590
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The association of metabolic syndrome and aging with cognition in Asian men

&
Pages 216-222 | Received 24 Jun 2014, Accepted 19 Sep 2014, Published online: 09 Oct 2014

Abstract

Background: The present cross-sectional study examined the associations of individual metabolic factors and age with the short-term memory and perceptual capacity in 472 healthy Asian men.

Methods: The symbol digit and digit span tests from the Swedish Performance Evaluation System were used to assess the perceptual capacity and memory cognitive domains. Linear regression with the stepwise method, and multivariate analyses of the General Linear Model with the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were carried out with the SPSS 21.0 package.

Results: High blood pressure and HDL were not significantly associated with either short-term memory or perceptual capacity. Age and glucose level were negatively associated but regular physical exercise was positively associated with perceptual capacity. On the other hand, high triglyceride level (TG) was positively associated but high waist/height ratio was negatively associated with short-term memory. When men without any component of the metabolic syndrome (MetS) were compared with men with one, two or three or more components of MetS, no significant differences in cognitive performance were noted.

Conclusion: Not all the metabolic factors were significantly associated with short-term memory or the perceptual capacity domains. Those that were did not show a sufficiently consistent pattern of association to support a role for MetS as a whole in cognitive decline with aging. It may not be meaningful to evaluate the association of MetS as a whole with cognition.

Introduction

Age is associated with a decline in cognition and as the population ages every society is faced with the challenge of addressing the increasing number of elderly with cognitive impairment and/or dementia [Citation1]. Different domains of human cognition are thought to be variably affected by behavioral, psychological, lifestyle, environmental and other factors including metabolic syndrome (MetS) risk factors [Citation2–15].

Metabolic syndrome (MetS) is not a disease but comprises a group of risk factors including high blood pressure, impaired levels of blood cholesterol, altered glucose metabolism or insulin resistance, and obesity. These risk factors together increase the likelihood of heart disease, stroke, peripheral vascular disease, and type 2 diabetes. Proposed definitions of MetS include those by the World Health Organization [Citation16], the European Group for the Study of Insulin Resistance (EGIR) [Citation17], the US National Cholesterol Education Program (ATPIII) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults [Citation18] and one introduced in 2005 by the International Diabetes Federation (IDF) [Citation19].

In Australia, based on the ATPIII definition of MetS, the overall prevalence is 22.1% with the prevalence in men higher than in women (24.4% versus 19.9%, respectively) [Citation20]. Numerous studies have shown that MetS can adversely affect cognition and increase the risk for dementia [Citation11–15]. When coupled with obesity, MetS is associated with a greater decline in cognitive performance [Citation21]. However, the association of MetS with cognitive decline is equivocal. Differences were observed when different cognitive domains were assessed and when different cognitive function tests with varying sensitivity were employed. MetS was significantly associated with memory and visuospatial but not with the executive cognitive domains [Citation22]. Gender, age, educational levels, and race have also been shown to have a bearing on cognitive performance. It has been shown that MetS was associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline in adults aged between 65 and 74 year, but in the oldest of old (those ≥80 y), the association disappeared [Citation22–25]. Other studies have shown that the association of MetS with cognition was gender dependent. Some studies showed more association of MetS with cognition in men than in women (Citation26, Citation27), while another study [Citation12] showed more association in women than in men. In another study [Citation11], no gender difference was noted. Most of the earlier studies evaluated subjects with MetS, those with at least 3 out of the 5 risk factors and compared the cognitive performance of those without MetS. However, the control group may not be a true control group in that subjects may have none or up to 2 MetS risk factors. Relatively few studies evaluated the association of the individual components of the metabolic syndrome with cognition [Citation13]. It is not valid to assume that every risk factor of the MetS is associated with cognition, in the same manner and to the same degree. In this cross-sectional study we sought to clarify the association of the individual risk factors of MetS, other metabolic factors and MetS as a whole, with cognition.

Subjects, materials and methods

Subjects

This study involved analyses of data collected from a group of 472 men. Details of recruitment of the subjects have been reported in several earlier publications [Citation28,Citation29]. Subjects were healthy men, aged between 29 y to 72 y living in the community.

Methodologies

Each subject answered a self-administered and investigator-guided questionnaire. Questions asked include their medical, dietary, social, sex, and family histories and other histories regarding consumption of hormones, supplements and medication and the types and frequency of beverages consumed.

Biochemical measurements

An overnight 12 h fasting blood sample was collected, and serum levels of cholesterol (TC) and triglycerides (TG) were measured using an automated procedure. High density lipoprotein-cholesterol (HDL) was determined after precipitation of apolipoprotein B-containing lipoproteins with sodium phosphotungstate and magnesium chloride [Citation30]. Low density lipoprotein cholesterol (LDL) was computed according to the following formula: LDL = TC − (HDL + [TG × 0.45]). The ratio of TC to HDL (TC/HDL) was used as the atherogenic index [Citation31]. Fasting glucose level was measured using the hexokinase method on an Abbott Architect automated platform (Abbott Laboratories, Abbott Park, IL).

Whole body DEXA scan

Every man had a whole body scan using the DEXA Hologic (Bedford, MA) [Citation28]. The total body fat (%BF) based on the Siri formula used by the DEXA machine was used in the present study.

Anthropometric measurements

Bodyweight (Wt) in kilogram was measured without shoes using an electronic measuring scale. Height (Ht) was taken to the nearest centimeter. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated as body-weight in kilogram divided by height in meter squared. Waist circumference (W) in centimeter was measured midway between the lower costal margin and iliac crest during the end-expiratory phase, while hip circumference (H) was measured in centimeters. In addition, the waist/hip (W/H) and waist/height (W/Ht) ratio were computed as indices of body fat.

Blood pressures

Brachial systolic (Sys) and diastolic (Dia) blood pressures were measured by trained clinical researchers using a standardized manual sphygnomanometric method after subjects had five minutes of rest in the supine position. Both blood pressures were recorded as millimeter of mercury (mmHg).

Cognitive function tests

Two tests from the Swedish Performance Evaluation System (SPES), the Symbol Digit for perceptual capacity and Digit Span for short-term memory, were used in the study [Citation32].

Symbol digit

The Symbol Digit is a test of perceptual capacity which includes matching, memory and the speed of processing. In one row, a key to this coding task is given by the pairing of symbols with randomly arranged digits, 1 to 9. The task is to key in as fast as possible the digits corresponding to the symbols presented in random order in a second row. Each set consists of nine pairs of randomly arranged symbols and digits, and a total of 10 sets are presented. Performance is evaluated as the mean reaction time (msec) (RT) and the number of errors (Err) for the last 54 pairs of the test. Symbol digit tests the individual’s ability to interpret and correctly match what he sees as well as the speed of his mental perception. It also involves hand-eye coordination. The two components of this test are reaction time (RT) and the number of errors (Err) [Citation32].

Digit Span

The Digit Span is a test of short-term memory capacity. In this test, a series of digits is presented on the screen. The digits are presented one at a time with a 1-second presentation time, and the task is to reproduce the series on the keyboard. Depending on the answer, the length of the following series is either increased or decreased. The test starts with a series of three digits and it is terminated after six incorrect answers. Performance is evaluated as the maximum string of numbers (DSpan) that the subject could remember successfully. A longer DSpan indicates a better short term visual memory [Citation32].

The digit symbol and digit span are computer-based tests. All participants underwent a familiarization trial test before the actual scorings were recorded.

Exercise scores and groupings

The type, duration and frequency/week of exercise for each participant were collated from data from the self-administered and investigator-guided questionnaire. The intensity of the physical exercise was scored using the Metabolic Equivalent of Task (MET) for each exercise type. The scoring took into account the duration of each exercise episode and the frequency of the exercise per week to derive an exercise score. In accordance with the guidelines for Americans [Citation33], the MET cut-off values were as follow: light intensity (<3 MET), moderate intensity (3–6 MET) and high intensity (>6 MET). The total exercise score per week was expressed as metabolic equivalent-min (MET-min). A total exercise score for each individual was derived from data from the self-administered questionnaire. Each exercise type was given a value for MET. For example, jogging and walking were assigned a MET of 8.5 and 3.0, respectively. An individual who jogs four times a week and each time for 45 min will have a total score/week (ExSc) of 1530 (8.5 × 45 × 4) MET-min. A person who walks for 60 min and six times per week will have an ExSc of 1080 (3 × 60 × 6) MET-min. Those who did not exercise routinely were given an arbitrary score of zero MET-min. By this scoring formula, all exercise type can be given a score which is used to indicate how intensely an individual was exercising as a lifestyle habit. Only when the exercise regime was carried out for at least 6 months was it considered a lifestyle habit. By using this index of intensity of exercise, association studies with various exercise regimes as lifestyle habits could be evaluated.

The intensities of exercise were categorized into three groups: ExGp1 (no exercise, MET-min = 0), ExGp2 (moderate intensity, MET-min = 53 – 1230), ExGp3 (high intensity, MET-min = 1260 – 4320) The 75 percentile of all men with an exercise score >0 MET-min was 1250. Therefore ExGp3 represented those men who have had exercise intensity above the 75 percentile.

Age groups

Men were categorised into 4 age groups as follows: Age groups (AgeGp)1 (29–40 y), AgeGp2 (41–50 y), AgeGp3 (51–60 y) and AgeGp4 (61–72 y). The decade interval in age groupings was commonly used in many of our earlier studies [Citation28,Citation29,Citation34,Citation35]

Metabolic syndrome groupings

The most commonly used NCEP ATPIII definition of MetS [Citation18] was used for the purpose of this study. It is a simple scoring system with no single dominant component, and is used by several studies evaluating the association of MetS with human cognition [Citation13,Citation26,Citation36]. In the present study, we evaluated the association of MetS as a whole as well as the individual risk components with the different parameters of cognition. Povel et al. [Citation37] had suggested that using the NCEP ATPIII definition to compose one entity from MetS risk factors, MetS could predict Type 2 Diabetes and cardiovascular disease. However, it may not necessarily be applicable to cognition assessment. An earlier study in fact found that MetS as an entity did not show a significant association with cognition, while some of the individual risk components of MetS did [Citation13].

According to the recommendations of the NCEP ATP III [Citation18], the 5 risk factors of MetS were defined as follows:

  • High-density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDL) <1.03 mmol/l

  • Fasting glucose level (GLU) ≥5.6 mmol/l

  • Systolic blood pressure/diastolic blood pressure – B/P ≥ 130/ ≥ 85 mmHg

  • Triglyceride level (TG) ≥1.7 mmol/l

  • Waist circumference (W) ≥92.5 cm (W of ≥92.5 cm was the index for central obesity found to be appropriate for the local population) [Citation38].

Each risk factor of the MetS and other metabolic parameters was divided into two categories, one above and the other below the recommended cut-off points. Other indices of obesity: body mass index (BMI), the percent body fat (%BF) as derived from the whole body DEXA scan using the Siri formula, waist/hip ratio (WH), and waist/height ratio (WHt) were included in the analyses. We also evaluated other lipids and lipoproteins in the analyses including total cholesterol (TC), low-density lipoprotein cholesterol (LDL) and the TC/HDL ratio. Therefore the groups used in the multivariate analyses for this study were:

The indices for obesity were established earlier for this same sample of men [Citation38]. The World Health Organization recommended a BMI of 30 kg/m2 as the cut-off value for obesity [Citation39]. However, we have shown that a BMI of 30 kg/m2 correlated very poorly with a measure of obesity based on DEXA-derived percent total body fat of 25% in the sample of Singaporean men [Citation38]. In this study, the DEXA-derived percent body fat as a measure of obesity and the more appropriate BMI of 27 kg/m2 were used in the analyses. The cut-off values for W, W/H and W/Ht of 92.5 cm, 0.94 and 0.55 appropriate for the Singaporean men [Citation38] differed from those in the Caucasian population.

No of MetS symptom groups (Msym)

Subjects were categorised into four Msym groups based on the number of metabolic risk factors they had as follows:

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for windows version 21.0. Basic descriptive statistics and the Multivariate analyses of the General Linear Model coupled with the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were used on continuous measurements. All the individual risk factors of MetS, other metabolic factors together with age, and exercise score (MET-min) were subjected to multiple linear regression analyses. To evaluate the parameters which were independently correlated to the cognitive parameters, the “stepwise” method of linear regression analyses was adopted. Significant correlation was set at a p value of <0.05.

Initial analyses showed that age, MET-min, TG, GLU and WHt were independently associated with cognitive functions. Hence, in the final analyses, all multivariate analyses were carried out with age, MET-min, TG, GLU and WHt as covariates.

Results

shows the metabolic parameters included in the linear regression analyses using the stepwise method, age, exercise score (MET-min), triglyceride level (TG), fasting glucose level (GLU) and the anthropometric parameter of WHt that were independently and significantly correlated with one or two of the three measures of cognition studied. Age was associated with longer RT in the perceptual capacity domain, while high GLU was associated with poorer perceptual capacity, with higher Err and longer RT (). Higher MET-min was associated with shorter RT (). High TG, on the other hand, was associated with better short–term memory, with longer DSpan (). WHt, an index of central obesity, was negatively associated with DSpan (). No significant correlation with the cognitive parameters was noted for other components of the metabolic syndrome: blood pressure, waist circumference, HDL and other indices of obesity BMI, W, WH, and %BF and LDL, TC and TC/HDL.

Table 1. Parameters with significant regression with the three cognitive parameters (number of errors (ERR), retention time (RT) and maximum length of string (DSpan) based on linear regression analyses using the “stepwise” method for all continuous variables including age, MET-min, %BF, systolic B/P, Diastolic B/P, BMI, waist circumference (W), waist/hip ratio (WH), waist/height ratio (WHt), total cholesterol (TC), high-density lipoprotein-C (HDL), low-density lipoprotein-C (LDL), TC/HDL ratio, fasting glucose concentration (GLU).

It was shown with the multivariate analyses that older aged men had longer RT than younger men. Men above the age of 60 y had significantly longer RT, by upto 12.9% when compared to men 40 y old or younger (). Men who had a regular physical exercise regime as a lifestyle habit (ExGp2), on the other hand, had shorter RT, by upto 6.8%, when compared to men without a regular physical exercise regime (ExGp1, ). also shows that age is significantly associated with some of the metabolic factors. Higher systolic and diastolic blood pressure, WH and WHt and fasting glucose levels were associated with age, while the number of risk factors for MetS was higher in older men ().

Table 2. The mean (+SE) of exercise score, metabolic and anthropometric parameters and 3 measures of cognitive function Err, RT and DSpan in the different age groups (MET-min, WHt, TG and GLU were covariates for analyses of Err, RT, and DSpan in different age groups).

Of the five components of MetS, only two had significant association with cognitive functions when using the multivariate analyses. High fasting glucose, >5.6 mmol/l (GLUGp2) was associated with higher Err, but not with RT in the perceptual capacity test as compared to men with GLU below 5.6 mmol/l (GLUGp1), and was not associated with short-term memory (). High TG, ≥1.7 mmol/l (TGGp2), on the other hand, had a positive association with short-term memory; DSpan being about 3.9% longer but not significantly associated with perceptual capacity (). Of all the other metabolic factors evaluated, only high total body fat, ≥25% (%BFGp2) was associated with poorer short-term memory, DSpan being shorter by 8.8% in obese men when compared with non-obese men (%BFGp1) (). No other metabolic factors were significantly associated with any of the 3 measures of the short-term memory and perceptual domains of cognition ().

Table 3. Associations of exercise groups and metabolic factors with Err, RT and DSpan using the multivariate analyses with the appropriate covariates including MET-min, age, WHt, TG and GLU.

Regular physical exercise as a lifestyle habit was significantly associated with only the RT component of the perceptual capacity. A moderate to high intensity of exercise (ExGp2 & ExHGp3) was associated with lower RT when compared to men who did not have regular exercises (ExGp1; ). It is interesting to note that in the men studied, men aged above 60 y old were exercising more intensely than all the younger men ().

Out of 472 subjects who had all the essential parameters for evaluation of the metabolic syndrome, 41.5% had no MetS symptom, 32.7% had 1 symptom, many of these men had high TG >1.7 mmol/l. About 20.3% had 2 symptoms, and 5.5% had 3 to 4 symptoms. When compared to men without any MetS risk factor (Msym0), men with 1 (Msym1), 2 (Msym2) and 3 or more symptoms (Msym3) showed no significant differences in any of the three components (Err, RT and DSpan) of cognition ().

Discussion

Aging is associated with a decline in cognitive performance [Citation40,Citation41]. However, the onset and rate of decline vary according to which cognitive domain is evaluated. In the present study, a significantly longer RT for perceptual capacity was noted only in men aged 50 years and above; and the RT appeared to be higher in men above 60 y. On the other hand, age was not associated with a decline in the short-term memory domain in men up to 72-years-old.

A regular physical exercise regime as a lifestyle habit is beneficial to the RT component of the perceptual capacity. Both moderate (ExGp2) and intense (ExGp3) physical exercises were associated with shorter RT than in men who were not exercising regularly (ExGp1). These observations imply the importance of a physically active lifestyle in warding off or attenuating the age-associated decline in cognitive performance [Citation8,Citation9]. We therefore advocate the promotion of a lifestyle habit of physical exercises as an important, non-invasive modality to help maintain cognitive health in aging men.

Association of metabolic syndrome and with cognition has been equivocal, with studies showing negative, positive or neutral associations [Citation10–13,Citation21,Citation23]. Gender and age may complicate the associations of MetS and cognition [Citation11,Citation12,Citation22–27]. Hence, the observations noted in the present study are applicable only to men between the ages of 29 to 72 y and specific to the perceptual and short-term memory cognitive domains. We have shown that individually, not all the risk factors of the MetS had significant association with the two cognitive domains evaluated. Those that did showed associations with different cognitive domains and in opposite directions. High fasting glucose was negatively associated with the perceptual capacity, both in terms of the number of errors as well as the retention time, an observation that is similar to those of an earlier paper [Citation13]. High TG levels, on the other hand, were positively associated with short-term memory. High WHt ratio was negatively associated with the short-term memory. Contrary to the earlier study [Citation13], no significant and independent correlations were noted for the other two components of the MetS, namely systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and HDL-C. No significant and independent correlations were noted for the other metabolic factors evaluated except for percent total body fat. Obesity in men was associated with poorer short-term memory when compared to non-obese men.

LDL and HDL are major transport lipoproteins of cholesterol and together with TG are independently associated with cardiovascular disease [Citation42] and possibly cognitive impairment [Citation43]. Some studies have shown conflicting associations of LDL, HDL and TG with cognition. Other studies have shown an association of high LDL, HDL and TG with decline in cognitive performance [Citation44–47]. Still other studies have shown no association of high LDL, HDL and TG with cognitive functions [Citation48–51]. Opposite associations were also noted for high LDL and HDL [Citation52–54]. The present study found that high levels of TG, in contrast to earlier studies [Citation47,Citation55] were associated with better short-term memory. Another recent study has shown that higher levels of LDL were associated with better general cognitive performance and processing speed [Citation56]. It is possible that the conflicting findings of the association of LDL, HDL and TG may be due, in part, to age, gender, cognition outcome measures, levels of lipoproteins and lipids and varying degrees of adjustments of other confounding factors such as adjusting for exercise intensity, and/or other metabolic factors. Further research is needed to clarify the roles of the individual metabolic factors with cognition.

We have shown that different components of the MetS may be associated with different cognitive domains and in different directions. Therefore, it is fortunate that we found dichotomizing men into MetS or Non-MetS overall did not predict scores in the cognitive parameters studied. This is especially true when the analyses were adjusted for confounding factors including, age and exercise intensity, etc. This observation was similar to that of an earlier paper [Citation13]. Since different components of the MetS have none or varying associations with cognition it is therefore not meaningful to study the relationship between MetS as a single construct and cognition. We need to clarify the role of each component of MetS in cognitive performance, in order to establish modalities to help enhance cognitive performance and/or reduce its decline with age.

A limitation of this study is that it is a cross-sectional study; hence no causal effect is attributable to the observed associations. The number of cases in some groupings was small and may have limited the validity of the findings. Only two cognitive domains, the perceptual and memory domains, were studied, hence cross comparisons with studies with other domains of cognition could not be made. On the other hand, a positive contribution of the present study is the involvement of a relatively wide age range (29 years to 72 years) of Asian men who were healthy with no known illnesses that might mar the actual association of the metabolic factors with cognition.

Declaration of interest

The authors report no declaration of interest. This study was supported, in part, by funds from the Academic Research Fund of the National University of Singapore, Singapore.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the technical assistance from staff of the Endocrine Research and Service Laboratory of the Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, National University of Singapore, Singapore. This study was designed, conducted and data collected while Prof. Victor H. H. Goh was at the Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, National University of Singapore, Singapore. Prof. William Hart was intimately involved in the interpretation of findings, drafting of the article and critical revision of the article for submission.

References

  • Small BJ, Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, et al. The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based sample. Arch Neurol 2000;57:839–44
  • Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, et al. Neuropyschological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004
  • Agrigoroaei S, Lachman ME. Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011;66:i130–40
  • Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 2009;30:507–14
  • Yaffe K, Fiocco AJ, Lindquist K, et al. Health ABC Study. Predictors of maintaining cognitive function in older adults: the Health ABC Study. Neurol 2009;72:2029–35
  • Seeman TE, Miller-Martinez DM, Stein Merkin S, et al. Histories of social engagement and adult cognition: midlife in the U.S. Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011;66:il41–52
  • Joffe H, Hall JE, Gruber S, et al. Estrogen therapy selectively enhances prefrontal cognitive processes: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study with functional magnetic resonance imaging in perimenopausal and recently postmenopausal women. Menopause 2006;13:411–22
  • Zhao E, Tranovich MJ, Wright VJ. The role of mobility as a protective factor of cognitive function in aging adults: a review. Sports Health 2014;6:63–69
  • Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J Appl Physiol 2011;111:1505–13
  • Bokura H, Nagai A, Oguro H, et al. The association of metabolic syndrome with executive dysfunction independent of subclinical ischemic brain lesions in Japanese adults. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:479–85
  • Hassenstab JJ, Sweat V, Bruehl H, Convit A. Metabolic syndrome is associated with learning and recall impairment in middle age. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;29:356–62
  • Schuur M, Henneman P, van Swieten JC, et al. Insulin-resistance and metabolic syndrome are related to executive function in women in a large family-based study. Eur J Epidemiol 2010;25:561–8
  • Tournoy J, Lee DM, Pendleton N, et al; the EMAS study group. Association of cognitive performance with the metabolic syndrome and with glycaemia in middle-age and older European men: the European Male Ageing Study. Diabetes Metab Res Rev 2010;26:668–76
  • Bourdel-Marchasson I, Lapre E, Laksir H, Puget E. Insulin resistance, diabetes and cognitive function: consequences for preventative strategies. Diabetes Metab 2010;36:173–81
  • Ryan CM, Freed MI, Rood JA, et al. Improving metabolic control leads to better working memory in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2006;29:345–51
  • World Health Organization, definition, diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus and its its complications. Part 1: Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Geneva: Department of Noncommunicable Disease Surveillance; 1999
  • Balkau B, Charles MA. Comment on the provisional report from the WHO consultation. European Group for the Study of Insulin Resistance (EGIR). Diab Med 1999;16:442–3
  • Executive Summary of The Third Report of The National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, And Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol In Adults (Adult Treatment Panel III). JAMA 2001;285:2486–97
  • Alberti KG, Zimmet PZ, Shaw J. The metabolic syndrome – a new worldwide definition. Lancet 2005;366:1059–62
  • Cameron AJ, Magliano DJ, Zimmet PZ, et al. The metabolic syndrome in Australia: prevalence using four definitions. Diab Res Clin Pract 2007;77:471–8
  • Singh-Manoux A, Czernichow S, Elbaz A, et al. Obesity phenotypes in midlife and cognition in early old age. The Whitehall II cohort Study. Neurology 2012;79:755–62
  • Muller M, Tang MX, Schupf N, et al. Metabolic syndrome and dementia risk in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007;24:185–92
  • Vanhanen M, Koivisto K, Moilanen L, et al. Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer disease: a population-based study. Neurology 2006;67:843–7
  • Yaffe K, Weston AL, Blackwell T, Kruger KA. The metabolic syndrome and development of cognitive impairment among older women. Arch Neruol 2009;66:324–8
  • van den Berg E, Biessels GJ, Craen AJ, et al. The metabolic syndrome is associated with decelerated cognitive decline in the oldest old. Neurology 2007;69:979–85
  • Cavalieri M, Ropele S, Petrovic K, et al. Metabolic syndrome, brain magnetic resonance imaging and cognition. Diabetes Care 2010;33:2489–95
  • Haring R, Volzke H, Felix SB, et al. Prediction of metabolic syndrome by low serum testosterone levels in men: results from the study of health in Pomerania. Diabetes 2009;58:2027–31
  • Goh VHH, Tong TYY, Mok HPP, Said B. Interactions among age, adiposity, bodyweight, lifestyle factors and sex steroid hormones in healthy Singaporean Chinese men. Asian J Androl 2007;9:611–21
  • Goh Victor HH, Tong Terry YY. The moderating impact of lifestyle factors on sex steroids, sexual activities and aging in Asian men. Asian J Androl 2011;13:596–604
  • Chang CJ, Wu CH, Lu FH, et al. Discriminating glucose tolerance status by regions of interest of dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry. Clinical implications of body fat distribution. Diabetes Care 1999;22:1938–43
  • Ettinger WH, Wahl PW, Kuller LH, et al. Lipoprotein lipids in old people. Results from the Cardiovascular Health Study. The CHS Collaborative Research Group. Circulation 1992;86:858–69
  • Iregren A, Gamberale F, Kjellberg A. SPES: a psychological test system to diagnose environmental hazards. Neurotoxicol Terotol 1996;18:485–91
  • US Department of Health and Human Services. Guidelines for Physical Activities for American. http://www.health.gov/PAguidelines/guidelines/appendix1.aspx [last accessed 20 Apr 2014]
  • Goh VHH, Tong TYY. The association of exercise with sleep, sex steroids, sexual activities and aging in Asian men. J Men Health 2009;6:317–24
  • Goh VHH, Tong TYY. Sleep, sex steroid hormones, sexual activities, and aging in Asian men. J Androl 2010;31:131–7
  • Katasumata Y, Todoriki H, Higashiuesata Y, et al. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline among the oldest old in Okinawa: In search of a mechanism. The KOCOA Project. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2012;67A:126–34
  • Povel CM, Beulens JW, van der Schouw YT, Dolle MET. Metabolic syndrome model definitions predicting Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Diabetes Care 2013;36:362–8
  • Goh VHH, Tain CF, Tong TYY, et al. Are BMI and other anthropometric measures appropriate as indices for obesity? A study in an Asian population. J Lipid Res 2004;45:1892–9
  • World Health Organization: Global database on body mass index: an interactive surveillance tool for monitoring nutrition transition. http:apps.who/bmi/index.jsp [last accessed 23 Jun 2014]
  • Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, et al. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging 2002;17:299–320
  • Sliwinski M, Buschke H. Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. Psychol Aging 1999;14:18–33
  • Morrison A, Hokanson JE. The independent relationship between triglycerides and coronary heart disease. Vasc Health Risk Manag 2009;5:89–95
  • Van Vliet P, van de Water W, de Craen AJ, Westendorp RGJ. The influence of age on the association between cholesterol and cognitive function. Exp Gerontol 2009;44:112–22
  • Helzner EP, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N, et al. Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer Disease. Arch Neurol 2009;66:343–8
  • Lesser GT, Haroutunian V, Purohit DP, et al. Serum lipids are related to Alzheimer’s pathology in nursing home residents. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;27:42–9
  • De Frias CM, Bunce D, Wahlin A, et al. Cholesterol and triglycerides moderate the effect of apolipoprotein E on memory functioning in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2007;62:112–18
  • Sims R, Madhere S, Callender C, Campbell A Jr. Patterns of relationships between cardiovascular disease risk factors and neurocognitive function in African Americans. Ethn Dis 2008;18:471–6
  • Reitz C, Tang MX, Luchsinger J, Mayeux R. Relation of plasma lipids to Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Arch Neurol 2004;61:705–714
  • Reitz C, Tang MX, Manly J, et al. Plasma lipid levels in the elderly are not associated with the risk of mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008;25:232–7
  • Tan ZS, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Plasma total cholesterol levels as a risk factor for Alzheimer disease: the Framingham study. Arch Intern Med 2003;163:1053–7
  • Hall K, Murrell J, Ogunniyi A, Deeg M, et al. Cholesterol, APOE genotype, and Alzheimer disease: an epidemiologic study of Nigerian Yoruba. Neurology 2006;66:223–7
  • Packard CJ, Westendorp RG, Stott DJ, et al. Prospective Study of Pravastatin in Elderly at Risk Group. Association between apolipoprotein E-4 and cognitive decline in elderly adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:1777–85
  • Atzmon G, Gabriely I, Greiner W, et al. Plasma HDL levels highly correlate with cognitive function in exceptional longevity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2002;57A:M712–15
  • Wolf H, Hensel A, Arendt T, et al. Serum lipids and hippocampal volume: the link to Alzheimer’s disease? Ann Neurol 2004;56:745–8
  • Kalmijn S, Foley D, White L, et al. Metabolic cardiovascular syndrome and risk of dementia in Japanese-American elderly men: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2000;20:2255–60
  • Van den Kommer TN, Dik MG, Comijs HC, et al. The role of lipoproteins and inflammation in cognitive decline: do they interact? Neurol Aging 2012;33:196.e1–e12

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.