesIran J Psychiatry2016111374227252767
  • Oreyzi SamaniSHRHaghayeghSAPsychometric properties of the Manchester Driving Behavior QuestionnairePayesh201092128
  • GoldbergDPHillierVFA scaled version of the General Health QuestionnairePsychol Med197991139145424481
  • NamjooSShaghaghiASarbakshPAllahverdipourHPakpourAHPsychometric properties of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) to be applied for the Iranian elder populationAging Ment Health201721101047105127333198
  • BussAHPerryMThe aggression questionnaireJ Pers Soc Psychol19926334524591403624
  • BuysseDJReynoldsCFMonkTHBermanSRKupferDJThe Pitts-burgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and researchPsychiatry Res19892821932132748771
  • FarrahiJNakhaeeNSheibaniVGarrusiBAmirkafiAPsychometric properties of the Persian version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index addendum for PTSD (PSQI-A)Sleep Breath200913325926219023608
  • RiemannDSpiegelhalderKFeigeBThe hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidenceSleep Med Rev2010141193119481481
  • HarveyAGPre-sleep cognitive activity: a comparison of sleep-onset insomniacs and good sleepersBr J Clin Psychol200039327528611033749
  • PillaiVRothTMullinsHMDrakeCLModerators and mediators of the relationship between stress and insomnia: stressor chronicity, cognitive intrusion, and copingSleep20143771199120825061248
  • PillaiVSteenburgLACieslaJARothTDrakeCLA seven day actigraphy-based study of rumination and sleep disturbance among young adults with depressive symptomsJ Psychosom Res2014771707524913345
  • YooS-SGujarNHuPJoleszFAWalkerMPThe human emotional brain without sleep — a prefrontal amygdala disconnectCurr Biol20071720R877R87817956744
  • GamaldoCEBenbrookARAllenRPOguntimeinOEarleyCJA further evaluation of the cognitive deficits associated with restless legs syndrome (RLS)Sleep Med20089550050517869573
  • RistPMElbazADufouilCTzourioCKurthTRestless legs syndrome and cognitive function: a population-based cross-sectional studyAm J Med201512891023.e33.e39
  • LeeHBRamseyCMSpiraAPVachonJAllenRMunroCAComparison of cognitive functioning among individuals with treated restless legs syndrome (RLS), untreated RLS, and no RLSJ Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci2014261879124515680
  • Driver-DunckleyEConnorDHentzJNo evidence for cognitive dysfunction or depression in patients with mild restless legs syndromeMov Disord200924121843184719609911
  • FuldaSBeitingerMEReppermundSWinkelmannJWetterTCShort-term attention and verbal fluency is decreased in restless legs syndrome patientsMov Disord201025152641264820836134
  • FuldaSSzesnyNIsingMFurther evidence for executive dysfunction in subjects with RLS from a non-clinical sampleSleep Med201112101003100722000419
  • WolfsonARCarskadonMAAceboCEvidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescentsSleep200326221321612683482