Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 31, 2014 - Issue 4
4,013
Views
86
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Does one hour of bright or short-wavelength filtered tablet screenlight have a meaningful effect on adolescents’ pre-bedtime alertness, sleep, and daytime functioning?

, , , , , & show all
Pages 496-505 | Received 29 Jul 2013, Accepted 02 Dec 2013, Published online: 07 Jan 2014
 

Abstract

Electronic media use is prevalent among adolescent populations, as is the frequency of sleeplessness. One mechanism proposed for technology affecting adolescents’ sleep is the alerting effects from bright screens. Two explanations are provided. First, screens emit significant amounts of short-wavelength light (i.e. blue), which produces acute alertness and alters sleep timing. Second, later chronotypes are hypothesised to be hypersensitive to evening light. This study analysed the pre-sleep alertness (GO/NOGO task speed, accuracy; subjective sleepiness), sleep (sleep diary, polysomnography), and morning functioning of 16 healthy adolescents (M = 17.4 ± 1.9 yrs, 56% f) who used a bright tablet screen (80 lux), dim screen (1 lux) and a filtered short-wavelength screen (f.lux; 50 lux) for 1 hr before their usual bedtime in a within-subjects protocol. Chronotype was analysed as a continuous between-subjects factor; however, no significant interactions occurred. Significant effects occurred between bright and dim screens for GO/NOGO speed and accuracy. However, the magnitude of these differences was small (e.g. GO/NOGO speed = 23 ms, accuracy = 13%), suggesting minimal clinical significance. No significant effects were found for sleep onset latency, slow-rolling eye movements, or the number of SWS and REM minutes in the first two sleep cycles. Future independent studies are needed to test short (1 hr) vs longer (>2 hrs) screen usage to provide evidence for safe-to-harmful levels of screenlight exposure before adolescents’ usual bedtime.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the adolescents who participated in this study, Ms. Chelsea Reynolds, Ms. Deanne Green, Ms. Elizabeth Dos Santos, and Mr Ian Smith for their assistance with data collection, Dr. Christian Cajochen, Mr. Oliver Stefani, and Mr. Paul Douglas for assistance developing the computerised GO/NOGO task, and Michael and Lorna Herf from Stereopsis for their assistance with testing f.lux. Funding was received by the School of Psychology at Flinders University.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.