1,830
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REVIEW

Parental Phubbing and Child Social-Emotional Adjustment: A Meta-Analysis of Studies Conducted in China

, , , ORCID Icon, &
Pages 4267-4285 | Received 18 Aug 2023, Accepted 28 Sep 2023, Published online: 19 Oct 2023

Abstract

Introduction

Parental phubbing refers to the act of parents using mobile phones in the presence of their children instead of engaging with them. With increasing smartphone use in many households, parental phubbing is a potential threat to children’s healthy development. This meta-analysis synthesized the existing evidence on the impact of parental phubbing on children’s social-emotional development to examine the effect sizes and identify the moderators.

Methods

Following the PRISMA guidelines, we conducted a systematic search across multiple electronic databases (Web of Science, EBSCO, ProQuest, Springer, and China National Knowledge Infrastructure) from 2012 to May 2023. Our search included both English and Chinese literature, encompassing published journal articles as well as thesis. To assess the risk of bias, we utilized the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies. Publication bias was evaluated using funnel plot interpretation and Egger’s regression intercept.

Results

Our comprehensive search identified 42 studies with 56,275 children and 59 effect sizes. A random-effects meta-analysis revealed that parental phubbing was positively associated with children’s internalizing problems (r = 0.270; 95% CI [0.234, 0.304]) and externalizing problems (r = 0.210; 95% CI [0.154, 0.264]), while negatively correlated with children’s self-concept (r = −0.206; 95% CI [−0.244, −0.168]) and social-emotional competence (r = −0.162; 95% CI [−0.207, −0.120]). Furthermore, the parental phubbing group moderated the association between parental phubbing and internalizing problems, when both parents engage in phubbing, there is a stronger association with children’s externalizing problems compared to when only one parent is engaging in phubbing.

Discussion

The findings of this meta-analysis provide strong evidence supporting the detrimental effects of parental phubbing on child social-emotional adjustment. Consequently, parents, researchers, and the government must collaborate to mitigate parental phubbing and promote the healthy development of children’s social-emotional abilities.

Introduction

Social-emotional adjustment plays a vital role in children’s current and future healthy development.Citation1,Citation2 However, a new phenomenon called “parental phubbing” has emerged in today’s digital age. Parental phubbing refers to the act of parents using mobile phones in the presence of their children instead of engaging with them.Citation3,Citation4 This behavior can have a detrimental effect on children’s social-emotional adjustment. When parents prioritize their digital devices over their children, it can lead to feelings of neglect, reduced emotional connection, and hindered social-emotional development in children.Citation3 Although previous empirical studies have investigated the influence of parental phubbing on child social-emotional adjustment,Citation5–7 their findings have been inconsistent due to variations in research design and measurement tools. There is an urgent need for a systematic review and meta-analysis to address this ambiguity and provide a more comprehensive understanding of the association between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. To fill this gap, this meta-analysis will integrate the findings from multiple studies to establish a more robust and reliable estimate of effect size and to verify the moderators that may influence the strength of this association. The findings will contribute to the existing literature and have practical implications for promoting social-emotional adjustment and the healthy development of children in the digital age.

Child Social-Emotional Adjustment

Social-emotional adjustment is a broad concept involving cognitive, emotional, and behavioral aspects. Empirical studies on social-emotional adjustment have mainly focused on two aspects: social-emotional competence and behavior problems.Citation8–10 Social-emotional competence refers to young children’s ability to form close and secure connections with adults and peers, explore the world, and promotes learning, experiencing, regulating, and expressing emotions in a socially and culturally acceptable manner.Citation11 Behavior problems are mainly divided into internalizing problems (eg, sad mood, depression, anxiety) and externalizing problems (eg, aggression, conduct problems).Citation12 Early social-emotional problems in children (eg, externalizing and internalizing behaviors) can cause disadvantages in later childhood and adulthood, such as inferior academic achievement, decreased peer acceptability and well-being, and mental health problems.Citation13,Citation14 On the contrary, children’s positive development of social-emotional competence enhances their current learning and adjustment and plays a vital role in later health outcomes.Citation1,Citation2,Citation15

Parental Phubbing and Child Social-Emotional Adjustment

However, parental phubbing may adversely affect children’s social-emotional adjustment, as parents commonly concentrate on phone use and neglect their children in parent-child social settings.Citation3,Citation4 A similar concept to phubbing is technoference,Citation16 but some researchers see they have two key differences.Citation17,Citation18 Firstly, “phubbing” specifically refers to the use of smartphones, whereas “technoference” encompasses any kind of technology device (eg, smartphones, tablets) and can occur in various settings. Secondly, “phubbing” refers to the intentional act of snubbing someone during face-to-face interaction by using a phone, while “technoference” focuses on the interruptions caused by technology devices. Given the prevalence of smartphones in daily life, and particularly in the field of studying parental distraction due to electronic device use, examining parental phone use during interactions with their children is the most common scenario.Citation16 Therefore, our study specifically focuses on this phenomenon, which we refer to as “parental phubbing”. In the digital era, parental phubbing is a common phenomenon in daily life. For instance, Vanden Abeele and colleagues found that nearly half of the parents had used a mobile phone at least once during their research observation period.Citation19 There are many reasons parents use mobile phones in front of their children, such as smartphones becoming an integral part of daily life, mobile phone addiction, boredom, loneliness, or negative emotional experiences.Citation3 However, a considerable number of studies have revealed that parental phubbing is detrimental to young children’s language, cognitive, social, and emotional development.Citation5,Citation20–22

Parental phubbing affects child’s social-emotional development mainly in two aspects. First, parental phubbing reduces parents’ responsiveness and sensitivity to their children and affects the quality of parenting.Citation3,Citation23 According to the displacement hypothesis,Citation23,Citation24 parents’ time on media and technology devices may displace and reduce meaningful parent-child interactions. Vanden Abeele and colleagues revealed that using a phone increased the likelihood that parents would ignore a request for attention from their child by five times, and parents also responded less quickly, weaker, with less emotion, and were less likely to put their child’s needs ahead of other activities.Citation19 Moreover, according to the multitasking theory,Citation25,Citation26 paying attentions to mobile phone information while accompanying their children and trying to complete multiple tasks simultaneously can lead to inefficiency and more mistakes. Thus, it is inferred that parental phubbing may be associated with lower parental responsiveness and sensitivity,Citation27 which subsequently hinders their care or support for their children, and thus impairs children’s development.

Second, based on the expectancy violations theory, parental phubbing affects children’s expectations of interaction with their parents.Citation28 This theory postulates that people often have some expectations about interpersonal communication and social interactions. Expectation violation will have a negative, interaction-destroying effect. In the case of parental phubbing, children expect their parents to be fully present and engaged during interactions, providing them with attention and emotional support. When parents engage in phubbing behavior during interactions with their children, it violates the child’s expectation of undivided attention and responsiveness.Citation3 This violation can lead to adverse emotional reactions, such as feelings of neglect and frustration. Elias et al found that children experienced frustration and disappointment during parental phone use, leading to different behavioral problems or withdrawal from communicating with their parents.Citation29 Even infants (under one-year-old) could respond to distractions caused by the mother’s phone during the mother-child interaction in the form of increased heart rate and negative emotions.Citation30 In summary, parental phubbing violates children’s expectations of parental attention and responsiveness, leading to adverse emotional reactions and potentially impacting their social-emotional adjustment.

Many studies have investigated the relationship between parental phubbing and children’s social-emotional development outcomes. However, it is worth noting that previous findings regarding the magnitudes and directions of the association between parental and child social-emotional adjustment are mixed. For instance, some researchers found that parental phubbing and child social competence are moderately negatively correlated,Citation31 while others have failed to detect a significant association between these two variables.Citation5 This discrepancy implies that it is vital to obtain reliable estimates of the effect sizes and examine a range of characteristics as moderators to account for such discrepancies through a meta-analysis.

Impact of Moderator Variables

The children’s gender differences have been documented in the relevant literature regarding parental phubbing and child development. A recent study examining the relationship between parental phubbing and adolescents’ mental health symptoms found that boys are at greater risk of developing deviant peer relationships and mobile phone addiction than girls when they are phubbed by their parents.Citation4 Furthermore, one study discovered that the negative correlation between parental phubbing and parent-adolescent communication is more pronounced in girls compared to boys.Citation32 This discrepancy suggests that parental phubbing may affect boys and girls differently. Hence, it is necessary to consider the role of gender when discussing the influence of parental phubbing on child development.

Children’s age might also moderate the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. As children grow older, parenting styles affect children’s behavior differently. Hoeve et al found that the relationship between general parenting and child delinquency was stronger in early adolescents than in mid and later adolescents.Citation33 The authors believed that as children grew older, their peers or other life experiences would significantly impact them more than their parents. However, Pinquart found that parenting dimensions and style with externalizing problems were more robust in older samples, possibly due to externalizing problems becoming more visible in adolescence.Citation34 Thus, parental phubbing may have different effects on children’s behavior of different ages. Therefore, child age is included in the moderator analysis.

Furthermore, some research has explored the relationship between phubbing and adolescents’ mental health by examining the phubbing behavior of fathers and mothers as a whole.Citation4,Citation6,Citation35,Citation36 However, such an approach is problematic as fathers and mothers may have different parenting roles and practices,Citation37,Citation38 which can shape children’s development differently. McDaniel and Radesky found that mother technoference in parenting was positively correlated with child externalizing and internalizing behaviors, while father technoference in parenting was not related to the child’s problem behavior.Citation39 Therefore, father phubbing and mother phubbing may affect children’s development differently. It is necessary to explore their influences respectively. This study examines the moderating effects of the parental phubbing group.

In addition, this study will also examine the moderating effects of parental phubbing measurement tools, publication status, and study design on the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. Specifically, we will investigate whether different measurement tools for parental phubbing influence the relationship. Furthermore, we will compare the findings of published studies with unpublished master’s theses to determine if the conclusions are consistent. Additionally, given that most studies on parental phubbing adopt a cross-sectional design, we will explore potential differences in conclusions between cross-sectional and longitudinal research.

The Current Study

As discussed earlier, the empirical research investigating the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment has yielded inconsistent results. To our knowledge, there are no meta-analysis studies in this area to explain these differences. The present meta-analysis aims to address two core research questions:

  1. What is the overall relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment?

  2. Are there any moderating variables that influence the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment? We will explore potential moderating variables, such as the child’s age, gender, measure of parental phubbing, parental phubbing group, and research design.

By addressing these research questions, we aim to contribute to the existing literature by providing a comprehensive analysis of the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment and identifying potential factors that may influence this relationship.

Method

The present meta-analysis was conducted by the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) statement.Citation40 In order to increase transparency and prevent unintended duplication of effort, the protocol of this meta-analysis was preregistered at the International Prospective Register for Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) with registration number CRD42022325091.

Search Strategy

A systematic literature search was performed according to the Journal Article Reporting Standards for Quantitative Research in PsychologyCitation41 and descriptions given by Braune-Krickau et alCitation27 and Garrido et alCitation42 Studies were searched in multiple electronic databases, including the Web of Science, EBSCO, ProQuest, Springer, and China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI). The search string was comprised of three elements: (a) parent (specific terms included parent*, parental*, dad*, father*, mom*, mother*), (b) child (specific terms included child*, infant*, preschool*, teen*, adolescent*), and (c) phubbing (specific terms included phubbing*, technology interference*, distraction with phones*). According to the suggestion of Frandsen et al,Citation43 outcomes terms related to child social-emotional adjustment will not be searched as searching for outcomes has been found to influence the retrieval of relevant research. The truncation character (*) was utilized to obtain different forms of the search term. Furthermore, operators such as “AND” were employed to merge the three categories of search terms, while “OR” was used to combine search terms within each category. The keywords of these three elements were searched in the title, abstract, and/or keywords of the databases to obtain the primary studies. The time limit is set as January 2012 to May 2023, as the search for phubbing by Garrido et alCitation42 was first recorded in 2012. In order to reduce the risk of missing relevant literature in the database search, the researchers manually searched the reference lists of eligible study reports. This process was iterated until no further studies could be identified.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The literature included in the meta-analysis should meet the following criteria: (a) child is less than 22 years old; (b) examine the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment; (c) use an valid and reliable measure of parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment; (d) the correlation coefficients (r) between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment, or t, F, chi-square values that can be converted into r, was clearly reported in the study; (e) The sample size was reported; (f) were cross-sectional or longitudinal studies; (g) were written in Chinese or English.

In addition, exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) studies examined parental technoference, that is, parental distraction caused by the use of computers, tablets, and other devices, not just phones; (b) the child development outcome indicator assessed was not socio-emotional adjustment; (c) included a sample size less than 30; and (d) were case reports or review articles.

It is worth mentioning that the protocol did not differentiate between technoference and phubbing. However, during the literature review process, we came across studies that argued for the existence of differences between phubbing and technoference. As a result, we decided to only include studies that examined phubbing, and studies focusing on technoference were excluded. By making this decision, we aimed to maintain consistency and specificity in our investigation of the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. This allowed us to provide a more targeted analysis within the scope of our research question.

Selection Procedure

Out of 643 reports initially identified (see ), 110 duplicates were excluded. Of the 533 reports remaining, 362 were excluded based on title and abstract. The remaining 171 reports were screened at the full-text level. Based on the predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria, 42 studies were included.

Figure 1 Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram of study search.

Figure 1 Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram of study search.

Data Extraction

According to the measurements used in the research, child social-emotional adjustment outcomes were coded according to how they were defined in each study and then divided into four groups. (1) Social-emotional competence includes emotional expressiveness, understanding of emotion, regulation of emotion and behavior, social problem-solving, and social and relationship skills.Citation15 (2) Internalizing problems, according to Goodman et alCitation44 and Labella et al,Citation8 include emotional problems and peer problems, such as sad moods, frequent worries, and somatic complaints. (3) Externalizing problems include symptoms such as peer fights, cheating, stealing, inattentiveness, and impulsiveness.Citation8,Citation44 (4) Self-concept includes self-esteem and core self-evaluation.Citation45

The following data were extracted: (1) author names and publication year. (2) correlation coefficient. (3) the number of study samples. (4) gender distribution of children (measured by “male ratio”). (5) average age of children. (6) measurement instrument. (7) parent phubbing group (father phubbing vs mother phubbing vs parental phubbing). (8) publication type (Journal article vs Thesis).

In the data-extracting process, several principles were followed: (1) The generation of effect size was based on an independent sample. The correlations between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment were analyzed separately for each kind of social-emotional adjustment. Each independent sample can contribute one effect size for a specific social-emotional adjustment. (2) If two effect sizes based on the same sample can be classified into two subgroups for the moderator analysis, the two effect sizes can be considered independent for the moderator analysis. For example, a sample separately reported the correlation coefficients between father phubbing and mother phubbing and children’s internalization problems, and these two effect sizes were considered independent when examining the moderating effect of the target parent. In other cases, to ensure the independence of the effect sizes, if more than one effect size was reported on the same parameter for the same group, the mean correlation was used to address the issue of dependence. (3) For longitudinal studies, we only kept one longitudinal correlation between the initial measurement of parental phubbing and the final measurement of child social-emotional adjustment. (4) If the participant’s characteristics (eg, boys and girls) were reported separately, we coded them separately.

Since the two authors completed our coding work separately, it was essential to determine their agreement level. We used the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) to calculate interrater reliability for continuous variables and Cohen’s Kappa (k) for categorical variables. For continuous variables, intercoder reliability was calculated for correlation coefficients (ICC = 1.000), sample (ICC = 0.999), gender (ICC = 0.990), and age (ICC = 1.000). For categorical variables, intercoder reliability was calculated for publication year (k = 1.000), publication status (k = 0.931), measurement of parental phubbing (k = 1.000), parental phubbing group (k = 1.000), and research design (k = 0.880). The findings indicated a favorable level of interrater reliability, suggesting a strong consensus between the two separate raters regarding the study characteristics. Any discrepancies were resolved by discussion.

Risk of Bias Assessment

The Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (National Institutes of Health, 2014) was used to evaluate the risk of bias of all the studies included in this analysis.Citation46 This assessment tool consists of 14 items, with each item offering five choices: yes, no, cannot determine, not reported, and not applicable. A score of 1 was assigned for “yes”, while the remaining choices did not receive any points. The quality of the literature was then categorized as good (total score > 7), fair (total score between 5 and 7), or poor (total score < 5). In general, a study of high quality is characterized by minimal risk of bias. The coding process was conducted independently by two authors, and their agreement on the total reached a high level of consistency (ICC = 0.906). Any discrepancies in the coding process were resolved through consensus discussions.

Statistical Analyses

The current meta-analysis was performed in R (version 4.2.1-win), using the functions of meta and metafor package. The random effects model in the analysis was used. The correlation coefficients (r) in the present study were employed as indicators of the effect sizes. Before conducting the meta-analysis, all correlation coefficients were converted to Fisher’s z-scores. After conducting the analysis, Fisher’s z-values were transformed back to Pearson’s correlation coefficient to make their interpretation easier.

To assess publication bias, funnel plot interpretation and Egger’s regression intercept were used. If the funnel plot displayed a symmetrical inverted funnel shape and Egger’s regression was insignificant, it was decided that publication bias should be disregarded. We applied the Q and I2 test statistics evaluating heterogeneity.Citation47 The significant Q test reflected the true heterogeneity among the included effect sizes. The I2 shows the extent of heterogeneity, ranging from 0–100% [small (25%), medium (50%), and large (75%)].Citation48 If there was statistically significant heterogeneity, moderator analysis was performed.

We used random effects models for the moderator analysis. The Q statistic was used to test for the significance of moderators. According to Huang,Citation49 each subgroup should include at least three studies before conducting the moderating analysis for categorized moderating variables.

Results

Study Characteristics

The specific information of the literature included in the meta-analysis is shown in . In total, 59 effect sizes from 42 studies were retrieved for the current meta-analysis, which included 56,275 participants. Study sample sizes ranged from 293 to 4213, with a mean participant age of 13.32 years. The publication year of the included studies ranged from 2018 to 2022. There were 38 cross-sectional studies and 4 longitudinal studies. There are 36 published journal articles and 6 unpublished master’s theses. The primary measurement method used in parental phubbing research is questionnaire surveys. Commonly employed questionnaires include the Partner Phubbing Scale (PPS), Phubbing Scale (PS), and the Generic Scale of Being Phubbed (GSBP). PPS, developed by Roberts and David,Citation50 is a single-dimensional scale with 9 items originally designed to measure partner phubbing but often adapted for parent phubbing research. PS, developed by Karadag et al,Citation51 consists of two factors, communication interference and mobile phone obsession, with 10 items assessing individuals’ self-perceived phubbing behavior. The GSBP, created by Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas,Citation52 comprises three factors: perceptual norms, feelings of neglect, and interpersonal conflict. These studies measured social-emotional adjustment in four aspects, including internalizing problems, externalizing problems, self-concept, and social competence, and there are 32, 13, 8, and 6 effect sizes, respectively. Regarding the literature quality, three articles were rated as “fair” while the remaining 39 were rated as “good”. In addition, it is worth noting that we planned to investigate the moderating effect of culture in our protocol. However, based on the established search strategy and inclusion and exclusion criteria, all the studies that met the requirements for inclusion in the meta-analysis were conducted with Chinese participants. Therefore, we were unable to explore cultural differences.

Table 1 Overview of Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

Analysis of Publication Bias

All funnel plots show that effect sizes were roughly evenly distributed on either side of the total effect (). Egger’s test showed no intercept values in the outcome categories were statistically significant. Specifically, for internalizing problems, the intercept was 2.08 (p = 0.169). For externalizing problems, the intercept was 3.64 (p = 0.249). For children’s social-emotional competence, the intercept was −0.11 (p = 0.961). Lastly, for children’s self-concept, the intercept was −3.51 (p = 0.065). Collectively, these analyses suggest that the findings of the current meta-analysis are robust and unlikely to be substantially influenced by publication bias.

Figure 2 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s internalizing problems.

Figure 2 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s internalizing problems.

Figure 3 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s externalizing problems.

Figure 3 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s externalizing problems.

Figure 4 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s social competence.

Figure 4 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s social competence.

Figure 5 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s self-concept.

Figure 5 Funnel plot of the association between parental phubbing and children’s self-concept.

Homogeneity Test

As shown in , the Q values fell between 15.16 and 251.32, the p-value were all less than 0.05, and the I-squared values were between 65.3% and 93.0%. These findings, in turn, indicate that the effect sizes were heterogeneous and that most of the observed variation was caused by the effect sizes of the real differences. Therefore, the current study adopted the random effect model to test the overall effect and moderating effect.

Table 2 Mean Effects of Outcome Categories

Overall Relations Between Parental Phubbing and Children’s Social-Emotional Adjustment

The overall relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment was analyzed using a random-effects model. The effect sizes of the relation between parental phubbing and children’s social-emotional adjustment were summarized in . The results showed a significant positive correlation between parental phubbing and internalizing problems (r = 0.270, 95% CI = [0.234, 0.304]), as shown in . The results showed a significant positive correlation between parental phubbing and externalizing problems (r = 0.210, 95% CI = [0.154, 0.264]), which are summarized in . The results showed a significant negative correlation between parental phubbing and social-emotional competence (r = –0.162, 95% CI = [–0.207, –0.120]), and the results are summarized in . The results showed a significant negative correlation between parental phubbing and self-concept (r = –0.206, 95% CI = [–0.244, –0.168]), as shown in . When |r| ≤ 0.1, this represents a low correlation; when 0.1 < |r| < 0.4, this denotes a medium correlation; and when |r| ≥ 0.4, the correlation is high (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Therefore, the mean r effect sizes of the correlation between parental phubbing and children’s social-emotional adjustment were considered medium.

Figure 6 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Internalizing problems.

Figure 6 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Internalizing problems.

Figure 7 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Externalizing problems.

Figure 7 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Externalizing problems.

Figure 8 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Social-emotional competence.

Figure 8 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Social-emotional competence.

Figure 9 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Self-concept.

Figure 9 Forest plot for correlation between Parental phubbing and Self-concept.

Analysis of Moderator Variables

Moderating analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between parental phubbing and child internalizing problems. For the continuous moderating variable, the results of the meta-regression analysis showed that the moderating effects of gender (b = –0.07, SE = 0.43, 95% CI = [–0.91, 0.77], Z = –0.16, p = 0.870) and age (b = 0.008, SE = 0.01, 95% CI = [–0.008, 0.023], Z = 0.935, p = 0.350) were not significant. The specific analysis results for the categorized moderating variables can be found in . The moderating effects of the measurement of parental phubbing, publish status and research design were not significant. However, the moderating effect of the parental phubbing group was found to be significant. In particular, the effect size of phubbing on children’s internalizing problems was found to be greater when both parents engage in phubbing compared to when only one parent engages in phubbing.

Table 3 Categorical Moderator Analysis of Parental Phubbing and Internalizing Problems

Moderating analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between parental phubbing and child externalizing problems. For the continuous moderating variable, the moderating effects of gender (b = –0.79, SE = 0.64, 95% CI = [–2.04, 0.45], Z = –1.25, p = 0.21), age (b = –0.012, SE = 0.013, 95% CI = [–0.037, 0.013], Z = –0.92, p = 0.36) were not significant. Due to the limitation of the number of effect sizes, we were unable to explore whether publication status, research design, and parental phubbing measurement play a moderating role in the relationship between parental phubbing and children’s externalizing problems. For the same reason, the moderating analysis of parental phubbing and child social-emotional competence and self-concept was not done.

Discussion

Although mounting empirical studies were performed to investigate the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment, no meta-analysis has been conducted to synthesize their findings. Uncertainty also remained about how parental phubbing was associated with child development. To address these issues, the present meta-analysis was conducted to estimate the overall relationship and examine moderator effects on it.

Overall Association Between Parental Phubbing and Child Social-Emotional Adjustment

The present meta-analysis contributes to the literature by providing further evidence on the relationship between parental phubbing and children’s social-emotional development. Specifically, it demonstrated that parental phubbing is associated with increased child internalizing and externalizing problems while negatively correlates with self-concept and social-emotional competence.

The positive association between parental phubbing and child internalizing and externalizing problems aligns with previous research,Citation4,Citation31,Citation36,Citation69 emphasizing the detrimental impact of parental phone use on children’s mental health and behavior. When parents are focused on their phones, they may inadvertently ignore their children’s emotional needs, leading to increased internalizing symptoms such as anxiety and depression. In addition, limited attention and responsiveness due to parental addiction to smartphones may lead to externalizing problems in children, including aggression and hyperactivity. These findings emphasize the importance of parental presence and involvement in promoting positive mental health and behavior in children.

Furthermore, the negative correlation between parental phubbing and self-concept emphasizes the impact of parental phone use on children’s perception of themselves. From the perspective of expectancy violation theory,Citation28 children often expect their parents to pay attention to them and meet their needs in parent-child interactions. However, parental phubbing violates these expectations, interrupting parent-child communication and resulting in reduced interaction and poor responsiveness to children’s cues.Citation39 This impact of expectancy violation conveys a message of diminished importance and value of children to their parents, leading to lower self-esteem and less positive self-concept. Therefore, parents must create a secure and supportive environment that promotes children’s healthy self-worth and identity development. Further research should also explore potential interventions and strategies to help parents balance digital devices and parenting responsibilities, promoting healthy child development and well-being.

The negative association between parental phubbing and social-emotional competence suggests that children exposed to high parental phone use may face challenges in developing crucial social and emotional skills. Parental phubbing disrupts the quality of parent-child interactions, hindering the child’s ability to communicate effectively, regulate emotions, and form secure attachments. These findings align with previous research highlighting the role of parental responsiveness and attunement in fostering children’s social-emotional competence.Citation5,Citation16,Citation36,Citation90 Parents must be present and actively engage with their children to promote the development of these essential skills.

In conclusion, the findings of this meta-analysis provide substantial evidence for the detrimental impact of parental phubbing on various aspects of children’s social-emotional development. The positive associations with internalizing and externalizing problems and the negative correlations with self-concept and social-emotional competence underscore the importance of minimizing parental phone use and prioritizing meaningful interactions with children. Future research should explore potential interventions and strategies to help parents strike a balance between their digital devices and their parenting responsibilities, ultimately fostering healthy child development and well-being.

Moderator Analysis

One notable finding was that the parent phubbing group moderated the association between parental phubbing and child internalizing problems. Specifically, when both parents engage in phubbing, there is a stronger association with children’s externalizing problems compared to when only one parent is engaging in phubbing. Some recent empirical studies have explored the role of father and mother phubbing, respectively.Citation32,Citation91 According to family system theory, the father and mother can cooperate in parenting and play a unique role in the development of children.Citation92 Therefore, in future research, it is still a valuable topic to study the relationship between father and mother phubbing and child development. Furthermore, this finding suggests that in addition to examining the unique role of father and mother phubbing, we should also pay attention to the possible superimposed effects of father and mother phubbing in the family. Children who are phubbed by both father and mother are likely to experience more internalization problems.

No significant moderator effect for child gender and age was found in the present meta-analysis. Regarding the absence of statistically significant gender differences in the moderating effect of parental phubbing on child social-emotional adjustment, it is possible that the existing studies did not capture the full range of gender-specific effects. While some studies have reported gender differences in the impact of parental phubbing on specific outcomes such as deviant peer relationships and mobile phone addiction,Citation4 the overall effect on social-emotional adjustment may not vary significantly between boys and girls. It is also essential to consider that the influence of parental phubbing on child development may be influenced by various factors beyond gender, such as individual differences, family dynamics, and cultural context. Similarly, the non-significant moderating effect of child age on the relationship between parental phubbing and social-emotional adjustment suggests that the impact of parental phubbing may be consistent across different age groups. While previous research has indicated that parenting styles may have varying effects on children’s behavior as they grow older,Citation33 the specific influence of parental phubbing on social-emotional adjustment may not differ significantly based on age. Overall, the non-significant moderating effects of gender and age in the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment suggest that parental phubbing may have a consistent impact regardless of gender or age. However, further research incorporating larger and more diverse samples across a wider age range is needed to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the potential moderating effects of gender and age in this relationship.

The findings of this study indicate that the moderating effects of parental phubbing measurement tools were not significant between parental phubbing and internalizing problems. It is highly likely that the measurement tools used in our study capture related aspects of parental phubbing. Firstly, considering the timeline of scale development, the PS scale was developed first, followed by the PPS, and the GSBP scale was developed last. It is common for later-developed scales to reference and include items from earlier scales, indicating a conceptual overlap. Secondly, upon examining the items themselves, we can observe that they all measure aspects such as parental distraction and neglect of children due to phone use. These behaviors are key components of parental phubbing. This implies that regardless of the specific measurement tool used, parental phubbing consistently impacts children’s internalizing problems.

Furthermore, the findings of this study indicate that research design and publication status were not significant between parental phubbing and internalizing problems. The non-significant moderating effects of publication status and study design indicate that the conclusions drawn from both published studies and unpublished master’s theses and from cross-sectional and longitudinal research designs are consistent. Overall, these non-significant moderating effects highlight the robustness of the relationship between parental phubbing and child internalizing problems. Regardless of the specific measurement tool used, publication status, or study design, parental phubbing consistently emerges as a significant factor influencing children’s internalizing problems. However, it is essential to acknowledge that further research is needed to explore potential moderators that were not examined in this study, such as individual differences, family socioeconomic status, and cultural context, which may play a role in shaping the impact of parental phubbing on child development.

Limitations and Future Directions

Clearly, some limitations of the present meta-analysis should be considered. Firstly, the participants included in this meta-analysis were all Chinese, and the conclusions should be generalized to other cultural backgrounds with caution. China features a collectivist culture that emphasizes family ties and fitting in with individuals belonging to a larger group.Citation93,Citation94 In contrast, individualism culture emphasizes individual freedom and independence. In collectivist cultures, where interpersonal relationships are more important,Citation95 parental phubbing may have a greater negative effect on children. Therefore, whether parental phubbing affects children differently in different cultural contexts remains to be explored in the future.

Secondly, as the meta-analysis mainly included cross-sectional studies, a limitation of this study was that the conclusion relating to the causal directions of the association between parental and child social-emotional adjustment was hindered. This causal association may be bidirectional. A six-month longitudinal study found that higher technology use during parent-child interactions predicted a significant increase in child externalizing behavioral difficulties over time.Citation96 Conversely, they also found that child behavioral difficulties were associated with later higher technology use during parent-child activities. Accordingly, it would be necessary for future experimental and longitudinal studies to establish the directions of causality for the relationship between parental phubbing and child development.

Another limitation of our study was the small number of effect sizes available for specific subgroups within the categorical moderators. For instance, when analyzing the relationship between parental phubbing and child externalizing problems, we found only one effect size for the subgroup of mother phubbing in the parental phubbing group measure, one effect size for the GSBP subgroup in the measure of parental phubbing, and one effect size for the longitudinal subgroup in the research design. As a result, we could not explore the potential moderating effects of publication status, research design, and parental phubbing group on the relationship between parental phubbing and externalizing problems, social-emotional competence, and self-concept. To address this limitation, future studies should adopt more longitudinal study designs to further investigate the association between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment and explore the unique and combined roles of father and mother phubbing.

Implications

Nevertheless, this meta-analysis provides essential insights into parenting practices in the digital age. The findings highlight the significant and enduring negative impact of parental phubbing on children’s emotional and behavioral problems and their self-concept and social skills. Given these findings, educational recommendations can be considered from parents’ perspectives, future research, and policy-making.

Firstly, for parents, it is crucial to clearly understand the widespread and long-lasting negative consequences of phubbing on child development. Parents should take steps to regulate their own smartphone usage in the presence of their children. Based on the American Academy of Pediatrics,Citation97 parents can be encouraged to establish boundaries for smartphone use and implement screen-free time or areas. During these designated periods or specific areas, phones should be set aside to minimize distractions and enhance parent-child relationship. The core characteristic of the parent-child relationship is parent-child attachment, which refers to an affectionate and mutually satisfying bond between a child and their caregiver. This attachment serves the purpose of making the child feel safe, secure, and protected.Citation98

Secondly, this meta-analysis also provides implications for future research. On the one hand, research can delve deeper into the underlying mechanisms and moderating factors in the relationship between parental phubbing and child outcomes. On the other hand, future studies should explore potential interventions and strategies to assist parents in reducing phubbing and increasing their awareness of its impact on children.

Finally, the findings of this meta-analysis can inform policy-making related to digital device use and parenting practices. Guidelines and recommendations, such as those provided by the American Academy of Pediatrics, can be integrated into policies and initiatives to promote healthy digital habits and strengthen parent-child relationships. Policy interventions can include public awareness campaigns, educational resources, and support programs for parents.

In conclusion, the implications of this meta-analysis call for concerted efforts from parents, researchers, and policymakers to address the negative impact of parental phubbing on children’s well-being. Promoting awareness, providing practical strategies, and integrating guidelines into policies can create a healthier digital environment for children and support positive parent-child interactions.

Conclusion

This study employed meta-analysis to quantitatively synthesize the relationship between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. The findings revealed that parental phubbing positively affects child internalizing and externalizing problems while negatively correlates with self-concept and social-emotional competence. This study served the purpose of a better understanding of the association between parental phubbing and child social-emotional adjustment. Furthermore, our findings might pave the way for further empirical studies of varying designs investigating parental phubbing on child development.

Data Sharing Statement

The datasets used or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

Acknowledgment

The abstract of this paper was presented at the Early Childhood Voices Conference 2022 (ECV2022) name “Parental phubbing and child-emotional adjustment: A meta-analysis” as a conference talk with interim findings. We only submitted the abstract but not the full text. (The link to the conference page: https://earlychildhoodresearch.csu.domains/early-childhood-voices-conference-2022/ecv2022-252/).

Additional information

Funding

This study was supported by STI 2030—Major Projects (No. 2022ZD0209000). The funding agents had no role in the study design, the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, or in the writing of the manuscript.

References

  • Domitrovich CE, Durlak JA, Staley KC, Weissberg RP. Social-Emotional Competence: an Essential Factor for Promoting Positive Adjustment and Reducing Risk in School Children. Child Dev. 2017;88(2):408–416. doi:10.1111/cdev.12739
  • Jones DE, Greenberg M, Crowley M. Early Social-Emotional Functioning and Public Health: the Relationship Between Kindergarten Social Competence and Future Wellness. Am J Public Health. 2015;105(11):2283–2290. doi:10.2105/ajph.2015.302630
  • McDaniel BT. Parent distraction with phones, reasons for use, and impacts on parenting and child outcomes: a review of the emerging research. Human Behav Em Tech. 2019;1(2):72–80. doi:10.1002/hbe2.139
  • Xie X, Chen W, Zhu X, He D. Parents’ phubbing increases Adolescents’ Mobile phone addiction: roles of parent-child attachment, deviant peers, and gender. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2019;105104426. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104426
  • Carson V, Kuzik N. The association between parent-child technology interference and cognitive and social-emotional development in preschool-aged children. Child Care Health Dev. 2021;47(4):477–483. doi:10.1111/cch.12859
  • Hong W, Liu R-D, Ding Y, Oei TP, Zhen R, Jiang SP. Phubbing and Problematic Mobile Phone Use: the Roles of the Parent-Child Relationship and Children’s Self-Esteem. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2019;22(12):779–786. doi:10.1089/cyber.2019.0179
  • Li B, Wang P, Xie X, et al. Father Phubbing and Adolescents’ Depressive Symptoms: perceived Father Acceptance as a Mediator and Resilience as a Moderator. Article. J Psychol. 2022;156(5):349–366. doi:10.1080/00223980.2022.2044745
  • Labella MH, Narayan AJ, McCormick CM, Desjardins CD, Masten AS. Risk and Adversity, Parenting Quality, and Children’s Social-Emotional Adjustment in Families Experiencing Homelessness. journal article. Child Dev. 2019;90(1):227–244. doi:10.1111/cdev.12894
  • Okorn A, Verhoeven M, Van Baar A. The Importance of Mothers’ and Fathers’ Positive Parenting for Toddlers’ and Preschoolers’ Social-Emotional Adjustment. Article. Parent Sci Pract. 2022;22(2):128–151. doi:10.1080/15295192.2021.1908090
  • Ruberry EJ, Klein MR, Thompson SF, Lengua LJ, Kiff CJ. Parenting as a moderator of the effects of cumulative risk on children’s social–emotional adjustment and academic readiness. Article. Infant Child Dev. 2018;27(3):1. doi:10.1002/icd.2071
  • Yates T, Ostrosky MM, Cheatham GA, Fettig A, Shaffer L, Santos RM Research synthesis on screening and assessing social-emotional competence. 2008. Available from: http://csefel.vanderbilt.edu/documents/rs_screening_assessment.pdf. Accessed October 11, 2023.
  • Halle TG, Darling-Churchill KE. Review of measures of social and emotional development. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;45:8–18. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.003
  • Kjeldsen A, Nilsen W, Gustavson K, Skipstein A, Melkevik O, Karevold EB. Predicting Well-Being and Internalizing Symptoms in Late Adolescence From Trajectories of Externalizing Behavior Starting in Infancy. Article. J Res Adolescence. 2016;26(4):991–1008. doi:10.1111/jora.12252
  • van Lier PAC, Vitaro F, Barker ED, Brendgen M, Tremblay RE, Peer Victimization BM. Poor Academic Achievement, and the Link Between Childhood Externalizing and Internalizing Problems. Article. Child Dev. 2012;83(5):1775–1788. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01802.x
  • Denham SA. Social-Emotional Competence as Support for School Readiness: what Is It and How Do We Assess It? Early Educ Dev. 2006;17(1):57–89. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1701_4
  • Komanchuk J, Toews AJ, Marshall S, et al. Impacts of parental technoference on parent–child relationships and child health and developmental outcomes: a scoping review. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2023;26(8):579–603. doi:10.1089/cyber.2022.0278
  • Gong YP, Chen Z, Xie JL, Xie XC. Phubbing: antecedents, consequences and functioning mechanisms. Adv Psychol Sci. 2019;27(7):1258–1267. doi:10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01258
  • Liu Q, Wu J, Zhou Z, Wang W. Parental technoference and smartphone addiction in Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of social sensitivity and loneliness. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;118105434. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105434
  • Vanden Abeele MMP, Abels M, Hendrickson AT. Are Parents Less Responsive to Young Children When They Are on Their Phones? A Systematic Naturalistic Observation Study. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2020;23(6):363–370. doi:10.1089/cyber.2019.0472
  • Bai Q, Lei L, Hsueh F-H, et al. Parent-adolescent congruence in phubbing and adolescents’ depressive symptoms: a moderated polynomial regression with response surface analyses. J Affect Disord. 2020;275:127–135. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.156
  • Corkin MT, Henderson AME, Peterson ER, Costantini SK, Sharplin HS, Morrison S. Associations between technoference, quality of parent-infant interactions, and infants’ vocabulary development. Infant Behav Dev. 2021;64101611. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101611
  • Konrad C, Berger-Hanke M, Hassel G, Barr R. Does texting interrupt imitation learning in 19-month-old infants? Infant Behav Dev. 2021;62:101513. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101513
  • Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Online communication and adolescent well-being: testing the stimulation versus the displacement hypothesis. J Computer Mediated Commun. 2007;12(4):1169–1182. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00368.x
  • Kraut R, Patterson M, Lundmark V, Kiesler S, Mukopadhyay T, Scherlis W. Internet paradox. A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? Am Psychol. 1998;53(9):1017–1031. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.53.9.1017
  • Chen Q, Yan Z. Does multitasking with mobile phones affect learning? A review. Comput Human Behav. 2016;54:34–42. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.047
  • Wood E, Zivcakova L, Gentile P, Archer K, De Pasquale D, Nosko A. Examining the Impact of Off-Task Multi-Tasking with Technology on Real-Time Classroom Learning. Comput Educ. 2012;58(1):365–374. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2011.08.029
  • Braune-Krickau K, Schneebeli L, Pehlke-Milde J, Gemperle M, Koch R, von Wyl A. Smartphones in the nursery: parental smartphone use and parental sensitivity and responsiveness within parent-child interaction in early childhood (0-5 years): a scoping review. Infant Ment Health J. 2021;42(2):161–175. doi:10.1002/imhj.21908
  • Burgoon JK. Interpersonal expectations, expectancy violations, and emotional communication. J Lang Soc Psychol. 1993;12(1–2):30–48. doi:10.1177/0261927X93121003
  • Elias N, Lemish D, Dalyot S, Floegel D. “Where are you?” An observational exploration of parental technoference in public places in the US and Israel. J Children Media. 2021;15(3):376–388. doi:10.1080/17482798.2020.1815228
  • Rozenblatt-Perkal Y, Davidovitch M, Gueron-Sela N. Infants’ physiological and behavioral reactivity to maternal mobile phone use-An experimental study. Comput Human Behav. 2022;127:107038. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2021.107038
  • Merkaš M, Perić K, Žulec A. Parent distraction with technology and child social competence during the covid-19 pandemic: the role of parental emotional stability. J Fam Commun. 2021;21(3):186–204. doi:10.1080/15267431.2021.1931228
  • Wang P, Mao N, Liu C, et al. Gender differences in the relationships between parental phubbing and adolescents’ depressive symptoms: the mediating role of parent-adolescent communication. J Affect Disord. 2022;302:194–203. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2021.12.073
  • Hoeve M, Dubas JS, Eichelsheim VI, van der Laan PH, Smeenk W, Gerris JRM. The Relationship Between Parenting and Delinquency: a Meta-analysis. Article. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(6):749–775. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9310-8
  • Pinquart M. Associations of Parenting Dimensions and Styles with Externalizing Problems of Children and Adolescents: an Updated Meta-Analysis. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(5):873–932. doi:10.1037/dev0000295
  • Niu G, Yao L, Wu L, Tian Y, Xu L, Sun X. Parental phubbing and adolescent problematic mobile phone use: the role of parent-child relationship and self-control. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;116105247. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105247
  • Stockdale LA, Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM. Parent and Child Technoference and socioemotional behavioral outcomes: a nationally representative study of 10-to 20-year-Old adolescents. Comput Human Behav. 2018;88:219–226. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.034
  • Collins WA, Russell G. Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle childhood and adolescence: a developmental analysis. Dev Rev. 1991;11(2):99–136. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(91)90004-8
  • McBride BA, Mills G. A comparison of mother and father involvement with their preschool age children. Early Child Res Q. 1993;8(4):457–477. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(05)80080-8
  • McDaniel BT, Radesky JS. Technoference: parent Distraction With Technology and Associations With Child Behavior Problems. Child Dev. 2018;89(1):100–109. doi:10.1111/cdev.12822
  • Stewart LA, Clarke M, Rovers M, et al. Preferred Reporting Items for a Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Individual Participant Data The PRISMA-IPD Statement. J Am Med Assoc. 2015;313(16):1657–1665. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.3656
  • Appelbaum M, Cooper H, Kline RB, Mayo-Wilson E, Nezu AM, Rao SM. Journal Article Reporting Standards for Quantitative Research in Psychology: the APA Publications and Communications Board Task Force Report. Am Psychologist. 2018;73(1):3–25. doi:10.1037/amp0000191
  • Garrido EC, Issa T, Esteban PG, Delgado SC. A descriptive literature review of phubbing behaviors. Heliyon. 2021;7(5). doi:10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e07037
  • Frandsen TF, Bruun Nielsen MF, Lindhardt CL, Eriksen MB. Using the full PICO model as a search tool for systematic reviews resulted in lower recall for some PICO elements. J Clin Epidemiol. 2020;127:69–75. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2020.07.005
  • Goodman A, Lamping DL, Ploubidis GB. When to Use Broader Internalising and Externalising Subscales Instead of the Hypothesised Five Subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): data from British Parents, Teachers and Children. Article. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(8):1179–1191. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x
  • Marsh HW, Craven RG. Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: beyond seductive pleasure and unidimensional perspectives. Perspectives Psychological Sci. 2006;1(2):133–163. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00010.x
  • NIo H. Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. Available from: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools. Accessed September 9, 2023.
  • Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. In: Applied Social Research Methods Series. Vol. 49. Sage Publications, Inc; 2001: 254.
  • Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
  • Huang C. Social network site use and Big Five personality traits: a meta-analysis. Article. Comput Human Behav. 2019;97:280–290. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.009
  • Roberts JA, David ME. My life has become a major distraction from my cell phone: partner phubbing and relationship satisfaction among romantic partners. Comput Human Behav. 2016;54:134–141. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.058
  • Karadağ E, Şb T, Erzen E, et al. Determinants of phubbing, which is the sum of many virtual addictions: a structural equation model. J Behav Addictions. 2015;4(2):60–74. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.005
  • Chotpitayasunondh V, Douglas KM. The effects of ‘phubbing’ on social interaction. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2018;48(6):304–316. doi:10.1111/jasp.12506
  • Ding Q, Luo XY, Huang L, Zhang YX. Interparental Conflict Affects Adolescent Mobile Phone Addiction: based on the Spillover Hypothesis and Emotional Security Theory.Chinese. J Clin Psychol. 2022;30(03):653–657. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2022.03.031
  • Ding Q, Dong SW, Chen BR, Fang JS. Snubbing Hurts: the Influence of Parental Phubbing on Adolescents’Self-aggression. Chine J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(02):417–421. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2023.02.032
  • Dong WH, Li S, Wang XC. Parental phubbing and Chinese adolescents’ SNSs addiction: loneliness as a mediator and self-esteem as a moderator. Curr Psychol. 2007. doi:10.1007/s12144-022-04066-y
  • Gao L, Meng WH, Liu JD, Yang JP, Wang XC. Parental Phubbing and Adolescent's Cyberbullying Perpetration:The Roles of Self-esteem and Basic Empathy. Psychol Dev Educ. 2023;39(03):439–448. doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2023.03.15
  • Geng J, Lei L, Ouyang M, Nie J, Wang P. The influence of perceived parental phubbing on adolescents’ problematic smartphone use: a two-wave multiple mediation model. Addict Behav. 2021;121. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.106995
  • He C, Wei H, Xie XC, Lei YJ. Effect of Parents’Phubbing on Adolescents’Self-injury: a Perspective of Experiential Avoidance Model. Psychol Dev Educ. 2022;38(02):287–294. doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.02.16
  • Ji HY. The Relationship Between Parents’phubbing and Problematic Smartphone Use in Middle School Students. Master. Tianjin Normal University; 2022.
  • Li XR. The Effect of Parental Phubbing on Adolescent Cyberbullying: A Moderated Mediation Model. Master. Sichuan Normal University; 2022.
  • Liu ZJ, Chen JW. The Impact of Parental Phubbing on Cyberbullying Perpetration and Victimization Among Adolescents: chain Mediating Effects of Parent-Child Closeness and Internalizing Problems. Studies Psychol Behav. 2023;21(01):79–85.
  • liu MH, Xiao QB. The influence of parental phubbing behavior on alexithymia in adolescents: the chain mediating role of social emotional competence and shyness. Chine J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(02):345–348+299. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2023.02.017
  • Liu K, Chen W, Lei L. Linking Parental Phubbing to Adolescent Self-Depreciation: the Roles of Internal Attribution and Relationship Satisfaction. J Early Adolescence. 2021;41(8):1269–1283. 0272431621989821. doi:10.1177/0272431621989821
  • Ou QW, Zhu WW. Parental smart Phone Cold Calling Behavior Impact on smartphone addiction among college students. J Campus Life Mental Health. 2021;19(04):326–329. doi:10.19521/j.cnki.1673-1662.2021.04.005
  • Qu J, Lei L, Wang X, Xie X, Wang P. Mother Phubbing and Adolescent Cyberbullying: the Mediating Role of Perceived Mother Acceptance and the Moderating Role of Emotional Stability. J Interpers Violence. 2020;0886260520983905. doi:10.1177/0886260520983905
  • Shao L, Dong Y, Feng JX, Zhang DH. The Relationship between Parent Phubbing and Aggression among Adolescents: the Role of Ostracism and Interpersonal Sensitivity. Psychology. 2020;8(09):513–520. doi:10.16842/j.cnki.issn2095-5588.2020.09.001
  • Shen X, Xie X, Wu S. Do adolescents addict to internet games after being phubbed by parents? The roles of maladaptive cognition and self-esteem. Curr Psychol. 2022. doi:10.1007/s12144-022-03255-z
  • Wang X, Gao L, Yang J, Zhao F, Wang P. Parental Phubbing and Adolescents’ Depressive Symptoms: self-Esteem and Perceived Social Support as Moderators. J Youth Adolesc. 2020;49(2):427–437. doi:10.1007/s10964-019-01185-x
  • Wang X, Qiao Y, Li W, Lei L. Parental Phubbing and Children’s Social Withdrawal and Aggression: a Moderated Mediation Model of Parenting Behaviors and Parents’ Gender. J Interpers Violence. 2021. doi:10.1177/08862605211042807
  • Wang X, Wang W, Qiao Y, Gao L, Yang J, Wang P. Parental Phubbing and Adolescents’ Cyberbullying Perpetration: a Moderated Mediation Model of Moral Disengagement and Online Disinhibition. Article. J Interpersonal Violence. 2022;37(7/8):NP5344–NP5366. doi:10.1177/0886260520961877
  • Wang P, Zhao M, Li B, et al. Mother Phubbing and Adolescent Loneliness: a Mediation Model of Mother-Adolescent Communication and Perceived Mother Acceptance. Soc Sci Comput Rev. 2021:08944393211017263. doi:10.1177/08944393211017263
  • Wang H, Zhou L, Geng J, Lei L. Sex differences of parental phubbing on online hostility among adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Article. Aggressive Behav. 2022;48(1):94–102. doi:10.1002/ab.21994
  • Wei H, Ding H, Huang F, Zhu LP. Phubbing and Cyberbullying Perpetration Among Adolescents: the Mediation of Anxiety and the Moderation of Zhong-Yong Thinking. Article. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2022;20(5):2609–2622. doi:10.1007/s11469-021-00535-w
  • Wei H, Li YY, Liu MT, HE AM. Parental phubbing and adolescents’ suicidal ideation: from the perspective of experiential avoidance model. Chine J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(01):246–249. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2023.01.047
  • Xiao X, Zheng X. The Effect of Parental Phubbing on Depression in Chinese Junior High School Students: the Mediating Roles of Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction and Self-Esteem. Front Psychol. 2022;13:868354. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.868354
  • Xiao MM. The Influence of Parents’ Phubbing on Adolescent Mobile Phone Addiction Tendency: He Chain Mediated Effect of Family Cohesion and Negative Emotion. Master. Harbin Normal University; 2020.
  • Xie X, Xie J. Parental phubbing accelerates depression in late childhood and adolescence:A two-path model. J Adolesc. 2020;78(1):43–52. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.12.004
  • Xie X, Guo Q, Wang P. Childhood parental neglect and adolescent internet gaming disorder: from the perspective of a distal—proximal—process—outcome model. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2021;120. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105564
  • Xie XC, Tang XY, Wu SQ, Shen XY. Mechanisms of maternal and paternal phubbing on adolescents’ self-control: the attenuating effect of having a sibling. Mobile Media Commun. 2023;2023. doi:10.1177/20501579231158225
  • Yang Y, Gao WB, Tao T, et al. Cross -lagged analysis of parental phubbing behavior, anxiety, depression, and mobile phone addiction of adolescents. China J Health Psychol. 2022;1–14.
  • Yang WY. A Study on the Present Situation of the Children’s Parental Phubbing and Its Influence on Children’s Problem Behavior. Master. Shenyang Normal University; 2021.
  • Zhang L, Zhang L. Revision of the Chinese Version of Generic Scale of Phubbing. Chine J Clin Psychol. 2020;28(01):29–32+36. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.01.007
  • Zhang WY, Jin HY. Chain mediator of rejection sensitivity and friendship quality in relationship between parental phubbing and depression in middle school students. Occup aHealth. 2022;38(22):3101–3105. doi:10.13329/j.cnki.zyyjk.2022.0642
  • Zhang Y, Ding Q, Wang Z. Why parental phubbing is at risk for adolescent mobile phone addiction: a serial mediating model. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2021;121105873. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105873
  • Zhang YX, Dong SW, Ding Q, Chen YY. Parental Phubbing and Adolescent Aggression: a Dual-pathway Model. Chine J Clin Psychol. 2022;30(01):129–133. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2022.01.026
  • Zhang RH, Zhang HR, Guo XF, Wang JL, Zhao ZX, Feng LA. Relationship between Helicopter Parenting and Chinese Elementary School Child Procrastination: a Mediated Moderation Model. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(22):14892. doi:10.3390/ijerph192214892
  • Zhou JH, Li X, Gong X. Parental Phubbing and Internet Gaming Addiction in Children: mediating Roles of Parent-Child Relationships and Depressive Symptoms. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2022;25(8):512–517. doi:10.1089/cyber.2022.0021
  • Zhou J, Li X, Gong X. Parental Phubbing and Internet Gaming Addiction in Children: mediating Roles of Parent–Child Relationships and Depressive Symptoms. Article. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2022;25(8):512–517. doi:10.1089/cyber.2022.0021
  • Zhou F. The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Adolescents’ Externalizing Problem Behavior: The Perspective of Ego-Depletion Theory. Master. Xinyang Normal University; 2020.
  • Zayia D, Parris L, McDaniel B, Braswell G, Zimmerman C. Social learning in the digital age: associations between technoference, mother-child attachment, and child social skills. J Sch Psychol. 2021;87:64–81. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2021.06.002
  • Meeus A, Coenen L, Eggermont S, Beullens K. Family technoference: exploring parent mobile device distraction from children’s perspectives. Mobile Media Commun. 2021;9(3):584–604. doi:10.1177/2050157921991602
  • Steinglass P. A systems view of family interaction and psychopathology. In: Jacob T, editor. Family Interaction and Psychopathology: Theories, Methods, and Findings. Plenum Press; 1987:25–65.
  • Grossmann I, Na J. Research in culture and psychology: past lessons and future challenges. WIREs Cognitive Sci. 2014;5(1):1–14. doi:10.1002/wcs.1267
  • Santos HC, Varnum MEW, Grossmann I. Global increases in individualism. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(9):1228–1239. doi:10.1177/0956797617700622
  • Oyserman D, Coon HM, Kemmelmeier M. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Article. Psychol Bull. 2002;128(1):3–72. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
  • McDaniel BT, Radesky JS. Technoference: longitudinal associations between parent technology use, parenting stress, and child behavior problems. Pediatr Res. 2018;84(2):210–218. doi:10.1038/s41390-018-0052-6
  • AAP Council on Communications and Media. Virtual Violence. Pediatrics. 2016;138(1):e20161298. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-2399
  • Ali E, Letourneau N, Benzies K. Parent-Child Attachment: a Principle-Based Concept Analysis. SAGE Open Nursing. 2021;7:23779608211009000. doi:10.1177/23779608211009000