650
Views
28
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Papers

Estimating the prevalence of negative attitudes towards people with disability: a comparison of direct questioning, projective questioning and randomised response

&
Pages 399-411 | Accepted 01 May 2010, Published online: 30 Jul 2011
 

Abstract

Purpose. Despite being susceptible to social desirability bias, attitudes towards people with disabilities are traditionally assessed via self-report. We investigated two methods presumably providing more valid prevalence estimates of sensitive attitudes than direct questioning (DQ). Most people projective questioning (MPPQ) attempts to reduce bias by asking interviewees to estimate the number of other people holding a sensitive attribute, rather than confirming or denying the attribute for themselves. The randomised-response technique (RRT) tries to reduce bias by assuring confidentiality through a random scrambling of the respondent's answers.

Method. We assessed negative attitudes towards people with physical and mental disability via MPPQ, RRT and DQ to compare the resulting estimates.

Results. The MPPQ estimates exceeded the DQ estimates. Employing a cheating-detection extension of the RRT, we determined the proportion of respondents disregarding the RRT instructions and computed an upper bound for the prevalence of negative attitudes. MPPQ estimates exceeded this upper bound and were thus shown to overestimate the prevalence. Furthermore, we found more negative attitudes towards people with mental disabilities than those with physical disabilities in all three questioning conditions.

Conclusions. We recommend employing the cheating-detection variant of the RRT to gain additional insight in future studies on attitudes towards people with disabilities.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dennis Winter, Martin Grupe and Sven Keiner for their help in collecting the data, and to Ute J. Bayen and Morten Moshagen for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (MU 2674/1–1).

Note

1. For reasons of consistency, we follow Clark and Desharnais [Citation44] by referring to these respondents as ‘cheaters’. It is important to note, however, that in more neutral terms they might just as well be called ‘noncompliant’ or ‘disobedient’ participants.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.