1,736
Views
19
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Article Addendum

Bowerbirds, art and aesthetics

Are bowerbirds artists and do they have an aesthetic sense?

Pages 281-283 | Published online: 01 May 2012

Abstract

Male bowerbirds create and decorate a structure called a bower which serves only to attract females for mating, and females visit and choose one among many bower owners before deciding which male to mate with. Is what they do art, and do they have an aesthetic sense? I propose operational definitions of art, judgement, and an aesthetic sense which depend upon communication theory which allow one to get explicit answers to this question. By these definitions Great Bowerbirds are artists, judge art, and therefore have an aesthetic sense.

This article refers to:

Introduction

Male bowerbirds create and decorate a structure called a bower which is used only for attracting and mating with females; females then go off and make their own nests and raise their offspring by themselves.Citation1 Female bowerbirds choose which male to mate with on the basis of various bower and male characteristics (1, references in ref. Citation2Citation3). Male great bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus nuchalis) build a large (0.6 min long) avenue of densely thatched sticks opening into two slightly larger flat areas or courts which are covered with colorless objects (gesso), and decorated with colored objects. From within the avenue, females watch the male display his head and lilac-pink nuchal crest, and colored objects in his bill, over the gesso.Citation1-Citation3 Great bowerbirds go further, and arrange the gesso objects in increasing size with distance from the avenue entrances.Citation3 The combined effect of the geometric gesso pattern and the fact that the avenue creates a predictable direction and field of view of the court sets up perfect conditions for a geometric optical illusion known as forced perspective.Citation3 The forced perspective has several possible consequencesCitation3,Citation4 including creating a more regular gesso pattern as seen by the female within the avenue, altering the perceived sizes of court and displayed objects, and creating further illusions which attract the female's attention.Citation3,Citation4 We now know that males which create better forced perspective gain more mates.Citation4 Is this art, and do great bowerbirds have an aesthetic sense?

Do Bowerbirds Produce Art?

To decide whether or not what bowerbirds do is art, in principle all that one would have to do would be to compare it to the definition of art. Unfortunately, the definition of art as a human activity is problematic; no two dictionaries define it the same and controversy rages. Here are some examples. “Skill, esp. human skill as opposed to nature; (ability in) skilled execution as an object in itself; cunning; imitative or imaginative skill applied to design”and “thing in which skill may be exercised”.Citation5 “Skill acquired by experience, study, or observation,” and “the conscious use of skill and creative imagination”.Citation6 “The production or expression of what is beautiful (especially visually), appealing, or of more than ordinary significance,” and “a method of doing a thing, especially if it is difficult”.Citation7 “The process or product of deliberately arranging elements in a way to affect the senses or emotions,” and “Fine art means that a skill is being used to express the artist's creativity, or to engage the audience's aesthetic sensibilities, or to draw the audience towards consideration of the finer things.Citation8 “the use of skill and imagination in the creation of aesthetic objects, environments, or experiences that can be shared with others.”Citation9 “The conscious production or arrangement of sounds, colors, forms, movements, or other elements in a manner that affects the sense of beauty.”Citation10

Note that some definitions depend upon other problematic definitions (e.g., production of beauty). Not only is the definition of Art variable and controversial, but to some merely defining it is thought to restrict artistic creativity. For a thorough discussion of definitions and their problems see the art section of the Stanford Encyclopaedia of Philosophy website.Citation11

One way to minimize confusion is to simply create an operational definition which attempts to capture most of the ideas. This also has the advantage of allowing testable hypotheses. For this reason I suggest that visual art can be defined as the creation of an external visual pattern by one individual in order to influence the behavior of others, and an artistic skill is the ability to create art. Influencing behavior can range from attraction to and voluntary viewing of the art by others to viewers mating with the artist (modified from the online supplemental material of reference Citation3). This definition equates art with conventional signalsCitation3 and aligns it with signaling theory, including that in animals.Citation12,Citation13 Art objects are conventional signals that are not part of the artist's body but have the same function as signals originating from the body. By this definition both bowerbirds and humans produce and react to art, and the practitioners of art can be called artists.

Do Bowerbirds have an Aesthetic Sense?

DarwinCitation14 and DiamondCitation15,Citation16 first suggested that an aesthetic sense underlies sexual selection for bowerbird art, but most discussions of animal aesthetics seem to be anthropomorphic or use human-style standards (e.g., symmetry or ‘beauty') in tests,Citation17,Citation18 measure human neural correlates of “beauty”,Citation19 or test animals with human paintings.Citation20,Citation21 Although these papers are interesting, it is difficult to know how to interpret them, although putting aesthetics in a sexual selection context is a good first step.Citation17

The general concept of aesthetics and the definition of aesthetic sense is at least as controversial as the definition and concept of art, and even more controversial when applied to animals.Citation11,Citation22,Citation23 What does seem to be generally agreed upon by philosophers is that aesthetics involves judgement;Citation23 the assessor (the one with an aesthetic sense) must be able to judge and perhaps even rank the quality of different art objects or individuals. But, just what is judgement? The Darwinian approachCitation17 suggests an operational definition: judgement is the active choice among different art objects or individuals leading to change of fitness in both artist and judge. Therefore aesthetics is the exercise of judgement leading to mutual fitness changes. These definitions implicitly include cases where both artist and judge benefit as well as cases of deceptionCitation12 which lead to fitness gain in the artist but fitness loss in the judge.

Variation in judgement is widespread in humans, but also found in animals. For example, individual female guppies vary in mating preferences.Citation24 In humans, variation in judgement probably encourages variation in art, and this may well work in animals. It would be illuminating to examine the theoretical relationship between variation in judgement and variation in art and other signals to investigate whether one leads to variation in the other, and vice versa, and what processes jointly maintain variation in judgement and judged.

The operational definitions of art, judgement and aesthetics suggest that Great Bowerbirds are artists and have an aesthetic sense. Males create art because their created bower is voluntarily viewed by females, leading to changes in their behavior up to and including mating with the artist. Males have an aesthetic sense in that they have to create the bowers and forced perspective, and appear to constantly maintaining and improving the bower geometry.Citation3 Individual males vary in the quality of the art (forced perspective and associated geometric patterns) that they produce.Citation3,Citation4 There is much stronger evidence for aesthetics in females than males. Females judge the art and mate with those males which produce the best art,Citation4 and both benefit by producing offspring. Males may judge their own work but females certainly judge that of the males.

One complication for bowerbirds is that there is a possibility that females choose males producing the best perspective not because the particular perspective pattern is best for that particular male, but because a more even pattern of court objects makes his displayed objects more conspicuous or generates more attention-grabbing illusions.Citation3,Citation4 However, the quality, conspicuousness and attention-grabbing aspects of the bower are not mutually exclusive and all of these factors affect the female’s judgement. As in humans, the target of aesthetics is not always straightforward or even unimodal. Seeking single explanations for things is a vestige of our monotheistic past.

Other animals build and decorate structures, for examples sticklebacks,Citation25,Citation26 cichlids,Citation27 chubb,Citation28 sand gobies,Citation29 raptors and owls.Citation30,Citation31 However, in these cases the structure (a nest) is also used for reproduction as well as signaling quality or breeding status so it has utility. Many people insist that art objects should not have a direct function. So, for example, by that more restricted art definition the colorfully decorated stickleback nestsCitation25 are not art. However, by the operational definitions given here, these species do exhibit art, judgement and aesthetics. So far, only the Great BowerbirdCitation3,Citation4 exhibits art which has no direct function other than to modify the behavior of its viewer. Many of these ideas need further clarification. The use of operational definitions of art, judgement and aesthetics should help to test hypotheses about these phenomena in other animals.

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Laura Kelley, Palestina Guevara-Fiore, Andreas Svensson, and Christa Beckmann for helpful comments on this paper.

References

  • Frith CB, Frith DW. The Bowerbirds. Oxford: Oxford University Press 2004.
  • Endler JA, Westcott DA, Madden JR. Robson, T. (2005) Animal visual systems and the evolution of color patterns; sensory processing illuminates signal evolution. Evolution 2005; 50:1795 - 818
  • Endler JA, Endler LC, Doerr NR. Great bowerbirds create theaters with forced perspective when seen by their audience. Curr Biol 2010; 20:1679 - 84; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.033; PMID: 20832314
  • Kelley LA, Endler JA. Illusions promote mating success in great bowerbirds. Science 2012; 335:335 - 8; http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1212443; PMID: 22267812
  • Concise Oxford Dictionary, 7th edition Oxford: Oxford University Press 1982.
  • Merriam-Webster dictionary online. http://www.merriam-webster.com/netdict/art%5B2%5D
  • Macquarie Dictionary, 3rd edition 2001, Macquarie Library Pty, Limited, Macquarie University, North Ryde, NSW: Macquarie Library.
  • “Art.” Wikipedia. online: Wikipedia; [accessed May 18 2010] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Art
  • Encyclopædia Britannica Online, s. v. “the arts,” accessed May 18 2010, http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/36405/the-arts
  • “Art.” The Free Dictionary: Houghton Mifflin Company, updated 2009. [May 18 2010] http://www.thefreedictionary.com/art
  • “The definition of art.” The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Oct 23, 2007; [accessed May 18 2010] http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/art-definition
  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland MA: Sinauer Associates 2011.
  • Ruxton GD, Schaefer HM. Resolving current disagreements and ambiguities in the terminology of animal communication. J Evol Biol 2011; 24:2574 - 85; http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02386.x; PMID: 21902751
  • Darwin C. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation To Sex (2 volumes). London: John Murray 1871.
  • Diamond JM. Evolution of bowerbirds' bowers: animal origins of the aesthetic sense. Nature 1982; 297:99 - 102; http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/297099a0
  • Diamond JM. Animal art: Variation in bower decorating style among male bowerbirds Amblyornis inornatus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1986; 83:3042 - 6; http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.83.9.3042; PMID: 16593691
  • Grammer K, Fink B, Møller AP, Thornhill R. Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2003; 78:385 - 407; http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1464793102006085; PMID: 14558590
  • Anderson JR, Kuwahata H, Kuroshima H, Leighty KA, Fujita K. Are monkeys aesthetists? Rensch (1957) revisited. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2005; 31:71 - 8; http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.31.1.71; PMID: 15656728
  • Kawabata H, Zeki S. Neural correlates of beauty. J Neurophysiol 2004; 91:1699 - 705; http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00696.2003; PMID: 15010496
  • Watanabe S. Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: picture discrimination in pigeons and humans. Anim Cogn 2001; 4:147 - 51; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710100112
  • Ikkatai Y, Watanabe S. Discriminative and reinforcing properties of paintings in Java sparrows (Padda oryzivora). Anim Cogn 2011; 14:227 - 34; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0356-3; PMID: 21132449
  • Welsch W. Animal Aesthetics. Contemporary Aesthetics, volume 2, Forum: Science in Aesthetics? (Online 2004) ISSN 1932-8478: http://www.contempaesthetics.org/newvolume/pages/article.php?articleID=243
  • Snaevarr S. Talk To The Animals: A short comment on Wolfgang Welsch's “Animal Aesthetics”. Contemporary Aesthetics, volume 2, Forum: Science in Aesthetics?(online 2004), ISSN 1932-8478 http://www.contempaesthetics.org/newvolume/pages/article.php?articleID=251
  • Brooks RL, Endler JA. Female guppies agree to differ: phenotypic and genetic variation in mate-choice behavior and the consequences for sexual selection. Evolution 2001; 55:1644 - 55; PMID: 11580024
  • Östlund-Nilsson S, Holmlund M. The artistic three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2003; 53:214 - 20; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-002-0574-z
  • Rushbrook BJ, Head ML, Katsiadaki I, Barber I. Flow regime affects building behaviour and nest structure in sticklebacks. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2010; 64:1927 - 35; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1003-3
  • Schaedelin FC, Taborsky M. Female choice of a non-bodily ornament: an experimental study of cichlid sand craters in Cyathopharynx furcifer. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2010; 64:1437 - 47; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0959-3
  • Wisenden BD, Unruh A, Morantes A, Bury S, Curry B, Driscoll R, et al. Functional constraints on nest characteristics of pebble mounds of breeding male hornyhead chub Nocomis biguttatus. J Fish Biol 2009; 75:1577 - 85; http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02384.x; PMID: 20738634
  • Lehtonen TK. Wong BBM. Should females prefer males with elaborate nests?. Behav Ecol 2009; 20:1014 - 9; http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arp091
  • Sergio F, Blas J, Blanco G, Tanferna A, López L, Lemus JA, et al. Raptor nest decorations are a reliable threat against conspecifics. Science 2011; 331:327 - 30; http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1199422; PMID: 21252345
  • Penteriani V, del Mar Delgado M. Owls may use faeces and prey feathers to signal current reproduction. PLoS One 2008; 3:e3014; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003014